Dynamic overshoot in saccadic eye movements is caused by neurological control signal reversals

https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4886(75)90226-5Get rights and content

Abstract

Three quite different types of overshoot occur in saccadic eye movements; each has unique characteristics determined by distinct neuronal control patterns. Most saccades have dynamic overshoot; it is more prevalent among, and more prominent in, small saccades. Dynamic overshoot is caused by nonrandom reversals of the neuronal control signals. It is a monocular phenomenon. The return velocities for dynamic overshoot are equal to saccadic velocities and are much larger than vergence velocities.

References (45)

  • M.R. Clark et al.

    Control of human eye movements

    Math. Biosci.

    (1974)
  • M.R. Clark et al.

    Time optimal behavior of human saccadic eye movements

    IEEE Trans. Auto. Control

    (1975)
  • C.C. Collins

    The human oculomotor control system

  • C.C. Collins et al.

    The eye movement control signal

  • G. Cook et al.

    Dynamic behavior of human eye-positioning mechanism

    Commun. Behav. Biol.

    (1968)
  • R. Dodge

    An experimental study of visual fixation

    Psy Rev.

    (1907)
  • E.V. Evarts

    Precentral and postcentral cortical activity in association with visual triggered movement

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1974)
  • A.F. Fuchs

    Saccadic and smooth pursuit movements in the monkey

    J. Physiol.

    (1967)
  • A.F. Fuchs et al.

    Firing patterns of abducens neurons of alert monkeys in relationship to horizontal eye movement

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1970)
  • A.F. Fuchs et al.

    Development of isometric tension in simian extraocular muscle

    J. Physiol.

    (1971)
  • G.L. Gottlieb et al.

    Interactions between voluntary and postural mechanisms of the human motor system

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1970)
  • S.B. Kater

    Feeding in Helisoma trivolvis: The morphological and physiological bases of a fixed action pattern

    Amer. Zool.

    (1974)
  • Cited by (149)

    • Dynamics of plant mechanics

      2022, Progress in Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      While not incorporated in the models of Figs. 2 and 4, Stark, Bahill and colleagues have pointed out that in a predominately second-order system, active braking (a small reversed pulse just at the end of the saccade) can bring the eye quickly to rest and act as a so-called time-optimal input signal. This idea is correct on theoretical grounds (e.g., Clark and Stark, 1975), and, evidently, on experimental grounds as well (Bahill et al., 1975), although the pulse actually seen is tiny compared to what these authors anticipated. Here again, the models (Figs. 2 and 4) produced correct saccades in the absence of a component of the input signal subsequently declared by their authors to be important.

    • A clinical and neurophysiological motor signature of Unverricht–Lundborg disease

      2018, Revue Neurologique
      Citation Excerpt :

      Gain (ratio of initial saccade amplitude to final eye position on target) was computed for each centrifugal and centripetal saccade, and the gain SD (gain variation) was computed in each subject for both centrifugal and centripetal saccades. As ULD saccade gains differed from those in the HV and because the VPeak depends on saccade amplitude [28], the VPeak of six amplitudes (9°–14°) were collected. The quality of fixation was also analyzed with the square-wave jerk (horizontal saccadic intrusions) frequency (SWJF) counted during central fixation.

    • Are high lags of accommodation in myopic children due to motor deficits?

      2017, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Studies on adults showed atypical patterns such as the dynamic overshoots and oscillations in saccades (Bahill, Clark, & Stark, 1975; Bahill, Hsu, & Stark, 1978; Doslak, Dell’osso, & Daroff, 1983; Zee, Robinson, & Eng, 1979). These atypical patterns were predicted to be due to either an unstable (Zee et al., 1979) or an inaccurate pulse generator (Bahill et al., 1975). Also, atypical patterns like the double step responses found here were shown to exist with the vergence system (Alvarez, Semmlow, Yuan, & Munoz, 2000; Semmlow, Hung, Horng, & Ciuffreda, 1994).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Dr. Clark's present address is Stanford Research Institute, Menlo Park, California 94025. We thank Dr. Robert Mandell for the soft contact lenses, and Robert Kenyon, Karen Bahill, and Cynthia Cowee for their assistance. We acknowledge partial support from NIH-GM 1418.

    View full text