Age-related changes in brain activation during a delayed item recognition task

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.03.002Get rights and content

Abstract

To test competing models of age-related changes in brain functioning (capacity limitation, neural efficiency, compensatory reorganization, and dedifferentiation), young (n = 40; mean age = 25.1 years) and elderly (n = 18; mean age = 74.4 years) subjects performed a delayed item recognition task for visually presented letters with three set sizes (1, 3, or 6 letters) while being scanned with BOLD fMRI. Spatial patterns of brain activity corresponding to either the slope or y-intercept of fMRI signal with respect to set size during memory set encoding, retention delay, or probe stimulus presentation trial phases were compared between elder and young populations. Age effects on fMRI slope during encoding and on fMRI y-intercept during retention delay were consistent with neural inefficiency; age effects on fMRI slope during retention delay were consistent with dedifferentiation. None of the other fMRI signal components showed any detectable age effects. These results suggest that, even within the same task, the nature of brain activation changes with aging can vary based on cognitive process engaged.

Introduction

Four extant hypotheses concerning changes in brain function with aging are compensatory reorganization, dedifferentiation, computational capacity limitation, and neural inefficiency. The purpose of the current paper is to test the ability of these hypotheses to predict age-related changes in brain function associated with various aspects of cognitive processing, including verbal working memory (WM) maintenance, engaged during performance of a delayed item recognition (DIR) task for letters.

WM is a psychological construct used to describe the maintenance and manipulation of information on a time scale of seconds [7]. WM seems to be divided into verbal, spatial, and object sub-systems [8], [37], [82], [94]. Verbal WM is thought to be critical for language comprehension and reasoning [5]. Based on neuropsychological dissociations [95], [98] and word length, phonemic similarity, irrelevant speech, and articulatory suppression effects [6], [11], [22], [51], the maintenance of information in verbal WM has been modeled as an articulatory loop in which sub-vocal rehearsal refreshes a phonological store. Experimental variation of the amount of information to be stored in verbal WM (WM load) has yielded findings of increases in fMRI signal in premotor, parietal, inferior frontal, and middle frontal areas [55], [72], [73], [76], [96]. At least some aspects of articulatory loop neural processing vary in intensity with WM load [45], [96], [106].

Even in the absence of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and other recognized brain diseases, aging is associated with impairment in several different memory variables [78], including WM [9], [17], [23], [49]. In particular, load-dependent deficits in WM processing have been observed with normal aging [2], [23], [26], [62], [64]. Age-related deficits in cognition are assumed to stem from age-related brain pathology [88]. Normal aging is associated with a decrease in neuropil and neuronal number in cortex [12], [21], [25] and in the subiculum region of the hippocampus [84], [99], an increase in the number of infarcts in cortex, basal ganglia, and white matter [65], an increase in MRI white matter lesions [80], an increase in density of neurofibrillary tangles in the CA1 region of the hippocampus [81], and a global decrease in gray matter volume [31].

There is the broad question of whether the functional neural circuitry of the brain remains static in the face of this neuropathology. Though not exhaustive, four extant hypotheses concerning changes in brain function with normal aging are compensatory reorganization, dedifferentiation, computational capacity limitation, and neural inefficiency. The purpose of the current study is to test the ability of these hypotheses to predict age-related changes in brain function associated with load-dependent and load-independent aspects of encoding, storage/rehearsal, and recognition/response components of a DIR task for letters [90], which is thought to tap verbal WM maintenance. These four hypotheses will now be briefly described, in turn.

Some have put forward a hypothesis that the brain is constructed such that it can in some sense compensate for neuropathology (such as that associated with normal aging) via macro-reorganization of neural circuits [4], [10], [14], [33], [87], [100]. The teleological argument is that the effect of this reorganization would be to reduce or potentially even eliminate any behavioral consequences of the neuropathology that would otherwise occur. Compensatory reorganization, occurring to varying degrees across individuals, could potentially explain how age-associated neuropathology exists even in certain proportions of the non-demented elderly [34], [81], [83], [85], and why variability in cognitive functioning increases with age [18]. Consistent with a special version of the compensatory reorganization hypothesis referred to as HAROLD (hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults [15]), a more bilateral PFC fMRI activation pattern in older adults than younger adults has been reported in word encoding [60], [71], source memory [14], retrieval [53], working memory [15], [69], and visual attention task contexts [15].

