Targeted gene mutation approaches to the study of anxiety-like behavior in mice

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00012-4Get rights and content

Abstract

Studying the behavioral phenotypes of transgenic and gene knockout mice is a powerful means to better understand the pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric disorders and ultimately improve their treatment. This paper provides an overview of the methods and findings of studies that have tested for anxiety-related behavioral phenotypes in gene mutant mice. In the context of improving the side effect burden of benzodiazepines, gene targeting has been valuable for dissociating the functional roles (i.e., anxiolytic, sedative, amnestic) of individual GABAA receptor subunits. Supporting the link between abnormalities in CRH function and anxiety, CRH overexpressing transgenic mice and CRH-R2 receptor knockout mutants have displayed significantly increased anxiety-like behavior, while CRH-R1 receptor knockout mice have shown an anxiolytic-like phenotype. Consistent with an important role for the serotonergic system in anxiety, 5-HT1A receptor deficient mice have consistently exhibited heightened anxiety-like behavior, while the evidence from 5-HT1B and 5-HT2C deficient mice remains somewhat equivocal. Mutant mice lacking either of the monoamine degrading enzymes, MAOA or COMT, have shown a number of behavioral and neurological effects, including alterations in anxiety-like behavior. With enhanced spatial and temporal control over gene mutations, in combination with an improved battery of behavioral tests, gene mutant mice will provide an increasingly valuable tool for understanding the neural substrates of anxiety.

Introduction

The emergence of molecular techniques that allow the alteration of genes and gene function in the intact animal is fashioning a new era in behavioral neuroscience. Protocols used in the generation of transgenic and gene knockout mice are now established as routine, and their widespread availability is producing proliferating numbers of gene mutant mice with direct relevance to the study of neural processes [1]. Fortunately for the behavioral neuroscientist, the mouse represents a good subject for studying the effects of genetic manipulations on behavior. Once generated, a mutation can be maintained in numbers of mice large enough to facilitate sound behavioral studies, with relatively little expense. Moreover, while there has been a long history of using the rat in behavioral neuroscience, many laboratories now have behavioral tests that are specifically designed, or can be successfully adapted, for use with mice.

The technology behind the generation of transgenic and gene knockout mice has been extensively described elsewhere, and the reader is referred to one of many excellent sources for a fuller discussion [2], [3], [4]. A transgenic mouse is often generated in order to study the functional consequences of gene overexpression. Briefly, this is achieved by microinjecting foreign DNA containing copies of a given gene into developing mouse embryos, where they have a chance to integrate into the host genome. The overall level of transgene expression is largely determined by the location at which the transgene inserts into the genome, which is random. However, patterns of expression can be directed to specific areas of the brain by using a region or cell specific promoter. With a gene knockout mouse, one can study the effects of removing a gene. First a DNA construct is designed that causes a functional disruption in the gene of interest. The DNA is then integrated into pluripotent embryonic stem cells (via homologous recombination) and inserted into foster embryos. Progeny are examined to see whether they have incorporated the null mutation (e.g., via coat color), and those chimeric offspring carrying the mutation in germline cells (thereby allowing the mutation to be transferred across generations) can be interbred to produce mice that are heterozygous for the missing gene. Assuming that the absence of the gene does not impact prenatal survival, interbreeding heterozygote offspring will normally give a complement of heterozygous null mutant, homozygous null mutant and ‘wild type’ littermate controls, in the ratio of 2:1:1.

Where a genetic alteration does impact prenatal survival, studying the behavioral consequences of that mutation is, of course, impossible. The ‘classical’ methods for generating gene knockout and transgenic mice produce genetic mutations in all cells where the gene is expressed, throughout ontogeny and adulthood. Therefore, even in viable animals, the presence of the mutation during development can complicate interpretation of behavioral phenotypes (observed or absent) in a mutant mouse due to indirect or multiple effects of the gene mutation. Thus, additional gene product in a transgenic mouse may lead to a cascade of molecular and neurochemical effects, confusing the causal link between phenotype and targeted gene. Conversely, where a gene has been inactivated, developmental adaptations may compensate for the deletion, thereby masking that gene's normal function. On one level, developmental effects can provide insight into the plasticity of neural systems [5]. Moreover, rendering a genetic alteration that is both global and chronic is a powerful means to model genetic contributions to a neurological or a neuropsychiatric disorder, when that is the goal. However, these same factors become undesirable when chronic, global expression impacts survival, or when studying the function of a gene in the normal adult brain [6]. In this context, techniques which allow for greater control over the temporal and spatial characteristics of a gene mutation represent an important advance [7].

