Elsevier

Behavioural Brain Research

Volume 192, Issue 1, 1 September 2008, Pages 124-136
Behavioural Brain Research

Review
The problem of relating plasticity and skilled reaching after motor cortex stroke in the rat

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.12.026Get rights and content

Abstract

The plasticity of the nervous system is illustrated in the many new neuronal connections that are formed during the acquisition of behavioral skills, loss of function after brain injury, and subsequent recovery of function. The present review describes the acquisition of skilled reaching, the act of reaching for food with a forelimb, and the changes that take place in skilled reaching following motor cortex stroke. The review then discusses the difficulty in associating plastic changes with specific aspects of behavioral change. Skilled reaching behavior is complex and consists of a number of oppositions (stimulus response relationships), between the rat and the food target, a number of forelimb gestures (non-weight supporting movements), which are performed to obtain food, and a complex series of segmental movements (of the limb, head, and trunk), all of which influence the success of the act. Measures of these four aspects of skilled reaching behavior following motor cortex stroke reveal that there are a number of learned changes that take place at different times, including learned nonuse, learned bad-use, and forgetting. The widespread dendritic proliferation, axonal growth, and synaptic formation that take place both before and after stroke are difficult to precisely relate to these behavioral changes. Whereas plasticity is usually proposed to be associated with improved performance it is suggested that future work should attempt to better relate plastic changes to the details of behavioral changes.

Introduction

There is evidence to show that the nervous system is plastic in forming new connections between neurons as a result of experience. Plasticity can be associated with the birth of new neurons, increases in the complexity of the dendritic arbor of existing neurons, and increases in the number, size, and shape of synapses. Following brain injury, plastic changes can also be widespread and can additionally include the growth of anomalous pathways. In much of the literature on neural plasticity, it is assumed that most changes are beneficial and mediate the acquisition of new behavior as well as any functional recovery that occurs after brain damage. Nevertheless, Paillard [49] has questioned this assumption by positing that behavior can be modified in a number of ways, not all of which involve plasticity. Thus, he argues that understanding functional acquisition and the loss and recovery of function after brain injury requires distinguishing between behavioral changes that are mediated by activation of pre-existing neuronal connections and behavioral changes that are due to plastic changes. Such a distinction is theoretically reasonable, but there is challenge in determining the relationship between neuronal alteration and behavioral change. As we will describe in the first section of this paper, behavioral changes are every bit as complex as neuronal changes. We will illustrate this point by describing some aspects of a behavior that fascinated Paillard [50], prehension. We will then consider the evidence that plastic changes are associated with prehension both before and during a variety of events related to recovery of function after brain injury. The future challenge is to document more precisely the relationship between behavior and neuronal plasticity.

Section snippets

Rodent skilled reaching

Prehension, or the ability to use one or both hands/paws to reach for and grasp an object, has a long evolutionary history and is a behavior that is displayed in many terrestrial vertebrates orders [26]. Because the behavior is displayed in animal species that have no direct connections onto motor neurons, no direct corticospinal pathways into the motor neuron pools of the spinal cord, by animals with a rudimentary forebrain and no corticospinal tract, as well as in animals with complex

Measuring skilled reaching

The skilled reaching behavior of the rat has been studied in a surprising array of tasks. In the first documentation of skilled reaching in the rat, Peterson [51] trained rats to reach for food from a tray that could be accessed by advancing the paw through an aperture. Subsequently, many variations of the “tray task” have been developed, including trays that hold but a single food pellet [9]. A somewhat more sophisticated and demanding task requires a rat to reach for a single pellet located

The modular organization of skilled reaching

Skilled reaching in the rat takes place in less than a third of a second and the speed of the act suggested to early investigators that the movement is a preprogrammed ballistic movement [6]. As such, the movement would consist of a single action dependent on rather simple neural control. Detailed high-speed video analysis and descriptive approaches using movement notation techniques show that skilled reaching is a composite movement. Because skilled reaching consists of combining separate more

Motor cortex stroke and skilled reaching

In the earliest study of cortical control of skilled reaching, Peterson and Francarol [52] delineated the region of the neocortex in which stroke produced a change of “handedness” in pretrained rats. They found that the most effective region for producing a change in handedness is the motor cortex. Later Castro [9] argued that damage to motor cortex disrupted success in some rats by impairing the effective use of digits, although this conclusion was inferred because he had no way of directly

Cognitive changes associated with recovery from motor cortex stroke

Stroke in human patients is accompanied not only by motor deficits, but also changes in emotion, alterations in mood, changes in motivation, and memory alterations for many skilled movements (e.g. [41], [56]). These changes may limit recovery, have negative consequences on remaining skills, and limit participation in rehabilitative programs. It is thus relevant to ask whether, in addition to motoric changes, there are nonmotoric alterations following motor cortex stroke in rats. The following

Plasticity and brain injury

We have seen that behavioral change following injury or training can result from a range of behavioral compensations and strategies. It is generally assumed that these behavioral changes will be correlated with plastic changes in cerebral organization that underlie the behavior. There appear to be multiple types of synaptic changes, however, which are providing a challenge in finding meaningful behavior–brain associations.

