Action comprehension in aphasia: linguistic and non-linguistic deficits and their lesion correlates

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.016Get rights and content

Abstract

We tested aphasic patients’ comprehension of actions to examine processing deficits in the linguistic and non-linguistic domains and their lesion correlates. Twenty-nine left-hemisphere injured patients and 18 age-matched control subjects matched pictured actions (with the objects missing) or their linguistic equivalents (printed sentences with the object missing) to one of two visually-presented pictures of objects. Aphasic patients performed poorly not only in the linguistic domain but also in the non-linguistic domain. A subset of the patients, largely consisting of severe and non-fluent aphasics, showed a greater deficit in the linguistic domain compared with the non-linguistic domain and across the patient group, deficits in the linguistic and non-linguistic domains were not tightly correlated. Poor performance in pantomime interpretation was associated with lesions in the inferior frontal, premotor and motor cortex, a portion of somatosensory cortex, and the caudate, while poor reading comprehension of actions was associated with lesions around the anterior superior temporal lobe, the anterior insula and the anterior portion of the inferior parietal lobe. Lesion size did not correlate with deficits. The lesion results for pantomime interpretation deficits demonstrate that lesions in the frontal component of the human analog of the “mirror neuron system” are associated with deficits in non-linguistic action understanding. For reading comprehension deficits, the lesion correlates are brain areas known to be involved in linguistic tasks including sentence processing and speech articulation; the parietal lesion site may also correspond to a subpart of the human mirror neuron system. These results indicate that brain areas important for the production of language and action are also recruited in their comprehension. Similar findings have been reported in electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies. Our findings now also lend neuropsychological support to an embodied view of brain organization for action processing.

Introduction

While aphasia is primarily characterized by disturbance of language functions following brain injury, patients have been observed to also exhibit impairments in nonverbal domains, revealed by tasks such as associating pictures with corresponding objects (De Renzi, Pieczuro, & Vignolo, 1968), colors with pictures (De Renzi, Faglioni, Scotti, & Spinnler, 1972), and environmental sounds with pictures (e.g., Saygin et al., 2003a, Spinnler and Vignolo, 1966). In particular, aphasic patients’ deficits in using and recognizing signs, gestures and pantomime have been examined in numerous studies (e.g., Bell, 1994; Duffy & Duffy, 1981; Gainotti & Lemmo, 1976; Goodglass & Kaplan, 1963; Pickett, 1974; Varney, 1978; Wang & Goodglass, 1992).

In the present study, we examined aphasic patients’ comprehension of visually presented action stimuli in both linguistic and non-linguistic domains. We used a variant of a classical neuropsychological paradigm commonly used to test comprehension in aphasic patients: an object selection task. We had a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) design and asked patients to choose the object that best matched visually presented stimuli containing action information. In both the linguistic and the non-linguistic domains, the associated objects upon which the actions should be carried out were removed from the stimuli; thus subjects matched either a sentence missing its object (such as “he is licking the …”), or a picture missing its object (such as a picture of a boy licking an invisible ice-cream cone) to the corresponding object (in this case, the ice-cream cone). We used static black and white drawings of pantomimed actions in our design (like Seron, van der Kaa, Remitz, & van der Linden, 1979) because they are more appropriate visual matches to written text stimuli which are also static in nature.

We had two main goals in this study: (1) to test linguistic and non-linguistic action comprehension at the same time, using the same task, on the same patients, and with stimuli as closely matched as possible, (2) to conduct lesion-symptom mapping analyses using voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM; Bates et al., 2003b) to identify the lesion correlates of action comprehension deficits.

Regarding the first goal, relationships between linguistic and non-linguistic deficits in aphasic patients are important to examine because they shed light on whether aphasia is a domain-specific disorder which affects only language, or is part of a larger deficit which affects other domains as well. Such questions have been asked since the early days of neurology. Finkelnburg (1870) was the first to propose what is known as the “asymbolia” hypothesis: he suggested that a single underlying factor was common to both the language impairments in aphasia and the deficits in nonverbal domains that these patients exhibit. This idea received some support from subsequent pioneers in neurology as well (e.g., Goldstein, 1948, Head, 1926). On the other hand, it does not seem plausible that aphasia is completely reducible to a strong version of asymbolia, especially given that dissociations in performance between linguistic and non-linguistic domains can be encountered in individual patients.

Even though nonverbal deficits in aphasia have been of interest to researchers for a long time, it has been difficult to assess whether the linguistic and the non-linguistic deficits patients exhibit are related to each other. First, performance on language processing and on non-linguistic tasks must be explored in the same patients. Furthermore, task and stimulus-level factors should be as closely matched as possible. Considerations such as these motivate the first goal of the present study: to contrast linguistic and non-linguistic comprehension of action information in aphasia by comparing performance in the two domains in the same patients more directly than in previous studies. Previous studies seeking correlations between patients’ performance in various language tests and various action comprehension tests do exist, although stimuli and tasks have often not been closely matched across the two domains. While some of these studies found correlations between language impairments and non-linguistic action processing impairments in aphasic patients (e.g., Duffy & Duffy, 1981; Pickett, 1974; Seron et al., 1979, Varney, 1978, Varney, 1982), others found largely uncorrelated performance (e.g., Bell, 1994; Goodglass & Kaplan, 1963; Kimura, 1977).

