Elsevier

Animal Behaviour

Volume 32, Issue 2, May 1984, Pages 470, IN3, 471-477
Animal Behaviour

Toque macaque food calls: Semantic communication concerning food distribution in the environment

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80283-3Get rights and content

Abstract

Upon the rare occasions that wild toque macaques (Macaca sinica) discover a large quantity of food in their natural forest environment they give distinctive calls. The durations of feeding bouts and the rates of feeding are significantly greater at sites where food calls are given than at sites of the same food types where no foods call are given. The calls designate a new location of a large quantity of any one of several food types. Dispersed group members hearing the call immediately run to the site of the call and feed there. I conclude that food calls are semantic signals (or symbols) that evoke the same response (rapid direct approach) as seeing the external referent of the calls (a source of abundant food) itself would. Food calls convey information about the presence of an edible food source, its quantity and location in the environment. As a general type, food calls appear to be related to, but are distinct from, contact calls which function to mintain group cohesion.

References (19)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (125)

  • Effect of urbanization on zoonotic gastrointestinal parasite prevalence in endemic toque macaque (Macaca sinica) from different climatic zones in Sri Lanka

    2022, International Journal for Parasitology: Parasites and Wildlife
    Citation Excerpt :

    In addition, macaques are also listed in Appendix II of CITES (Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species) that includes species not necessarily threatened with extinction (CITES, 2005). Even though there are reports on the health, behaviour, and ecology of toque macaques (Dittus, 1984, 1986; Dittus and Ratnayeke, 1989; Keane et al., 1997; Weerasekara et al., 2021), parasite diversity of these animals is not well studied. A single study was reported on the macaque populations across the country, representing the three subspecies (Huffman et al., 2013) while others were mainly focused on macaques inhabiting specific regions, i.e Polonnaruwa (M. s. sinica), Peradeniya (M. s. aurifrons) (Dewit et al., 1991; Ekanayake et al., 2004, 2006; Thilakarathne et al., 2021), and in captivity (Gunasekera et al., 2012; Aviruppola et al., 2016).

  • Fruits of the forest: Human stable isotope ecology and rainforest adaptations in Late Pleistocene and Holocene (∼36 to 3 ka) Sri Lanka

    2017, Journal of Human Evolution
    Citation Excerpt :

    This, and the above interpretation of δ13C, highlights the main problem – equifinality –with detailed primate niche reconstructions on the basis of stable isotope analysis, in the absence of modern observation data (Roberts et al., 2017 - accepted in press). Elevated δ18O could simultaneously result from M. sinica drinking from open, evaporative water sources on the ground (Fooden, 1979; Dittus, 1984) and T. vetulus being a non-obligate drinker obtaining the majority of its water from foliage (Cerling et al., 2004; Krigbaum et al., 2013). In the case of the Sri Lankan Microlithic tradition, the basic zooarchaeological identification of these monkey species, with known ecological distinctions in the present, provides a better indication of human hunting strategies.

  • Contextually variable signals can be functionally referential

    2015, Animal Behaviour
    Citation Excerpt :

    For example, fork-tailed drongos, Dicrurus adsimilis, utter drongo-specific false alarm calls but also mimic false alarm calls of other target species (e.g. meerkats, Suricata suricatta, and pied babblers, Turdoides bicolor) to scare members of such species away from their food source, which they then steal (Flower, 2011; Flower, Gribble, & Ridley, 2014). Food-associated calls typically show even less stimulus specificity than alarm signals and are often produced in a variety of nonfeeding contexts (e.g. toque macaque, Macaca sinica: Dittus, 1984; Geoffroy's spider monkey, Ateles geoffroyi: Chapman & Lefebvre, 1990; rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta: Hauser & Marler, 1993; golden lion tamarins, Leontopithecus roaslia: Halloy & Kleiman, 1994; cottontop tamarins, Saguinus oedipus: Roush & Snowdon, 2000; bonobos, Pan paniscus: Clay, Smith, & Blumstein, 2012; Clay & Zuberbühler, 2009). For example, golden lion tamarins and spider monkeys produce food calls during intergroup encounters and predator mobbing (Chapman & Lefebvre, 1990; Halloy & Kleiman, 1994).

  • Food-associated vocalizations in mammals and birds: What do these calls really mean?

    2012, Animal Behaviour
    Citation Excerpt :

    Golden-lion tamarins produce the ‘chuck’ call during feeding but also during intergroup encounters and predator mobbing (Halloy & Kleiman 1994). Toque macaques produce a specific call in response to food; however, they also sometimes produce this call during nonfood contexts associated with elation, such as at the onset of rain following dry periods, or on hot sunny days towards the end of the rainy season (Dittus 1984). Thus, although such calls may be associated with feeding, their production within nonfood contexts indicates that these calls may function more generally in social recruitment and may more accurately reflect the caller’s motivational response to an event rather than the caller’s discovery of food specifically.

View all citing articles on Scopus
*

Field address: 4/4 Galkanda Road, Anniewatte, Kandy, Sri Lanka.

View full text