One technique which produces regional restriction of a gene deletion works by flanking the gene of interest with loxP sequences that act as recognition sites for the bacteriophage enzyme Cre recombinase [8]. Intercrossing animals carrying the loxP flanking regions with Cre-expressing transgenic mice will result in excision of the ‘floxed’ site containing the target gene. Therefore, when Cre is driven by a promoter that is specific for a given region, inactivation of the gene is restricted to that region. In order to add temporal control over the genetic mutation, Cre expression can be linked to a regulating system, such as tetracycline or interferon [9]. The ability to determine where and when a gene is inactivated or overexpressed is already providing important insights into the link between genes and learning and memory [10], [11], and it is hoped that it will have a similar impact for studying anxiety-related processes. Of course, understanding the effects of increasingly sophisticated genetic manipulations necessitates sounds methods for behavioral phenotyping. The next section provides and introduction and overview of some of the behavioral methods used to assess anxiety-related phenotypes in gene mutant mice.

Section snippets

Exploration-based tests

Numerous tests have been designed to test anxiety-like behavior in rodents [12], [13]. The majority of these behavioral paradigms were developed and pharmacologically-validated in rats and some valuable tests have not readily transferred for use in mice, e.g., the social interaction test [14]. Nonetheless, there remains a wide choice of established behavioral tests with which to test for anxiety-related phenotypes in a transgenic or gene knockout mouse. Many of the behavioral assays that are

Anxiety-related phenotypes in mutant mice

Historically, understanding the neural bases of anxiety-related behavior in rodents has been mainly driven by studying the effects of exogenous manipulations (pharmacological, electrical), and the consequences of neural ablations. Perhaps the principal disadvantage of these approaches is that they can lack selectivity or reproducibility. With the molecular approach, brain manipulations can potentially be rendered at the genetic level with both precision and reliability. The literature on the

Concluding remarks

Studies in transgenic and gene knockout mice have already produced some significant insights into the neural mechanisms underlying anxiety. Evidence that mice with either loss-of-function or gain-of-function mutations in a specific gene exhibit significant alterations in anxiety-related behavior can have direct implications for the discovery of novel drug therapies for anxiety disorders. In this context, the more recent clinical classifications have increasingly considered anxiety as a

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Dr Jacqueline Crawley, Dr Dennis Murphy and Professor John Rodgers for their critical reading of a draft of this manuscript, and Sara Kinsley for her help in constructing the References Section.

References (152)

  • R.J. Nelson et al.

    Behavior in mice with targeted disruption of single genes

    Neurosci Biobehav Rev

    (1998)
  • M.R. Capecchi

    The new mouse genetics: altering the genome by gene targeting

    Trends Genet

    (1989)
  • A. Zimmer

    Manipulating the genome by homologous recombination in embryonic stem cells

    Annu Rev Neurosci

    (1992)
  • A.L. Joyner

    Gene targeting: a practical approach

    (1993)
  • J.N. Crawley

    Unusual behavioral phenotypes of inbred mouse strains

    Trends Neurosci

    (1996)
  • A. Routtenberg

    Knockout mouse fault lines

    Nature

    (1995)
  • J.R. Gingrich et al.

    Inducible gene expression in the nervous system of transgenic mice

    Annu Rev Neurosci

    (1998)
  • B. Sauer

    Manipulation of transgenes by site-specific recombination: use of Cre recombinase

    Methods Enzymol

    (1993)
  • R. Kuhn et al.

    Inducible gene targeting in mice

    Science

    (1995)
  • Y.P. Tang et al.

    Genetic enhancement of learning and memory in mice

    Nature

    (1999)
  • M. Mayford et al.

    Control of memory formation through regulated expression of a CaMK11 transgene

    Science

    (1996)
  • R.G. Lister

    Ethologically-based animal models of anxiety disorders

    Pharmacol Ther

    (1990)
  • G. Griebel

    5-Hydroxytryptamine-interacting drugs in animal models of anxiety disorders: more than 30 years of research

    Pharmacol Ther

    (1995)
  • S.E. File et al.

    Can social interaction be used to measure anxiety?

    Br J Pharmacol

    (1978)
  • J.N. Crawley

    Exploratory behavior models of anxiety in mice

    Neurosci Biobehav Rev

    (1985)
  • C.S. Hall

    Emotional behavior in the rat. I. Defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality

    J Comp Psychol

    (1936)
  • S.A. Barnett

    The rat: a study in behavior

    (1963)
  • N.E. Grossen et al.

    Species-specific behavior and acquisition of avoidance behavior in rats

    J Comp Physiol Psychol

    (1972)
  • D. Treit et al.

    Thigmotaxis as a test for anxiolytic activity in rats

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1988)
  • M. Xu et al.

