Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 58, Issue 3, 1 October 2011, Pages 935-945
NeuroImage

Social anhedonia is associated with neural abnormalities during face emotion processing

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.059Get rights and content

Abstract

Human beings are social organisms with an intrinsic desire to seek and participate in social interactions. Social anhedonia is a personality trait characterized by a reduced desire for social affiliation and reduced pleasure derived from interpersonal interactions. Abnormally high levels of social anhedonia prospectively predict the development of schizophrenia and contribute to poorer outcomes for schizophrenia patients. Despite the strong association between social anhedonia and schizophrenia, the neural mechanisms that underlie individual differences in social anhedonia have not been studied and are thus poorly understood. Deficits in face emotion recognition are related to poorer social outcomes in schizophrenia, and it has been suggested that face emotion recognition deficits may be a behavioral marker for schizophrenia liability. In the current study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to see whether there are differences in the brain networks underlying basic face emotion processing in a community sample of individuals low vs. high in social anhedonia. We isolated the neural mechanisms related to face emotion processing by comparing face emotion discrimination with four other baseline conditions (identity discrimination of emotional faces, identity discrimination of neutral faces, object discrimination, and pattern discrimination). Results showed a group (high/low social anhedonia) × condition (emotion discrimination/control condition) interaction in the anterior portion of the rostral medial prefrontal cortex, right superior temporal gyrus, and left somatosensory cortex. As predicted, high (relative to low) social anhedonia participants showed less neural activity in face emotion processing regions during emotion discrimination as compared to each control condition. The findings suggest that social anhedonia is associated with abnormalities in networks responsible for basic processes associated with social cognition, and provide a starting point for understanding the neural basis of social motivation and our drive to seek social affiliation.

Highlights

► Social anhedonia is linked to abnormal BOLD responses during emotion recognition. ► The most consistent abnormalities were in anterior medial prefrontal cortex. ► Superior temporal and left somatosensory cortex also showed abnormal responses. ► Social approach motivation is linked to differences in emotion perception regions.

Introduction

As fundamentally social creatures, humans are driven by the desire for meaningful and frequent social interaction (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). There are individual differences in the strength of this desire, however, and some individuals exhibit a significantly reduced drive for social affiliation known as social anhedonia (Brown et al., 2007, Kwapil, 1998, Kwapil et al., 2009). Social anhedonia (SA) has been characterized as a deficiency in the need to belong to a social group and is distinct from other constructs that might also predict abnormalities in social interaction such as social anxiety (Brown et al., 2007, Kwapil et al., 2009). Individuals high in SA exhibit a genuine preference for solitude, disengagement during social interactions (Brown et al., 2007), and reduced negative affect when alone (Kwapil et al., 2009). Higher levels of SA are related to lower levels of social support and social functioning (Blanchard et al., 2011). Reduced social support and smaller social networks are associated with differences in immune functioning and other clinically significant health outcomes (Miller et al., 2009). Furthermore, high SA has been identified as one of the single most predictive traits for future onset of schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Kwapil, 1998) and has long been recognized as a core attribute of psychosis vulnerability (Bleuler, 1950, Horan et al., 2007, Kraepelin and Gosline, 1919, Meehl, 1962, Rado, 1953, Stone et al., 2005). Altogether, existing evidence indicates that SA is a deviation in a psychologically and clinically important social and emotional process that has broad implications for our understanding of normal and abnormal functioning.

Despite evidence for serious physical and mental health difficulties associated with reduced desire for social affiliation, no research to our knowledge has been done exploring the neural basis of SA in nonclinical populations. In schizophrenia, SA is considered a negative symptom that is stable (Blanchard et al., 2001) and can be reliably assessed (Horan et al., 2006). Studies of SA in schizophrenia have indicated that a number of neural systems may be involved in reduced desire for social affiliation, including the amygdala (Becerril and Barch, 2010), caudate nucleus (Dowd and Barch, 2010), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Becerril and Barch, 2010), and somatosensory areas (Arnfred and Chen, 2004). However, disease-related confounds and secondary characteristics of schizophrenia illness, such as psychosocial stress and neurodegenerative processes, make it difficult to generalize these findings to SA among healthy individuals or to identify whether neural abnormalities associated with SA are associated with the psychosis vulnerability (Lenzenweger, 2006).