The types of compensatory reorganization models that we consider here (subsuming, but not limited to, the HAROLD model) posit that higher performing elders are higher performing because of a change in brain reorganization relative to both young subjects and lower performing elders. Therefore, under this type of compensatory reorganization there would be a cross-sectional correlation within elders between the degree of brain reorganization and performance, such that the brain activation patterns of higher performing elders would be more dissimilar than those of lower performing elders to young subject activation patterns [14]. We refer to all such models as cross-sectional compensatory reorganization models, to distinguish them from other types of compensatory models which do not require such cross-sectional correlations [89]. The current work can only weakly test the latter type of models, so we focus on testing cross-sectional compensatory reorganization models.

Dedifferentiation is another hypothesis that predicts non-identical brain activity patterns between young and elder populations. But, unlike compensatory reorganization, this change is not beneficial for the behavior in question, and is thought to represent a general deterioration in the integrity of brain circuitry [15]. Dedifferentiation and compensatory reorganization can be distinguished as the two make opposite predictions concerning the cross-sectional relationship of age-related differences in activation patterns and performance.

A critical notion concerning both of these theories is that the spatial pattern of neuronal activity in a brain that has been reorganized or de-differentiated is not identical to within a scaling factor to the corresponding canonical pattern of brain activation (in our case, that of the healthy, young population). From here on, the phrase “identical patterns” implicitly means identical to within a scaling factor. In Section 1.5, we discuss the method used to test whether elder and young activation patterns are identical.

Another general hypothesis regarding the effect of neuropathology on brain function is a reduction in the capacity of information representation or throughput in a brain circuit. This might perhaps be caused by a limitation on the amount or quality of information entering a brain circuit due to impairment in sensory systems [35], [47], [50]. A simple reduction in computational capacity would predict, in the context of identical task stimuli and instructions, a decrease in both performance and neurophysiologic activity (i.e., less total ionic flux across neuronal membranes due to synaptic transmission, therefore less ATP utilization through ionic pumps, and presumably less cerebral blood flow), and so would arguably be associated with identical brain activity patterns in young and elders. Reductions in activation with aging have been reported in anterior frontal cortex [32], [57], [79], dorsolateral PFC [41], [43], [58], [74], [75], hippocampus [57], [59], anterior cingulate [58], temporal [40], parietal [40], [58] and occipital cortices [15], [33], [40], [52], [57]. Some of these reduced activations have been associated with age-related impairments in certain cognitive functions, such as resolution of competing response impetuses [43], memory scanning speed [74], and feature binding [59].

Often, age-related decreases in activation in certain areas have been found concomitantly with age-related increases in other areas [15], [33], [57], [58], [75], which is inconsistent with a simple capacity limitation hypothesis. Increases in brain activation in a behaviorally impaired group have been sometimes conceived as a reduction in neural efficiency [16], [76]. Here, we define neural efficiency as the amount of performance-relevant computational work (operationalized here as measures of behavioral performance) performed per unit of synaptic activity (operationalized here as BOLD fMRI signal change). We also define neural inefficiency (which might be a more stable measure than neural efficiency) as simply the reciprocal of neural efficiency. We consider neural (in)efficiency as being not the property of individual regions, but as a property of a brain system/circuit. For example, if elders show lowered performance on average compared to young subjects and engage the same brain system during task performance but to a greater degree, then we would say that the elders’ brain system is less efficient than that of young subjects. Neural efficiency, like capacity limitation, would be associated with identical patterns of brain activation in young and elders.

We have established a dichotomy between models of age-related brain activation change with respect to changes in brain activation patterns. On the one hand, compensatory reorganization and de-differentiation both predict (on average) different activation patterns in young and elders. On the other, neural inefficiency and capacity limitation both predict identical activation patterns in young and elders. Obviously, then, discrimination between these two pairs of hypotheses requires some sort of test as to whether young and elder brain activation patterns are identical.

