Elsevier

Consciousness and Cognition

Volume 7, Issue 4, December 1998, Pages 559-595
Consciousness and Cognition

Regular Article
The Timing of Conscious Experience: A Critical Review and Reinterpretation of Libet's Research,☆☆

https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1998.0332Get rights and content

Abstract

An extended examination of Libet's works led to a comprehensive reinterpretation of his results. According to this reinterpretation, the Minimum Train Duration of electrical brain stimulation should be considered as the time needed to create a brain stimulus efficient for producing conscious sensation and not as a basis for inferring the latency for conscious sensation of peripheral origin. Latency for conscious sensation with brain stimulation may occurafterthe Minimum Train Duration. Backward masking with cortical stimuli suggests a 125–300 ms minimum value for the latency for conscious sensation of threshold skin stimuli. Backward enhancement is not suitable for inferring this latency. For determining temporal relations between stimuli that correspond to subjects' reports, theendof cerebral Minimum Train Duration should be used as reference, rather than its onset. Results of coupling peripheral and cortical stimuli are explained by a latency after the cortical Minimum Train Duration, having roughly the same duration as the latency for supraliminal skin stimuli. Results of coupling peripheral stimuli and stimuli to medial lemniscus (LM) are explained by a shorter LM latency and/or a longer peripheral latency. This interpretation suggests a 230 ms minimum value for the latency for conscious sensation of somatosensory near-threshold stimuli. The backward referral hypothesis, as formulated by Libet, should not be retained. Long readiness potentials preceding spontaneous conscious or nonconscious movements suggest that both kinds of movement are nonconsciously initiated. The validity of Libet's measures of W and M moments (Libet et al., 1983a) is questionable due to problems involving latencies, training, and introspective distinction of W and M. Veto of intended actions may be initially nonconscious but dependent on conscious awareness.

References (47)

  • P.S. Churchland

    Discussion: The timing of sensations: Reply to Libet

    Philosophy of Science

    (1981)
  • A.C. Danto

    Consciousness and motor control

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1985)
  • L. Deecke et al.

    Voluntary finger movement in man: cerebral potentials and theory

    Biological Cybernetics

    (1976)
  • A. Delmas

    Voies et centres nerveux

    (1970)
  • D.C. Dennett et al.

    Time and the observer: The where and when of consciousness in the brain

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1992)
  • R. Efron

    The duration of the present

    Annals New York Academy of Sciences

    (1967)
  • E. Fehrer et al.

    A comparison of reaction time and verbal report in the detection of masked stimuli

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1962)
  • R. Fox

    Visual masking

    Handbook of Sensory Physiology, VIII (Perception)

    (1978)
  • I.M. Glynn

    Consciousness and Time

    Nature

    (1990)
  • I.M. Glynn

    Glynn replies

    Nature

    (1991)
  • G. Gomes

    Self-awareness and the mind-brain problem

    Philosophical Psychology

    (1995)
  • C.M. Gray et al.

    Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1989)
  • H.H. Kornhuber

    Attention, readiness for action, and the stages of voluntary decision—some electrophysiological correlates in man

    Experimental Brain Research

    (1984)
  • Cited by (76)

    • Libet's legacy: A primer to the neuroscience of volition

      2024, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • Time marking in perception

      2023, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • A meta-analysis of Libet-style experiments

      2021, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • Libet's experiment: A complex replication

      2018, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Libet believed that this introspection accuracy training was crucial, because a systematic bias in the S series would also apply to other tasks, such as the W series (he also suggests a correction for the W and M reports by subtracting the S time, see Libet et al., 1983, pp. 630–631). However, Gomes (1998, p. 590) points out that this feedback training performed on a regular basis throughout the experiment might lead to variable results across the sessions. Additionally, there seems to be no reason to use the training based on the skin stimulation specifically, because Danquah et al. (2008) showed that the introspective reports differ for tactile, visual and auditive modality – this implies that the reports provided after an auditory feedback training would presumably differ from those provided after a tactile feedback training.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Address reprint requests to Gilberto Gomes, 15, pass. Ste. Anne Popincourt, 75011 Paris, France. Fax (33 1) 48 07 80 97. E-mail:[email protected].

    ☆☆

    R. HeldH. W. LeibowitzH. L. Teuber

    View full text