Differences in the neural processing of face emotion provide a potential starting point for identifying abnormalities associated with high SA. Accurate face emotion recognition is critical for recognizing and responding to other's mental states and is a building block to more complex social behaviors (Adolphs, 2002). Importantly, face emotion recognition ability (but not face identity processing ability) predicts social functioning in schizophrenia participants (Hooker and Park, 2002) and varies with psychometric psychosis-proneness in nonclinical populations (Germine and Hooker, 2011). Previous work has also shown that face emotion perception is abnormal in individuals high in social anhedonia (Luh and Gooding, 1999). Thus, individual differences in social anhedonia may be related to deficits in the neural mechanisms supporting face emotion recognition.

The neural substrates of face emotion recognition are well characterized in healthy and clinical populations. Previous work indicates that effective emotion recognition involves the recruitment of a network of regions, including the amygdala (Adolphs, 2002, Adolphs et al., 1994, Anderson and Phelps, 2001), superior temporal sulcus (Allison et al., 2000, Haxby et al., 2000), medial prefrontal cortex (Amodio and Frith, 2006, Blair et al., 1999, Dolan et al., 1996, Gorno-Tempini et al., 2001, Gur et al., 2002a, Phillips et al., 1998, Sprengelmeyer et al., 1998, Wright et al., 2002), and somatosensory-related cortices (including insula, S1, S2, and anterior supramarginal gyrus) (Adolphs, 2002, Adolphs et al., 2000). Using functional neuroimaging, researchers have consistently found abnormalities in these regions during emotion recognition in individuals with schizophrenia (Das et al., 2007, Farrer et al., 2004, Gur et al., 2002b, Gur et al., 2007, Hall et al., 2004, Hempel et al., 2003, Holt et al., 2006, Kosaka et al., 2002, Phillips et al., 1999, Pinkham et al., 2008, Schneider et al., 1998, Spence et al., 1997, Taylor et al., 2002, Waberski et al., 2004, Williams et al., 2004).

Deficits in emotion recognition have been associated with lesions to the amygdala (Adolphs et al., 1994), somatosensory and related cortices (Adolphs et al., 2000) and medial prefrontal cortex (Heberlein et al., 2008). The medial prefrontal cortex, in particular, likely plays a broad role in many social-cognitive processes and has been implicated in lower-level emotion perception as well as higher-level processes including theory of mind attributions (Gallagher et al., 2000), self-referential processing (Mitchell et al., 2005), and distinguishing between self and other (Heatherton et al., 2006, Ochsner et al., 2004). In terms of functional divisions, the anterior portion of rostral medial prefrontal cortex (arMFC) has been consistently identified in measures of social cognition and emotion processing (Amodio and Frith, 2006) and in social cognition in schizophrenia (Brunet-Gouet and Decety, 2006).