With regards to the second goal, although it has been known since the early days of neurology that left-hemisphere lesions can often cause receptive and/or expressive disorders in both language and action domains (i.e., aphasic and apraxic disorders) and that patients with right hemisphere injury will rarely exhibit such impairments, the precise lesion sites leading to aphasic and apraxic deficits remain quite unclear. Specifically, results on lesion correlates of impairments in action, pantomime and gesture comprehension deficits are few, and not entirely consistent. Heilman and colleagues have reported that apraxic patients with posterior lesions have more trouble in comprehending the meaning of pantomimes (Heilman et al., 1982, Rothi et al., 1985) and have suggested that posterior parietal regions of the cortex may mediate the production and comprehension of purposeful movements (see also De Renzi et al., 1986, Kertesz et al., 1984). On the other hand, Ferro, Martins, Mariano, and Castro Caldas (1983) reported that while gesture recognition impairments were most commonly associated with parietal lesions in chronic stages of brain damage, in acute stages it was the patients with left frontal and basal ganglia damage who showed deficiencies, but unfortunately this study had a rather small sample size. Other studies failed to find reliable lesion sites associated with deficits (e.g., Schnider, Hanlon, Alexander, & Benson, 1997; Wang & Goodglass, 1992). Recently, Tranel, Kemmerer, Adolphs, Damasio, and Damasio (2003) used more novel lesion-mapping methods and reported that lesions in the left premotor/prefrontal and parietal cortex and in the white matter underlying the left posterior middle temporal cortex are implicated in deficits in tasks which were designed to tap into conceptual knowledge for actions.

There is also a substantial literature on the related question of brain areas differentially involved in the naming of actions versus objects, and/or the processing of verbs versus nouns. Many researchers have argued that left frontal areas are differentially involved in the processing of actions or verbs. For example, in a PET study using a lexical decision task, Perani, Cappa, Schnur, and Tettamanti (1999) found that verbs activated left dorsolateral frontal cortex more than nouns. However, other studies have failed to find significant differences; for instance, Tyler, Russell, Fadili, and Moss (2001), using carefully matched stimuli, did not find any regions differentially involved in the lexical decision or semantic processing of nouns versus verbs. Hillis, Tuffiash, Wityk, and Barker (2002) reported that damage or hypoperfusion in precentral and middle temporal gyri were associated with action naming deficits in patients with acute left hemisphere injury, while for object naming, middle temporal and superior temporal gyri were associated with impairment. However, for comprehension of action and object words, they did not find separate sites; impairments were associated with superior temporal lesions. Hillis et al. suggested in light of this finding that only the naming of actions, rather than semantic knowledge, may be localized to left frontal cortex.

Given the diverse results which have been reported in the literature, we wanted to use VLSM, a quantitative lesion-symptom mapping technique, to contribute to identifying lesion correlates of action comprehension in aphasia in linguistic and non-linguistic domains.

In addition to these two major goals, we had some other points in mind in our design: In line with earlier studies (e.g., Varney, 1978), we also addressed the effect of semantic competition in both domains in order to see if processing in the two domains is similarly modulated by higher-level conceptual constraints. In addition, following Seron et al. (1979), Wang and Goodglass (1992) and Bell (1994), we also used distracters that were related to the targets in the way they may be handled, to see if there are differential effects of this kind of competition (previous researchers termed these morphological, perceptual or motoric distracters; here we refer to these as “affordance-based” distracters; see Gibson, 1977).

Section snippets

Participants

Patients were voluntary participants recruited from the community in San Diego, CA or the VA Northern California Health Care System (VANCHCS) in Martinez, CA, and were paid US$ 25.00 for their participation. Twenty-nine left-hemisphere injured patients with varying types and severity of aphasia participated in the experiment. All aphasic patients were administered the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB; Kertesz, 1979) and were diagnosed as Anomic (N = 9), Broca’s (N = 12), or Wernicke’s aphasics (N =

Results

Here, we report differences in accuracy and reaction time between patient and control groups, the correlation in performance across verbal and nonverbal domains, and the relationship between lesion site and processing deficits.

Action processing impairments in aphasia and their relation to language deficits

Aphasic patients were significantly impaired in our action-to-picture matching task compared with control subjects. Performance was compromised in both linguistic and non-linguistic domains. However, patients tended to show deficits that were more pronounced in the linguistic domain and the severity of aphasia was strongly related to the relative disparity between performance in linguistic and non-linguistic domains. There was no overall correlation between patients’ deficits in the two

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to H. Saygın for help with preparing the picture stimuli, R. Buffington, C. Ludy, S. Moineau, and E. Schleicher for assistance with testing or programming, R.T. Knight for lesion reconstructions, F. Dick, and M. Iacoboni for helpful discussions and all of our subjects for participating in our experiments. We also thank L. Barsalou, G. Goldenberg, the anonymous reviewers, and M. Moscovitch for their suggestions on previous versions of this manuscript, which greatly improved our

References (86)

  • G. Gainotti et al.

