Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 50, Issue 14, December 2012, Pages 3554-3563
Neuropsychologia

Research Report
The Met-genotype of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is associated with reduced Stroop interference in elderly

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.042Get rights and content

Abstract

Aging is accompanied by impairments of executive functions that rely on the functional integrity of fronto-striatal networks. This integrity is modulated by the release of neurotrophins like the brain-derived-neurotrophic factor (BDNF). Here, we investigate effects of the functional BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on interference processing in 131 healthy elderly subjects using event-related potentials (ERPs). In a Stroop task, participants had to indicate the name or the colour of colour-words while colour was either compatible or incompatible with the name. We show that susceptibility to Stroop-interference is affected by the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism: the Met-allele carriers showed better performance and enhanced N450 in interference trials. Other processes necessary to prepare and allocate cognitive resources to a particular task were not affected by BDNF Val66Met polymorphism, underlining the specificity of the observed effects. The observed performance and ERP difference is possibly due to dopamine related effects of BDNF in fronto-striatal networks, where it putatively mediates a shift in the balance of the direct and indirect pathway involved in inhibitory functions.

Highlights

► Met-allele of the Val66Met polymorphism is related to lower Stroop interference in elderly. ► Lower Stroop susceptibility was associated with larger centro-parietal N450 in incompatible trials. ► Preparatory processes and several neuropsychological parameters were not affected by the BDNF Val66Met genotype. ► Enhanced inhibitory control in the Met-genotype is discussed within the framework of fronto-striatal networks.

Introduction

Molecular genetic techniques are increasingly recognized as important tools to elucidate neurobiological determinants of human cognitive function (van Thriel et al., 2012). They may also prove useful to understand the large inter-individual differences in cognitive functions in aging (Harris and Deary, 2011, Hultsch and MacDonald, 2004, Lindenberger et al., 2008). Particularly, executive functions relying on the functional integrity of prefrontal networks are most affected in aging (MacDonald, Nyberg, & Bäckman, 2006). This neuronal integrity is modulated by neurotrophins like the brain-derived-neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which regulates survival, growth, maintenance and genesis of neurons (McAllister et al., 1999, Pezawas et al., 2004). BDNF also regulates activity-dependent changes in synaptic plasticity, such as long-term potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampus (Bekinschtein et al., 2008, Egan et al., 2003). A common single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the human BDNF gene (Val66Met; rs6265) leads to a valine (Val)/methionine (Met) substitution in the pro-domain of the encoded protein (Chen et al., 2004). The functional polymorphism alters the intracellular tracking and packaging of pro-BDNF, affecting the secretion of mature peptide (Egan et al., 2003). As compared to the Met-allele, the Val-allele is associated with higher activity of the BDNF system (e.g., Rybakowski, 2008), which linearly relates to increased neural activity (Kafitz, Rose, Thoenen, & Konnerth, 1999).

Association studies on the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism have revealed contradictory effects on cognitive functions, which may be also due to the variability of BDNF isoforms and the diversity of transcripts in different brain areas (Mandelman & Grigorenko, 2012). Specific memory functions are compromised in Met-allele carriers (episodic memory: Dempster et al., 2005, Egan et al., 2003, Li et al., 2010, declarative memory: Hariri et al., 2003) see also Miyajima et al., 2008, Raz et al., 2009, but a number of studies found no association between BDNF and memory performance in different populations (Houlihan et al., 2009, Nacmias et al., 2004, Strauss et al., 2004, van Wingen et al., 2010; see also Mandelman and Grigorenko, 2012, Payton, 2009, for an overview). While Mandelman and Grigorenko (2012) did also not detect a consistent association between BDNF and executive control processes, several recent studies showed that some executive functions are more efficient in Met-allele carriers (Beste et al., 2010a, Beste et al., 2010b) and particularly so in older subjects (Erickson et al., 2008, Foltynie et al., 2005, Gajewski et al., 2011, Harris et al., 2006, Matsushita et al., 2005, Ventriglia et al., 2002). In elderly individuals, the relevance of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism as a modulator of cognitive functions is even more salient than in young subjects, since cognitive processes move away from their optimum (Lindenberger et al., 2008, Nagel et al., 2008). Moreover, it has been shown that BDNF secretion is decreased in aging (Hayashi et al., 2001, Pang and Lu, 2004).

We aimed at providing a further test for the hypothesis that the Met allele of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is associated with enhanced executive control processes in healthy elderly. We do so by means of event-related potentials (ERPs) in a healthy elderly cohort using a computerised version of the Stroop paradigm. Processes related to interference control have repeatedly been shown to depend on anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal areas (Beste et al., 2010c, Beste et al., 2010e, Liotti et al., 2000, West, 2003). These areas are part of fronto-striatal loops (Chudasama and Robbins, 2006, Foltynie et al., 2005) known to play a crucial role in executive functions like response inhibition and response selection (Beste et al., 2012, Gajewski et al., 2008, Gajewski et al., 2011, Mostovsky and Simmonds, 2008, Paus, 2001, Picard and Strick, 1996, Turken and Swick, 1999). Inhibitory processes have generally been suggested to be crucial for interference control (Friedman and Miyake, 2004, Kok, 1999, Zysset et al., 2001) and also for processing of incompatible Stroop responses (i.e., when the dominant task dimension (word reading) has to be suppressed) (Cohen et al., 1990, Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, 1992, Stroop, 1935, West and Alain, 2000).