In the present study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine differences in the neural circuitry underlying face emotion discrimination in otherwise normal individuals who were high versus low in SA. As our face emotion recognition task, we used the Queen Square Face Discrimination Test (QFDT; Garrido et al., 2009). Our primary hypothesis was that high SA would have specific deficits in face emotion processing even when controlling for broader, but equally complex aspects of face perception. The QFDT was chosen because it can dissociate face emotion processing and face identity processing (Banissy et al., 2011, Garrido et al., 2009, Germine and Hooker, 2011, Pitcher et al., 2008). In the QFDT, participants view sequentially presented emotional faces; in one condition they judge whether the two faces are expressing the same emotion, and in another condition they judge whether the two faces have the same identity. Importantly, the two conditions have identical stimuli and are equally difficult for healthy participants. As a result, any differences found between emotion discrimination and identity discrimination can be attributed to differences in specific cognitive processes related to emotion perception and cannot be attributed to differences in the stimuli, number of response options, or difficulty level of the two conditions. This feature of the task is an improvement over face processing studies where the experimental and control tasks differ along these dimensions (e.g. labeling emotional faces using four options vs. same/different identity of paired neutral faces). In the QFDT, the emotion recognition and identity recognition conditions use the same task structure (both are a forced choice same/different judgment) and the same stimuli. Therefore the comparison of emotion recognition and identity recognition of emotional faces isolates the specific cognitive processes for attending to, processing, and judging face emotions. Using a behavioral version of this task, we found that higher levels of psychosis risk (based on self-report of cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized psychosis-prone characteristics) are associated with reduced emotion discrimination performance, but normal identity discrimination performance (Germine and Hooker, 2011). The QFDT was also used by Pitcher et al. (2008), who found that applying transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to the face area of somatosensory cortex impaired performance in the emotion discrimination condition, but not the identity discrimination condition. Thus, we have good reason to believe that the QFDT emotion discrimination condition depends on one or more processes specific to emotion processing that also vary with psychosis vulnerability. The current fMRI study included three additional control conditions designed to reveal potential group differences in the broader face emotion processing neural network. These conditions included identity discrimination of neutral faces, visual discrimination of objects, and visual discrimination of patterns. Given the putative relationship between SA and vulnerability for psychosis (Kwapil, 1998), we predicted that individuals high in SA would exhibit reduced recruitment of cortical regions involved in face emotion recognition, particularly superior temporal sulcus/gyrus, medial prefrontal cortex and somatosensory-related parietal regions, as well as reduced responses in the amygdala. Between group differences in one or more of these regions would indicate that higher levels of SA are associated with neural abnormalities during emotion perception, and help us better understand the neural basis of differences in the desire for social affiliation as well as psychosis vulnerability.

Section snippets

Participants

We recruited a community-based sample comprised of thirty participants who were high or low in social anhedonia based on their scores on the Revised Chapman Social Anhedonia Scale (RSAS; Chapman and Chapman, 1980). Fifteen high social anhedonia participants (high SA) were selected based on scoring in the top 10% on this measure (RSAS score > 16 for females, > 19 for males). Fifteen low social anhedonia participants (low SA) were selected based on having scores at or below the mean (RSAS score < 7

Behavioral

No significant between group differences were found in performance on any condition (EE, EI, NI, OD, and PD) (all p > 0.2). Details of performance in each condition for each group are shown in Fig. 1.

Effects of task across all participants

We conducted one sample t-tests on EE (vs. baseline) contrasts across all participants to verify that the EE task was inducing BOLD signal changes in the expected face emotion and face processing regions. This analysis verified task-related activity in a network of regions that included the right

Discussion

Although social anhedonia has long been recognized as a key feature of schizophrenia illness and liability, there is surprisingly little known about its underlying neural substrates. In this study we investigated whether otherwise healthy individuals with high social anhedonia (SA) had deficient neural activity during face emotion discrimination — a social cognitive process associated with robust behavioral and neural deficits in schizophrenia. The results show that people with high SA have

Conclusion

The wide range of physical and mental health outcomes arising from differences in social affiliation and social support argues that the experience of pleasure that accompanies social interaction is a vital component of a functioning social cognitive system (Brown et al., 2007, Kwapil et al., 2009) with broad and meaningful health consequences. Social impairments and low levels of social affiliation are related to increased risk of mental illness (Hooley, 2010), as well as differences in immune

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Taylor Benson and Roger Mercado for help with data collection and Tom Zeffiro for assistance with data analysis. We would also like to thank Yaoda Xu for assistance with initial experimental design. This research was supported by Harvard University research funds to Christine Hooker and a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship to Laura Germine.

References (94)

  • J.E. Downhill et al.

    Temporal lobe volume determined by magnetic resonance imaging in schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2001)
  • J. Edwards et al.

    Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: a methodological review

    Clin. Psychol. Rev.

    (2002)
  • C. Farrer et al.

    Neural correlates of action attribution in schizophrenia

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2004)
  • H. Gallagher et al.

    Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks

    Neuropsychologia

    (2000)
  • M. Gorno-Tempini et al.

    Explicit and incidental facial expression processing: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2001)
  • R. Gur et al.

    Brain activation during facial emotion processing

    Neuroimage

    (2002)
  • J.V. Haxby et al.

    The distributed human neural system for face perception

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2000)
  • A. Hempel et al.