Certain approaches that have been used previously to compare patterns of brain activation have caveats. Direct comparison of voxel-wise signal intensities between groups via statistical parametric mapping (SPM; [15]) is an ambiguous test of identical spatial patterns, as even a pure scaling could lead to the existence of true voxel-wise intensity differences (Fig. 1). Also, visual comparison of group [14], [32] or condition-specific [73] thresholded statistical maps suffers from the same problem [77] (Fig. 1). An additional difficulty of this latter method is that substantial variability between realizations of thresholded maps would be expected due to intermediate levels of statistical power [20], [70]. Finally, region of interest laterality indices [13], though providing a valid test of identical patterns of region of interest effects in noiseless data, can be exceedingly unstable in practice due to their involving ratios of estimated activation; moreover, they do not compare entire brain activation patterns.

To test for non-identity of young and elder fMRI activation patterns, we used sequential latent root testing in the context of a canonical variates analysis (CVA) for imaging data with spatially correlated errors [103]. Unlike partial least-squares [56], this CVA method is invariant to linear transformations of the predictor variables and provides parametric distributional approximations that are valid for correlated observations, as is the case in our repeated measures design [103].

Section snippets

Study population

Forty healthy, young subjects (31 M and 9 F; mean (S.D.) age = 25.1 (3.9); mean years of education = 15.7 (1.4); all right handed), recruited through flyers posted at the Columbia University campus and advertisements placed in local newspapers, and 18 healthy, elderly subjects recruited from senior centers in the New York City area (7 M and 11 F; mean (S.D.) age = 74.4 (6.9); mean (S.D.) years of education = 15.3 (2.4); all right handed) participated. Global cognitive functioning was assessed with the

Reaction time

As expected [90], reaction time was affected by set size in both young (F(1.8, 68.9) = 106.2, p < 0.001) and elder (F(1.7, 28.0) = 77.8, p < 0.001) subjects (Fig. 3). The interaction of age group and set size was significant (F(1.7, 96.1) = 6.1, p = 0.005). Relatedly, a direct comparison of RT slopes revealed a significantly larger slope in elders (59.0 (30.8) ms/letter in young, 85.7 (35.7) ms/letter in elders; t(56) = −2.91, two-tailed p = 0.005). The variability of RT slope in the elder group was not

Age effects on brain activation associated with performance of a DIR task

In the current study, aging was associated with set size dependent decrements in both DIR accuracy and memory scanning speed, which is consistent with previous findings [2], [23], [26], [62]. However, as indexed with the y-intercept of the RT versus set size relationship, we did not detect an expected overall slowing with aging. Assuming an age effect size on reaction time corresponding to R = 0.50 [97], power for the current design was >0.9 with α controlled at 0.05.

Nevertheless, in the context

Conclusions

None of the models under consideration were able to explain all of the age-related differences in brain activity during performance of a DIR task. There was evidence for neural inefficiency (load-dependent aspects of encoding and load-independent aspects of verbal WM) and dedifferentiation (or possibly compensation [89]; load-dependent aspects of verbal WM maintenance). Neither capacity limitation nor cross-sectional models of compensatory reorganization were supported for any effect examined.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by NIA grants RO1 AG026158 and RR00645. Eric Zarahn was supported in part by a 2002 NARSAD Young Investigator Award.

References (107)

  • S.A. Huettel et al.

    The effects of aging upon the hemodynamic response measured by functional MRI

    Neuroimage

    (2001)
  • T. Iidaka et al.

    An fMRI study of the functional neuroanatomy of picture encoding in younger and older adults

    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res

    (2001)
  • S.C. Johnson et al.

    The relationship between fMRI activation and cerebral atrophy: comparison of normal aging and Alzheimer disease

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • M. Jueptner et al.

    Review: does measurement of regional cerebral blood flow reflect synaptic activity?—implications for PET and fMRI

    Neuroimage

    (1995)
  • M.P. Kirschen et al.

    Load- and practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal working memory: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • D.J. Madden et al.

    Adult age differences in regional cerebral blood flow during visual world identification: evidence from H215O PET

    Neuroimage

    (1996)
  • D. Manoach et al.