Using ERPs it has been shown that incompatible Stroop stimuli evoke a centro-parietal negativity about 450 ms post-stimulus (Mager et al., 2007, Rebai et al., 1997, West, 2003, West and Alain, 1999, West and Alain, 2000, West et al., 2005), which is most likely generated by areas in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Liotti et al., 2000, Markela-Lerenc et al., 2004) with a posterior shift for manual responses (Atkinson et al., 2003, Liotti et al., 2000, West, 2004) which suggests different sources within the ACC. The N450 reflects response, but not stimulus interference (Chen, Bailey, Tiernan, & West, 2011) and has consistently been associated with response conflict interference control induced by incompatible Stroop stimuli (Coderre et al., 2011, Liotti et al., 2000, West et al., 2005, West, 2004). Paralleling the increase of Stroop interference in elderly, this negativity has been shown to be attenuated in aging (Mager et al., 2007, West, 2004, West and Alain, 2000). This suggests that an attenuated negativity in incompatible trials is related to compromised interference control.

If the Met genotype confers a benefit to its carriers in executive control processes, we expect Met-allele carriers to show weaker interference effects on a behavioural level, as compared to Val/Val-allele carriers. This may be expressed in shorter reaction times (RTs), lower intra-individual variability of speed and lower error rates in Met-allele carriers, compared to Val/Val genotype carriers (cf. Gajewski et al., 2011; Getzmann et al. in press). On a neurophysiological level we expect that the central negativity, the N450, indicating successful interference control is attenuated in Val/Val genotype carriers, compared to Met-allele carriers. Moreover, in tasks imposing high demands on cognitive control the allocation of processing resources is essential (Freude & Ullsperger, 2000). These preparatory processes are reflected in the contingent negative variation (CNV; Falkenstein et al., 2003, Walter et al., 1964). As such, possible differences between elderly Met-allele and Val/Val genotype carriers in interference control may only be a side effect of more efficient preparatory processes that allocate processing resources to the task-relevant cognitive process. If this is the case, the CNV should be enhanced in elderly Met-allele carriers, compared to Val/Val genotype carriers, and particularly so in conditions with higher conflict.

Section snippets

Participants

One hundred thirty one healthy volunteers aged from 65 to 88 (M=70.5, SD=4.5) participated in the study. 81 (61.8%) of them were female. Six participants of the Val/Val and five participants of the Val/Met group were left handed or ambidextrous. They had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants received payment for their participation. The sample consisted of 79 subjects carrying the Val/Val genotype, 47 carrying the Val/Met genotype and five subjects carrying the Met/Met genotype

Neuropsychological data

The sample was examined using neuropsychological and psychiatric tests. The results of these tests are given in Table 1. The Val/Val vs. Val/Met–Met/Met subgroups did not significantly differ regarding a number of neuropsychological, psychiatric and personality parameters (Table 1).

Behavioural data

Mean reaction times (RTs) and error rates (ERs) for compatible and incompatible trials in the word reading and colour naming block are presented in Fig. 1.

Generally, for the analysis of response times, error trials

Discussion

We investigated the role of the functional BDNF Val66Met polymorphism for interference control in healthy old individuals. The behavioural data of the Stroop test shows a generally increased level of RTs, increased RTs in the interference prone block and a trend for higher intraindividual variability of speed in Val/Val genotype carriers, compared to Met-allele carriers. Moreover, error rates increased in Val/Val genotype carriers and hence performance was impaired in incompatible Stroop trials

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the German Insurance Association (GDV, Gesamtverband der Deutschen Versicherungswirtschaft) to M.F. and P.G. and by a Grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) BE 4045/10-1 to C.B.

We thank Ludger Blanke, Christiane Westedt, Brita Rietdorf, Claudia Wipking, Kirsten Liesenhoff-Henze and Marion Page for excellent technical and organizational assistance, Dr. Meinolf Blaszkewicz and Marie-Louise Lehmann (IfADo) for helpful discussion and advise and

References (87)

  • M.F. Egan et al.

    The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function

    Cell

    (2003)
  • M.F. Folstein et al.

    Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician

    Journal of Psychiatry Research

    (1975)
  • P.D. Gajewski et al.

    The Met-allele of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism enhances task switching in elderly

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2011)
  • P.D. Gajewski et al.

    ERP—correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm

    Brain Research

    (2008)
  • J.T. Gale et al.