    Impairment in basal limbic function in schizophrenia during affect recognition

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2003)
  • D.J. Holt et al.

    Increased medial temporal lobe activation during the passive viewing of emotional and neutral facial expressions in schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2006)
  • C. Hooker et al.

    Emotion processing and its relationship to social functioning in schizophrenia patients

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2002)
  • C. Hooker et al.

    Brain networks for analyzing eye gaze

    Brain Res.

    (2003)
  • C. Hooker et al.

    Neural activity during social signal perception correlates with self-reported empathy

    Brain Res.

    (2010)
  • J. Hooley et al.

    Pain insensitivity in the relatives of schizophrenia patients

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2001)
  • W.P. Horan et al.

    Social anhedonia and schizotypy: the contribution of individual differences in affective traits, stress, and coping

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2007)
  • T.R. Kwapil et al.

    The social world of the socially anhedonic: exploring the daily ecology of asociality

    J. Res. Pers.

    (2009)
  • C.M. Loughland et al.

    Visual scanpaths to positive and negative facial emotions in an outpatient schizophrenia sample

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2002)
  • J.A. Maldjian et al.

    An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • J.A. Maldjian et al.

    Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the Talairach atlas

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • M.L. Phillips et al.

    Investigation of facial recognition memory and happy and sad facial expression perception: an fMRI study

    Psychiatry Res.

    (1998)
  • M.L. Phillips et al.

    A differential neural response to threatening and non-threatening negative facial expressions in paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenics

    Psychiatry Res.

    (1999)
  • F. Schneider et al.

    Differential amygdala activation in schizophrenia during sadness

    Schizophr. Res.

    (1998)
  • S.F. Taylor et al.

    A functional anatomic study of emotion in schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2002)
  • P. Vuilleumier et al.

    Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study

    Neuron

    (2001)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala

    Nature

    (1994)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping

    J. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • D.M. Amodio et al.

    Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • A.K. Anderson et al.

    Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events

    Nature

    (2001)
  • S.M. Arnfred et al.

    Exploration of somatosensory P50 gating in schizophrenia spectrum patients: reduced P50 amplitude correlates to social anhedonia

    Psychiatry Res.

    (2004)
  • M.J. Banissy et al.

    Superior facial expression, but not identity recognition, in mirror-touch synesthesia

    J. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • R. Baumeister et al.

    The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1995)
  • K. Becerril et al.

    Influence of emotional processing on working memory in schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (2010)
  • R.J.R. Blair et al.

    Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger

    Brain

    (1999)
  • J. Blanchard et al.

    Diagnostic differences in social anhedonia: a longitudinal study of schizophrenia and major depressive disorder

    J. Abnorm. Psychol.

    (2001)
  • J. Blanchard et al.

    Social anhedonia and schizotypy in a community sample: the Maryland Longitudinal Study of Schizotypy

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (2011)
  • E. Bleuler

    Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias

    (1950)
  • L. Brown et al.

    When the need to belong goes wrong: the expression of social anhedonia and social anxiety in daily life

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2007)
  • B.P. Chang et al.

    Somatosensory processing in the biological relatives of schizophrenia patients: a signal detection analysis of two-point discrimination

    J. Abnorm. Psychol.

    (2001)
  • Cited by (86)

    • Associations between different facets of anhedonia and neural response to monetary, social, and food reward in emerging adults

      2022, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Individuals who experience diminished pleasure from social interactions tend to have fewer relationships (Brown et al., 2007) and show poorer social functioning (Assaad & Lemay, 2018; Llerena et al., 2012). Prior work suggests that the magnitude of one’s neural response to social reward can predict changes in social behaviour (King-Casas et al., 2005; Weinberg et al., 2021), and blunted neural responses to social stimuli, as well as altered activity in neural regions that process social information, are associated with social anhedonia (Barkus & Badcock, 2019; Germine et al., 2011; Healey, Morgan et al., 2014) and heightened internalizing symptoms (Lau et al., 2012; Silk et al., 2014). Food, a primary reward that is innately rewarding and essential for survival (Delgado et al., 2006; Hull, 1943; Matyjek et al., 2020; Skinner, 1938), can also meaningfully alter affect and behaviour.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text