    Comprehension of symbolic gestures in aphasia

    Brain and Language

    (1976)
  • G. Goldenberg

    Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres

    Neuropsychologia

    (1999)
  • J. Grèzes et al.

    Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • U. Halsband et al.

    Recognition and imitation of pantomimed motor acts after unilateral parietal and premotor lesions: A perspective on apraxia

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • M. Jeannerod

    Mental imagery in the motor context

    Neuropsychologia

    (1995)
  • M. Jeannerod

    Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism of motor cognition

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • T.A. Keller et al.

    Brain imaging of tongue-twister sentence comprehension: Twisting the tongue and the brain

    Brain and Language

    (2003)
  • C.J. Moore et al.

    Three distinct ventral occipitotemporal regions for reading and object naming

    NeuroImage

    (1999)
  • G. Rizzolatti et al.

    Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (1996)
  • A. Schnider et al.

    Ideomotor apraxia: Behavioral dimensions and neuroanatomical basis

    Brain and Language

    (1997)
  • X. Seron et al.

    Pantomime interpretation and aphasia

    Neuropsychologia

    (1979)
  • H. Spinnler et al.

    Impaired recognition of meaningful sounds in aphasia

    Cortex

    (1966)
  • N.R. Varney

    Pantomime recognition defect in aphasia: Implications for the concept of asymbolia

    Brain and Language

    (1982)
  • N.R. Varney et al.

    Qualitative aspects of pantomime recognition defect in aphasia

    Brain and Cognition

    (1982)
  • L. Wang et al.

    Pantomime, praxis, and aphasia

    Brain and Language

    (1992)
  • L.W. Barsalou

    Perceptual symbol systems

    The Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1999)
  • E. Bates et al.

    Quantifying dissociations in neuropsychological research

    Journal of Experimental and Clinical Neuropsychology

    (2003)
  • E. Bates et al.

    Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • S.C. Blank et al.

    Speech production: Wernicke, Broca and beyond

    Brain

    (2002)
  • L.R. Caplan et al.

    Caudate infarcts

    Archives of Neurology

    (1990)
  • J.D. Cohen et al.

    PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers

    Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers

    (1993)
  • M. Coltheart et al.

    Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches

    Psychological Review

    (1993)
  • Cummings, A., Saygin, A. P., Dick, F., & Bates, E. (2004). Is there a dissociation between verbal and environmental...
  • E. De Renzi et al.

    Limb apraxia in patients with damage confined to the left basal ganglia and thalamus

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (1986)
  • E. De Renzi et al.

    Impairment in associating colour to form, concomitant with aphasia

    Brain

    (1972)
  • DeArmond, S. J., Fusco, M. M., & Dewey, M. M. (1976). Structure of the human brain: A photographic atlas. New York:...
  • N.F. Dronkers

    A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation

    Nature

    (1996)
  • R.J. Duffy et al.

    Three studies of deficits in pantomimic expression and pantomime recognition in aphasia

    Journal of Speech and Hearing Research

    (1981)
  • L. Fadiga et al.

    Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1995)
  • J.M. Ferro et al.

    CT scan correlates of gesture recognition

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (1983)
  • Finkelnburg, D. C. (1870). [Niederrheinische Gesellschaft in Bonn, Sitzung vom 21. März 1870.] Berliner Klinische...
  • N. Fujimaki et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging of neural activity related to orthographic, phonological, and lexico-semantic judgments of visually presented characters and words

    Human Brain Mapping

    (1999)
  • V. Gallese et al.

    Action recognition in the premotor cortex

    Brain

    (1996)
  • Cited by (128)

    • Anterior insula as a gatekeeper of executive control

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • What modulates the Mirror Neuron System during action observation?: Multiple factors involving the action, the actor, the observer, the relationship between actor and observer, and the context

      2021, Progress in Neurobiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Another fMRI study found that when subjects judged sequences of three static body postures, responses in primary and supplementary motor areas were greater when those sequences were natural and fluent (ABC) than when they were unnatural and nonfluent (ACB) (Orgs et al., 2016). In addition, a number of neuropsychological and TMS studies have shown that real or virtual lesions to left inferior frontal, premotor, primary motor, and/or inferior parietal regions impair performance on nonlinguistic tasks that require judgments about static action stimuli (Tranel et al., 2003; Bak and Hodges, 2004; Saygin et al., 2004; Hillis et al., 2006; Urgesi et al., 2007; Moro et al., 2008; Fazio et al., 2009; Kemmerer et al., 2012). Furthermore, a TMS study showed that, compared with static images of either a relaxed hand or a hand at the endpoint of an action, static images of a hand executing a thumb-forefinger pincer grip increased the excitability of the muscles that would perform that action; those muscles were not affected, however, by static images of nonbiological entities with (e.g., waterfalls) or without (e.g., icefalls) implied motion (Urgesi et al., 2006; for follow-up studies see Urgesi et al., 2010, and Mattiassi et al., 2014; and for contrary data see Bunday et al., 2016).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Elizabeth Bates, Professor of Cognitive Science at the University of California, San Diego, passed away December 13, 2003.

    View full text