Skip to main content
Log in

Trajectory control in targeted force impulses

III. Compensatory adjustments for initial errors

  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

In the preceding study (Gordon and Ghez 1987), we showed that accurately targeted isometric force impulses produced by human subjects are governed by a pulse height control policy. Different peak forces were achieved by modulating the rate of rise of force while force rise time was maintained close to a constant value and independent of peak force. An early measure of the rate of rise of force, peak d2F/dt2, was scaled to the required force (target amplitude) and highly predictive of the peak force achieved. In six subjects examined, peak d2F/dt2 accounted for between 70% and 96% of the total variance in peak force. In the present study, we further examined these targeted responses to determine whether the residual variability not predicted by peak d2F/dt2 could be accounted for by adjustments to the force trajectories which compensated for initial errors in the scaling of the d2F/dt2. A statistical model of the determinants of peak force was tested. This model included two paths by which the target amplitude could independently influence the peak force achieved. The first path was preprogrammed pulse height control. In this path, target amplitude determined the initial rate of rise of force (peak d2F/dt2) which in turn determined the final peak force achieved. The second path was an independent influence of errors in the initial scaling of peak d2F/dt2 on peak force. Multiple regression analysis was performed on trajectory variables within the sets of responses by each subject in each condition to determine whether the second path contributed significantly to explaining the variance in peak force. In each subject and condition, there was a significant independent influence of error in d2F/dt2 on peak force, and the direction of this effect was to decrease the magnitudes of peak force errors. These compensatory adjustments accounted for between 1% and 14% of the total variance in peak force. Further multiple regression analyses revealed that inappropriate scaling of the initial phase of the trajectories was compensated for by shortening or prolonging the force rise time. These trajectory adjustments were in turn implemented by modulation of the timing and magnitude of the contractions in the agonist and antagonist muscles that produced the force trajectories. Because these compensatory adjustments were evident in the EMG pattern at latencies too short to be accounted for by peripheral feedback, we assume that they depend on internal monitoring of the unfolding neural commands. These internal feedback processes act in parallel with the programmed commands, both determining the force trajectory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alstermark B, Lundberg A, Norrsell U, Sybirska E (1981) Integration in descending motor pathways controlling the forelimb in the cat. 9. Differential behavioral defects after spinal cord lesions interrupting defined pathways. Exp Brain Res 42: 299–318

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks VB (1974) Some examples of programmed limb movements. Brain Res 71: 299–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke JD, Diggles VA (1984) Rapid error correction during human arm movements: evidence for central monitoring. J Mot Behav 16: 348–363

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik KJW (1948) Theory of the human operator in control systems: II. Man as an element in a control system. Br J Psychol 38: 142–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmedt JE, Godaux E (1978) Ballistic skilled movements: load compensation and patterning of the motor commands. In: Desmedt JE (ed) Progress in clinical neurophysiology, Vol 4. Cerebral motor control in man: long loop mechanisms. Karger, Basel, pp 21–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Evarts EV (1971) Feedback and corollary discharge: a merging of the concepts. Neurosci Res Prog Bull 9(1): 86–112

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgopolous AP, Kalaska JP, Massey JT (1981) Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. J Neurophysiol 46: 725–743

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghez C (1979) Contribution of central programs to rapid limb movement in the cat. In: Asanuma H, Wilson V (eds) Integration in the nervous system. Igaku-Shoin, Tokyo, pp 305–319

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghez C, Gordon J (1987) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. I. Role of opposing muscles. Exp Brain Res 67: 225–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J (1985) Mechanisms contributing to accuracy in aimed force impulses of human subjects. Doctoral dissertation. Columbia University, Teachers College

  • Gordon J, Ghez C (1985) Mechanisms contributing to accuracy in aimed force impulses: rise time regulation and corrective adjustments. Soc Neurosci Abstr 11: 75

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghez C (1987) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. II. Pulse height control. Exp Brain Res 67: 241–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Hening W, Vicario D, Ghez C (1983) Choice reaction time conditions alter response dynamics in an isometric tracking task in humans. Soc Neurosci Abstr 9: 1031

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins JR, Angel RW (1970) Correction of tracking errors without sensory feedback. J Exp Psychol 84: 412–416

    Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg A (1979) Integration in a propriospinal motor centre controlling the forelimb in the cat. In: Asanuma H, Wilson V (eds) Integration in the nervous system. Igaku-Shoin, Tokyo, pp 47–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Megaw ED (1974) Possible modification to a rapid on-going programmed manual response. Brain Res 71: 425–441

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur EJ (1982) Multiple regression in behavioral research (2nd edn). Holt Rinehart Winston, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulton EC (1981) Human manual control. In: Brooks VB (ed) Handbook of physiology, Sect 1. The nervous system, Vol 2: Motor control. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, pp 1337–1389

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor FV, Birmingham HP (1948) Studies of tracking behavior. II. The acceleration pattern of quick manual corrective responses. J Exp Psychol 38: 783–795

    Google Scholar 

  • Vicario DS, Ghez C (1984) The control of rapid limb movement in the cat. IV. Updating of ongoing isometric responses. Exp Brain Res 55: 134–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Welford AT (1952) The psychological refractory period and the timing of high-speed performance. A review and a theory. Brit J Psychol 43: 2–19

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev 3 (2, Suppl 13): 1–114

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gordon, J., Ghez, C. Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. Exp Brain Res 67, 253–269 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00248547

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00248547

Key words

Navigation