Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 39, Issue 3, June 1991, Pages 215-258
Cognition

Critical period effects on universal properties of language: The status of subjacency in the acquisition of a second language

https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90054-8Get rights and content

Abstract

Recent studies have shown clear evidence for critical period effects for both first and second language acquisition on a broad range of learned, language-specific grammatical properties. The present studies ask whether and to what degree critical period effects can also be found for universal properties of language considered to be innate. To address this issue, native Chinese speakers who learned English as a second language were tested on the universal principle subjacency as it applies to wh-question formation in English. Subjects arrived in the U.S.A. between the ages of 4 and 38 years. They were immersed in English for a number of years (a minimum of 5) and were adults at the time of testing. Non-native performance on subjacency was found for subjects of all ages of arrival. Performance declined continuously over age of arrival until adulthood, (r = -.63). When immersion occurred as late as adulthood, performance dropped to levels slightly above chance. In all of the analyses performed, subjacency did not differ from language-specific structures in the degree or manner in which it was affected by maturation. These results suggest that whatever the nature of the endowment that allows humans to learn language, it undergoes a very broad deterioration as learners become increasingly mature.

References (39)

  • H. Clahsen et al.

    The availability of universal grammar to adult and child learners: A study of the acquisition of German word order

    Second Language Research

    (1986)
  • S. Curtiss

    Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern day “wildchild”

    (1977)
  • S. Curtiss

    The case of Chelsea: A new test case of the critical period for language acquisition

    (1988)
  • J. duPlessis et al.

    UG or not UG, that is the question: A reply to Clahsen and Muysken

    Second Language Research

    (1987)
  • N. Erteschik

    On the nature of island constraints

    MIT Dissertation

    (1973)
  • S. Flynn

    Differences between first and second language acquisition: Setting the parameters of universal grammar

  • S. Flynn

    A universal in L2 acquisition based on a PBD typology

  • E.H. Hess

    Imprinting

    (1973)
  • K. Immelmann et al.

    Sensitive phases in development

  • Cited by (243)

    • Showing strength through flexibility: Multi-accent toddlers recognize words quickly and efficiently

      2022, Brain and Language
      Citation Excerpt :

      Language researchers have long been fascinated with young children’s ability to acquire new languages and accents. Based on many decades of work, we know that children who are immersed in two languages early in life can efficiently learn both languages (e.g., Johnson & Newport, 1991), and children who move into a new dialect region are faster than adults in their adoption of the local variety of English (e.g., Chambers, 1992; Tagliamonte & Molfenter, 2007). Overall, those previous studies paint a picture where young children are optimally designed to learn to produce multiple languages at once – or to add to, or even replace, the language or language variety they speak – with what increasingly seems to be very little (if any) negative impact on their overall linguistic abilities.

    • Limited evidence for probability matching as a strategy in probability learning tasks

      2021, Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory
    • A sensitive period in the neural phenotype of language in blind individuals

      2020, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Young children acquire language rapidly and effortlessly, without explicit training (Bonvillian et al., 2012; Gleitman and Wanner, 1982; Petitto et al., 2001a, 2001b; Gleitman and Newport, 1995; Lightbrown and Spada, 1993). By contrast, when language is acquired in adulthood, learning proceeds more slowly and plateaus at lower levels of proficiency (Johnson and Newport, 1989, 1991; Neville et al., 1992; Newport et al., 2001). This pattern is evident in second language learners and individuals born deaf who do not gain access to sign language until later in life (Emmorey et al., 1995; Mayberry et al., 2011; Mayberry and Eichen, 1991; Mayberry and Lock, 2003).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This research was supported in part by NIH Training Grant no. HD07205 to the University of Illinois and by NIH Grant no. DC00167 to E. Newport and T. Supalla. We would like to thank Diane Lilo-Martin and Peter Cole for raising questions that helped stimulate this study, and Renee Baillargeon, Marie Banich, Dedre Gentner, Gabriel Hermon, Doug Medin and Lydia White and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful advice and comments, and Jeff Bettger for assistance in preparing the figures. This paper is based on a portion of the Ph.D. dissertation of J.S. Johnson (University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, 1988, unpublished). Early portions of this work were presented at the 1987 Boston University Conference on Language Development. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jacqueline Johnson. Psychology Department, Gilmer Hall, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22903, U.S.A.

    View full text