    Dopamine D1 receptor mutant mice are deficient in striatal expression of dynorphin and in dopamine-mediated behavioral responses

    Cell

    (1994)
  • R.J. Blanchard et al.

    Passive and active reactions to fear-eliciting stimuli

    J Comp Physiol Psychol

    (1969)
  • J. Flint et al.

    A simple genetic basis for a complex psychological trait in laboratory mice

    Science

    (1995)
  • D. Accili et al.

    A targeted mutation of the D3 dopamine receptor gene is associated with hyperactivity in mice

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (1996)
  • H. Steiner et al.

    D3 dopamine receptor-deficient mouse: evidence for reduced anxiety

    Physiol Behav

    (1997)
  • J. Crawley et al.

    Preliminary report of a simple animal behavior model for the anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1980)
  • J.N. Crawley

    Neuropharmacologic specificity of a simple animal model for the behavioral actions of benzodiazepines

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1981)
  • B. Costall et al.

    Actions of sulpiride and tiapride in a simple model of anxiety in mice

    Neuropharmacology

    (1987)
  • C. Mathis et al.

    Characterization of benzodiazepine-sensitive behaviors in the A/J and C57BL/6J inbred strains of mice

    Behav, Genet

    (1994)
  • G. Griebel et al.

    Limited anxiolytic-like effects of non-benzodiazepine hypnotics in rodents

    J Psychopharmacol

    (1998)
  • S. Hogg

    A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1996)
  • K.C. Montgomery

    The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behaviour

    J Comp Physiol Psychol

    (1955)
  • S.L. Handley et al.

    Effects of alpha-adrenoceptor agonists and antagonists in a maze-exploration model of ‘fear’-motivated behaviour

    Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol

    (1984)
  • S. Pellow et al.

    Validation of open:closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat

    J Neurosci Methods

    (1985)
  • R.G. Lister

    The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse

    Psychopharmacology

    (1987)
  • S.E. File et al.

    Trial 2 in the elevated plus-maze: a different form of fear?

    Psychopharmacology

    (1993)
  • A. Ramos et al.

    Multiple-test study of anxiety-related behaviours in six inbred rat strains

    Behav Brain Res

    (1997)
  • A. Holmes et al.

    Responses of Swiss-Webster mice to repeated plus-maze experience: further evidence for a qualitative shift in emotional state?

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1998)
  • D.C. Blanchard et al.

    Diazepam changes risk assessment in an anxiety/defense test battery

    Psychopharmacology

    (1990)
  • R.J. Rodgers et al.

    Anxiety enhancement in the murine elevated plus maze by immediate prior exposure to social stressors

    Physiol Behav

    (1993)
  • H.P. Kaesermann

    Stretched attend posture, a non-social form of ambivalence, is sensitive to a conflict-reducing drug action

    Psychopharmacology

    (1986)
  • R.J. Blanchard et al.

    Antipredator defensive behaviors in a visible burrow system

    J Comp Psychol

    (1989)
  • H.E. Molewijk et al.

    The ambivalent behaviour ‘stretched approach posture’ in the rat as a paradigm to characterize anxiolytic drugs

    Psychopharmacology

    (1995)
  • G. Griebel et al.

    Risk assessment behaviour: evaluation of utility in the study of 5-HT-related drugs in the rat elevated plus-maze test

    Pharmacol Biochem Behav

    (1997)
  • R.J. Rodgers et al.

    Corticosterone response to the plus-maze: high correlation with risk assessment in rats and mice

    Physiol Behav

    (1999)
  • D. Brunner et al.

    Anxiety, motor activation, and maternal-infant interactions in 5HT1B knockout mice

    Behav Neurosci

    (1999)
  • L.K. Heisler et al.

    Elevated anxiety and antidepressant-like responses in serotonin 5-HT1A receptor mutant mice

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (1998)
  • J.K. Shepherd et al.

    Behavioural and pharmacological characterisation of the elevated ‘zero-maze’ as an animal model of anxiety

    Psychopharmacology

    (1994)
  • S.F. Kash et al.

    Increased anxiety and altered responses to anxiolytics in mice deficient in the 65-kDa isoform of glutamic acid decarboxylase

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (1999)
  • G. Griebel et al.

    The free-exploratory paradigm: an effective method for measuring neophobic behaviour in mice and testing potential neophobia-reducing drugs

    Behav Pharm

    (1993)
  • F. Crestani et al.

    Decreased GABAA-receptor clustering results in enhanced anxiety and a bias for threat cues

    Nat Neurosci

    (1999)
  • Cited by (175)

    • Genetics of Stress Responsiveness

      2020, Stress: Genetics, Epigenetics and Genomics Volume 4: Handbook of Stress
    • The role of CaMKII-Tiam1 complex on learning and memory

      2019, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text