Elsevier

Hearing Research

Volume 34, Issue 3, August 1988, Pages 295-305
Hearing Research

Comparative psychoacoustics

https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-5955(88)90009-3Get rights and content

Abstract

Psychophysical data on unsperialized mammals commonly used in auditory research were compiled from the literature, and an attempt was made to compare the hearing capacities of these species with man. Binaural hearing and sound localization were not considered. The most complete psychoacoustic data exist for chinchilla, cat, various primates, and the mouse. The existing data include audiograms, frequency and intensity discrimination thresholds, critical masking ratios, critical bandwidths, temporal summation functions at threshold, psychophysical tuning curves, gap detection thresholds, temporal modulation transfer functions, temporal discriminations, and auditory filter shapes. In general, the qualitative forms of most all psychoacoustic functions for these mammals are similar to those for man, and there is little reason to believe that the mechanisms underlying these capacities are different across mammals. Although the discriminative capacities of humans are generally more acute than those of non-humans, the database on the capacities of non-humans is not yet sufficient for systematic comparisons across species to be made with confidence.

References (62)

  • W.W. Clark et al.

    Cochlear damage: Audiometric correlates

  • W.W. Clark et al.

    Noise-induced hearing loss in the chinchilla, as determined by a positive-reinforcement technique

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1974)
  • J.A. Costalupes

    Broadband masking noise and behavioral pure tone thresholds in cats

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1983)
  • R. Davis et al.

    Comparison between AER and behavioral thresholds in normally and abnormally hearing chinchillas

    Ear Hear.

    (1984)
  • G. Ehret

    Age-dependent hearing loss in normal hearing mice

    Naturwissenschaften

    (1974)
  • G. Ehret

    Frequency and intensity difference limens and nonlinearities in the ear of the housemouse (Mus musculus)

    J. Comp. Physiol.

    (1975)
  • G. Ehret

    Temporal summation for pure tones and white noise in the house mouse (Mus musculus)

    J. Acoust Soc. Am.

    (1976)
  • G. Ehret

    Masked auditory thresholds, critical ratios, and scales of the basilar membrane of the housemouse (Mus musculus)

    J. Comp. Physiol.

    (1975)
  • D.N. Elliot et al.

    Effects of cochlear lesions on audiograms and intensity discrimination in cats

    Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol.

    (1965)
  • D. Elliot et al.

    Determination of Absolute-intensity thresholds and frequency difference thresholds in cats

    J. Acoust Soc. Am.

    (1960)
  • R.R. Fay

    Auditory Frequency discrimination in vertebrates

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1973)
  • R.R. Fay
  • P.J. Fitzgibbons

    Temporal gap detection in noise as a function of frequency, bandwidth, and level

    J. Acoust Soc. Am.

    (1983)
  • D. Giraudi et al.

    Gap detection by the chinchilla

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1980)
  • G. Gourevitch

    Auditory masking in the rat

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1965)
  • G. Gourevitch

    Detectability of tones in quiet and in noise by rats and monkeys

  • G.G. Gourevitch

    Directional hearing in terrestrial mammals

  • G.G. Gourevitch

    Aspects of psychoacoustics in non-human primates

  • D.D. Greenwood

    Auditory masking and the critical band

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1961)
  • M. Hack

    Auditory intensity discrimination in the rat

    J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.

    (1971)
  • L. Halperin et al.

    Auditory filter shapes in the chinchilla

    J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    (1986)
  • Cited by (63)

    • Signatures of cochlear processing in neuronal coding of auditory information

      2022, Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Humans have a very accurate ability to discriminate tones whose frequencies differ by only 0.2% around 1 kHz, where our ear is the most sensitive (Moore, 1973). Frequency selectivity fluctuates from 1 to 5% in other species (Fay, 1988), but such measurements are challenging to acquire and often depend on behavioral paradigms. The perceived intensity of a pure sound is reduced in the presence of a second sound (Fletcher, 1938).

    • Extended high frequency hearing and speech perception implications in adults and children

      2020, Hearing Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      A fundamental principle of biology is that each species’ and individual’s sensory systems are tailored to meet the demands placed upon them by their environments and experiences. Accordingly, auditory systems across different species exhibit distinctive upper and lower limits for the frequency range of hearing (Fay, 1988; Heffner and Heffner, 2007; Heffner, 2004; Masterton et al., 1969). The audible frequency range for humans spans approximately 20 Hz to 20 kHz.

    • Ecological cocktail party listening reveals the utility of extended high-frequency hearing

      2019, Hearing Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Different species have distinctive upper and lower limits to the frequency range of hearing (Fay, 1988; Heffner and Heffner, 2007; Heffner, 2004; Masterton et al., 1969), each presumably tailored to enable reproductive success according to the environmental demands for that species.

    • Frequency selectivity in macaque monkeys measured using a notched-noise method

      2018, Hearing Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Therefore, we suggest that similarities in frequency-specific filter effects may emerge if the species’ audible range is taken into account. In evaluating an animal's utility as a model for human hearing, one needs a basic understanding of the animal's psychophysical auditory abilities, such as frequency selectivity (Fay, 1988). One previous review suggests that small laboratory animals, such as mice (Ehret, 1976), rats (Gourevitch, 1965), chinchillas (Niemiec et al., 1992), and cats (Nienhuys and Clark, 1979; Pickles, 1979), have broader auditory filters than humans (see Fig. 8 in Fay, 1988), which may implicate an evolutionary aspect of frequency selectivity.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text