    Schizophrenic subjects show aberrant fMRI activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia during working memory performance

    Biol Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • A.R. McIntosh et al.

    Spatial pattern analysis of functional brain images using partial least squares

    Neuroimage

    (1996)
  • M.P. Milham et al.

    Attentional control in the aging brain: insights from an fMRI study of the stroop task

    Brain Cogn

    (2002)
  • K.J. Mitchell et al.

    fMRI evidence of age-related hippocampal dysfunction in feature binding in working memory

    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res

    (2000)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Intelligence and individual differences in becoming neurally efficient

    Acta Psychologica

    (2004)
  • S. Ogawa et al.

    Functional brain mapping by blood oxygenation level-dependent contrast magnetic resonance imaging. A comparison of signal characteristics with a biophysical model

    Biophys J

    (1993)
  • D.P. Perl et al.

    Neuropathology of dementia

    Neurol Clin

    (1986)
  • C. Ranganath et al.

    Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes

    Neuropsychologia

    (2004)
  • F. Remy et al.

    Verbal episodic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: a combined structural and functional MRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • B. Rypma et al.

    Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • Z.S. Saad et al.

    The spatial extent of the BOLD response

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • T. Thomsen et al.

    Brain localization of attentional control in different age groups by combining functional and structural MRI. [Erratum appears in Neuroimage 2005;25(1) 332. Note: Rimol Lars Morten [added]]

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • M.C. Tresch et al.

    Double dissociation of spatial and object visual memory: evidence from selective interference in intact human subjects

    Neuropsychologia

    (1993)
  • G. Vallar et al.

    Fractionation of working memory—neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store

    J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav

    (1984)
  • D.J. Veltman et al.

    Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • M.J. West

    Regionally specific loss of neurons in the aging human hippocampus

    Neurobiol Aging

    (1993)
  • P. Alvarez et al.

    The animal-model of human amnesia—long-term-memory impaired and short-term-memory intact

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (1994)
  • T. Anders et al.

    Effects of age upon retrieval from short-term memory

    Dev Psychol

    (1972)
  • P. Atchley et al.

    Spatial cuing in a stereoscopic display: attention remains “depth-aware” with age

    J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci

    (1998)
  • L. Backman et al.

    Brain regions associated with episodic retrieval in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease

    Neurology

    (1999)
  • A. Baddeley

    Working memory

    Science

    (1992)
  • A. Baddeley et al.

    Is the word length effect in STM entirely attributable to output delay? Evidence from serial recognition

    Q J Exp Psychol A

    (2002)
  • A.D. Baddeley

    Working memory

    (1986)
  • A. Baudouin et al.

    Age-related cognitive slowing: the role of spontaneous tempo and processing speed

    Exp Aging Res

    (2004)
  • J.T. Becker et al.

    Compensatory reallocation of brain resources supporting verbal episodic memory in Alzheimer's disease

    Neurology

    (1996)
  • H. Brody

    The nervous system and aging

    Adv Pathobiol

    (1980)
  • R. Cabeza

    Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model

    Psychol Aging

    (2002)
  • R. Cabeza et al.

    Task-independent and task-specific age effects on brain activity during working memory, visual attention and episodic retrieval

    Cereb Cortex

    (2004)
  • J. Callicott et al.

    Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited

    Cereb Cortex

    (2000)
  • N. Chaytor et al.

    Working memory and aging: a cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis using a self-ordered pointing task

    J Int Neuropsychol Soc

    (2004)
  • H. Christensen et al.

    An analysis of diversity in the cognitive performance of elderly community dwellers: individual differences in change scores as a function of age

    Psychol Aging

    (1999)
  • J. Cohen et al.

    PsyScope: an interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers

    Behav. Res. Methods, Instrum. Comput.

    (1993)
  • M.S. Cohen et al.

    Stability, repeatability, and the expression of signal magnitude in functional magnetic resonance imaging

    J Magn Reson Imaging

    (1999)
  • N. Cowan et al.

    Memory-search and rehearsal processes and the word length effect in immediate recall: a synthesis in reply to Service

    Q J Exp Psychol A

    (2000)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text