The first systematic studies showing that experience altered cerebral

What does the fractionation of reaching behavior reveal about plasticity?

The variation in recovery on different endpoint measures and the organization of skilled reaching into subcomponents as defined by oppositions, gestures, and segmental movements implies different neural changes at a number of hierarchical levels of the nervous system. In other words, because individual movements are combined to produce skilled reaching, plastic changes are necessary to link them into the skilled reaching act.

The presence of three oppositions in skilled reaching reveals that

Conclusions

The challenge in improving functional recover after brain damage is in knowing what neural change goes with which behavioral change. As the present review summarizes, behavioral changes associated with learning and recovery from brain injury are complex, even for a behavior as specific as skilled reaching. There are changes in endpoint performance, oppositions, gestures, and segmental movements as well as changes in cognition. Changes in the nervous system are also abundant and complex and

Acknowledgements

This research was sponsored by grants from the Natural Sciences and Research Council of Canada and by the Canadian Stroke Network

References (75)

  • T.A. Jones et al.

    Synaptogenesis and dendritic growth in the cortex opposite unilateral sensorimotor cortex damage in adult rats: a quantitative electron microscopic examination

    Brain Res

    (1996)
  • J.M. Juraska

    Gender differences in the dendritic tree of granule neurons in the hippocampal dentate gyrus of weaning age rats

    Brain Res Dev Brain Res

    (1990)
  • B. Kolb et al.

    Experience-dependent changes in dendritic arbor and spine density in neocortex vary with age and sex

    Neurobiol Learn Mem

    (2003)
  • E.I. Miklyaeva et al.

    The ground reaction forces of postural adjustments during skilled reaching in unilateral dopamine-depleted hemiparkinson rats

    Behav Brain Res

    (1997)
  • R.J. Nudo

    Mechanisms for recovery of motor function following cortical damage

    Curr Opin Neurobiol

    (2006)
  • R.J. Nudo

    Plasticity

    NeuroRx

    (2006)
  • T.P. Platz et al.

    Motor learning after recovery from hemiparesis

    Neuropsychologia

    (1994)
  • G. Silasi et al.

    Chronic inhibition of cyclooxygenase-2 induces dendritic hypertrophy and limited functional improvement following motor cortex stroke

    Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • A.M. Turner et al.

    Differential rearing effects on rat visual cortex synapses. I. Synaptic and neuronal density and synapses per neuron

    Brain Res

    (1985)
  • C.J. Winstein et al.

    Motor learning after unilateral brain damage

    Neuropsychologia

    (1999)
  • I.Q. Whishaw

    Lateralization and reaching skill related: Results and implications from a large sample of Long-Evans rats

    Behav Brain Res

    (1992)
  • I.Q. Whishaw

    Loss of the innate cortical engram for action patterns used in skilled reaching and the development of behavioral compensation following motor cortex lesions in the rat

    Neuropharmacology

    (2000)
  • I.Q. Whishaw

    Did a change in sensory control of skilled movements stimulate the evolution of the primate frontal cortex?

    Behav Brain Res

    (2003)
  • I.Q. Whishaw et al.

    The structure of skilled forelimb reaching in the rat: a proximally driven movement with a single distal rotatory component

    Behav Brain Res

    (1990)
  • I.Q. Whishaw et al.

    Proximal and distal impairments in rat forelimb use in reaching follow unilateral pyramidal tract lesions

    Behav Brain Res

    (1993)
  • I.Q. Whishaw et al.

    Neurotoxic lesions of the caudate–putamen on a reaching for food task in the rat: acute sensorimotor neglect and chronic qualitative motor impairment follow lateral lesions and improved success follows medial lesions

    Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • M. Alaverdashvili et al.

    “Learned baduse” limits recovery of skilled reaching for food after forelimb motor cortex stroke in rats: a new analysis of the effect of gestures on success

    Behav Brain Res

    (2007)
  • Alaverdashvili M, Leblond H, Rossignol, Whishaw IQ. Cineradiographic (video X-ray) analysis of skilled forelimb...
  • M. Alaverdashvili et al.

    No improvement by amphetamine on learned nonuse, attempts, success, or movement in skilled reaching by the rat after motor cortex stroke

    Eur J Neurosci

    (2007)
  • J. Biernaskie et al.

    Efficacy of rehabilitative experience declines with time after focal ischemic brain injury

    J Neurosci

    (2004)
  • J. Biernaskie et al.

    Enriched rehabilitative training promotes improved forelimb motor function and enhanced dendritic growth after focal ischemic injury

    J Neurosci

    (2001)
  • C.M. Butefisch

    Neurobiological bases of rehabilitation

    Neurol Sci

    (2006)
  • M.C. Cirstea et al.

    Compensatory strategies for reaching in stroke

    Brain

    (2000)
  • M.C. Cirstea et al.

    Feedback and cognition in arm motor skill reacquisition after stroke

    Stroke

    (2006)
  • Comeau W, McDonald R, Kolb B. Learning-induced alterations in prefrontal cortical circuitry, submitted for...
  • A. Dickinson

    Actions and habits: the development of behavioral anatomy

    Phil Trans R Soc Lond

    (1985)
  • N. Eshkol et al.

    Movement notation

    (1958)
  • Cited by (73)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text