    From symphony to cacophony: pathophysiology of the human basal ganglia in Parkinson’s disease

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews

    (2008)
  • H. Garavan et al.

    Individual differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory control

    Brain Research

    (2006)
  • G. Gratton et al.

    A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1983)
  • S.E. Harris et al.

    The genetics of cognitive ability and cognitive ageing in healthy older people

    Trends in Cognitive Science

    (2011)
  • M. Hayashi et al.

    Changes in BDNF-immunoreactive structures in the hippocampal formation of the aged macaque monkey

    Brain Research

    (2001)
  • A. Kok

    Varieties of Inhibition: manifestations in cognition, event-related potentials and aging

    Acta Psychologica

    (1999)
  • M. Liotti et al.

    An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop colour-word interference effect

    Neuropsychologia

    (2000)
  • S.W.S MacDonald et al.

    Intraindividual variability in behavior: links to brain structure, neurotransmission, and neuronal activity

    Trends in Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • R. Mager et al.

    Age-related changes in cognitive conflict processing: an event-related potential study

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2007)
  • J. Markela-Lerenc et al.

    Prefrontal-cingulate activation during executive control: which comes first?

    Brain Research, Cognitive Brain Research

    (2004)
  • B. Nacmias et al.

    Brain-derived neurotrophic factor, apolipoprotein E genetic variants and cognitive performance in Alzheimer’s disease

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2004)
  • P.T. Pang et al.

    Regulation of late-phase LTP and long-term memory in normal and aging hippocampus: role of secreted proteins tPA and BDN

    Ageing Research Reviews

    (2004)
  • C. van Thriel et al.

    Translating neurobehavioural endpoints of developmental neurotoxicity tests into in vitro assays and readouts

    Neurotoxicology

    (2012)
  • G. van Wingen et al.

    The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism affects memory formation and retrieval of biologically salient stimuli

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • R. West

    Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • R. West et al.

    Event-related neural activity associated with the Stroop task

    Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research

    (1999)
  • S. Zysset et al.

    Colour-word matching Stroop task: separating interference and response conflict

    Neuroimage

    (2001)
  • A.R. Aron et al.

    Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • Aschenbrenner, S. Tucha. O., & Lange, K.W. (2001). Der Regensburger Wortrflüssigkeits-Test. Göttingen:...
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    An inventory for measuring depression

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1961)
  • P. Bekinschtein et al.

    BDNF is essential to promote persistence of long-term memory storage

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2008)
  • C. Beste et al.

    The role of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism for the synchronzation of error-specific neural networks

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2010)
  • C. Beste et al.

    Effects of stimulus–response compatibility on inhibitory processes in Parkinson’s disease

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2009)
  • R. Brickenkamp

    Test d2, Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test

    (1994)
  • D.E. Broadbent et al.

    The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates

    British Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (1982)
  • Z.Y. Chen et al.

    Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Met66) alters the intracellular trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of wild-type BDNF in neurosecretory cells and cortical neurons

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • J.D Cohen et al.

    On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect

    Psychological Review

    (1990)
  • J.D. Cohen et al.

    Context, cortex and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia

    Psychological Review

    (1992)
  • P.T. Costa et al.

    Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual

    (1992)
  • Cited by (39)

    • The moderating effect of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on inhibitory control in elderly individuals

      2021, Assessments, Treatments and Modeling in Aging and Neurological Disease: The Neuroscience of Aging
    • Sex-dependent effect of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on executive functioning and processing speed in older adults: evidence from the health ABC study

      2019, Neurobiology of Aging
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, in contrast to these findings, other studies have failed to find a significant association between the Met allele and cognitive decline in participants at risk for AD (Honea et al., 2013; Lim et al., 2013; Weinstein et al., 2014). Interestingly, studies have even found the opposite, with greater cognitive performance in Met carriers compared with Val/Val carriers in different populations, including patients with Parkinson's disease, AD, and mild cognitive impairment, as well as in cognitively healthy older adults (Erickson et al., 2008; Feher et al., 2009; Gajewski et al., 2012, 2011; Harris et al., 2006; Nagata et al., 2012; van der Kolk et al., 2015). These conflicting results may be because of several reasons, including the specific cognitive domain under investigation as many of the original studies have focused on memory-based tasks and there is suggestion that executive functions may show a greater association with this polymorphism (Mandelman and Grigorenko, 2012), the age of participants, and differences in cognitive status between populations.

    • Methamphetamine-associated difficulties in cognitive control allocation may normalize after prolonged abstinence

      2019, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      When investigating cognitive control, the fronto-central N2 and N450 components are the potentially most interesting ERPs. They reflect conflict detection and monitoring, which is crucial for goal-directed response adaptation and thus for cognitive control (Beste et al. 2010; Gajewski et al. 2012; Larson et al. 2014). Larger amplitudes usually reflect greater conflict monitoring, which should allow for greater adaptation to perceived task difficulties and control requirements (Larson et al. 2014).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text