Neural representations during sleep: From sensory processing to memory traces

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2006.10.006Get rights and content

Abstract

In the course of a day, the brain undergoes large-scale changes in functional modes, from attentive wakefulness to the deepest stage of sleep. The present paper evaluates how these state changes affect the neural bases of sensory and cognitive representations. Are organized neural representations still maintained during sleep? In other words, despite the absence of conscious awareness, do neuronal signals emitted during sleep contain information and have a functional relevance? Through a critical evaluation of the animal and human literature, neural representations at different levels of integration (from the most elementary sensory level to the most cognitive one) are reviewed. Recordings of neuronal activity in animals at presentation of neutral or significant stimuli show that some analysis of the external word remains possible during sleep, allowing recognition of behaviorally relevant stimuli. Event-related brain potentials in humans confirm the preservation of some sensory integration and discriminative capacity. Behavioral and neuroimaging studies in humans substantiate the notion that memory representations are reactivated and are reorganized during post-learning sleep; these reorganisations may account for the beneficial effects of sleep on behavioral performance. Electrophysiological results showing replay of neuronal sequences in animals are presented, and their relevance as neuronal correlates of memory reactivation is discussed. The reviewed literature provides converging evidence that structured neural representations can be activated during sleep. Which reorganizations unique to sleep benefit memory representations, and to what extent the operations still efficient in processing environmental information during sleep are similar to those underlying the non-conscious, automatic processing continually at work in wakefulness, are challenging questions open to investigation.

Introduction

Although the concept of representation has been questioned, in particular by cognitive psychologists (O’Regan & Noe, 2001) and neurophilosophers (Maturana and Varela, 1987, Thompson and Varela, 2001), in the field of neurosciences the notion of neural representation has become so popular over the last thirty years that it has now come into common use (see reviews by Andersen et al., 1997, Knudsen and Brainard, 1995, Logothetis, 1998, Maunsell, 1995, Phillips, 1993, Roland and Gulyas, 1994, Singer, 1998). This is not surprizing: albeit may be naive, it is traditionally considered that brain’s function is to integrate features of the external world and to build internal representations so as to generate a “model” of the world enabling complex sensory–motor interactions and cognitive functions. However, the notion of neural representation is not univocal. From one article to another, there is a very large diversity in the level and complexity of what is represented, as well as in the type of neural code supposed to underlie the neural representations. Beyond the classical topographic representations of the sensory epithelium described at the cortical (Cowey and Ellis, 1969, Kaas et al., 1979, Merzenich et al., 1975) and subcortical (Covey and Casseday, 1986, Malpeli and Baker, 1975, Merzenich and Reid, 1974) levels, neural representation ranges from simply a neuronal signal that has a content and performs a function (deCharms & Zador, 2000), to a response pattern obtained over a cortical area at presentation of a natural stimulus (Wang, Merzenich, Beitel, & Schreiner, 1995), and to widely distributed neuronal assemblies whose firing rate is synchronized across various brain areas in the millisecond scale (Engel, Fries, & Singer, 2001). The smallest common denominator to all these views is the idea that a neural representation contains information. The questions addressed in the present paper are thus: Do neuronal signals emitted during sleep carry information? Have they a content? Have they any functional relevance?

Addressing these questions may surprise given that loss of consciousness, loss of sensory awareness, and unresponsiveness to stimuli from the external world are among the major features defining sleep. However, sleep does not bring a little death. The neuroscience community no longer conceives of the sleeping brain as simply dormant, fully disconnected from the environment and completely quiescent. In particular, the relations between sleep and cognition have attracted considerable interest in the last decade, as attested by the recent spate of publications on that topic. In the field of sleep and cognition, now called “the cognitive neuroscience of sleep” (Hobson & Pace-Schott, 2002), it is explicitly or implicitly assumed that neural representations can be activated or can be spontaneously active and eventually modified during sleep. The present review presents some aspects of that literature, posing as many questions as it answers. Starting with the issue of sensory representations in the first two sections, we will progress by evaluating whether learned representations are accessible and can be expressed during sleep, to finally address, in the last section, the question of the dynamics of memory representations during sleep. Despite partial overlaps, the review does not cover the fields of “information processing during sleep” (see Atienza et al., 2001, Bonnet, 1982, Coenen and Drinkenburg, 2002), nor that of “sleep and memory” (see Maquet, Smith, & Stickgold, 2003b, as well as Gais and Born, 2004a, Rauchs et al., 2005, Walker and Stickgold, 2006). Lastly, though dreaming is the clearest evidence of the activation of internal representations during sleep, the special field of “dreams” is not tackled at all. Dreaming has long been the object of a neurobiological approach (Hobson & McCarley, 1977), but linking the subjective mental experiences of dreams with any neural representation is a challenge that is far from being achieved, if ever (see Fagioli, 2002, Hobson et al., 2000, Nielsen, 2000, Nielsen, 2004, Revonsuo, 2000, Schwartz and Maquet, 2002, Solms, 2000).

Classically, two states of sleep are distinguished, paradoxical sleep (PS; also known as rapid-eye movement sleep or REM sleep) and slow-wave sleep (SWS; also referred to as non-REM sleep). In animals, the terms SWS and non-REM sleep are synonymous, and the former is generally used. In humans, the two terms are not synonymous; non-REM sleep is subdivided into a continuum of four stages reflecting the depth of sleep: sleep onset period (stage 1), light sleep (stage 2), and SWS (stages 3 and 4). Because many of the reported data are from animal studies, the term SWS is used here in its more global acceptance (i.e., the whole non-REM sleep).

Contrary to waking and PS which are both associated with fast rhythms of brain electrical activity, SWS is characterized by large-amplitude, low-frequency (<15 Hz) oscillations reflecting a massive synchronization of neuronal activities in thalamocortical networks. Spindles, delta waves, and slow oscillation are the three brain rhythms defining SWS. The neuronal patterns prevailing in thalamocortical systems, a burst-silence mode during SWS versus a sustained single-spike activity during waking and PS, are under the control of generalized modulatory systems originating in the brainstem, the hypothalamus, and the basal forebrain (review in Jones, 2005, Pace-Schott and Hobson, 2002, Steriade, 2003, Steriade and McCarley, 1990). But none of the natural states of vigilance is uniform. It is well known that within the waking state, the level of arousal modulates sensory responses (e.g., Foote et al., 1991, Hubel et al., 1959, Morrow and Casey, 1992), and that attentional processes strongly influence sensory processing, either facilitating responses to target stimuli or suppressing responses to non-target stimuli (review in Desimone & Duncan, 1995). Qualitatively different epochs, and thereby differences in sensory processing, also exist within a given sleep state. This is the case of the epochs with or without ocular saccades in PS (e.g., Baust et al., 1964, Cairns et al., 2003). This is also the case of the depolarizing and hyperpolarizing phases of the slow oscillation in SWS (Massimini, Rosanova, & Mariotti, 2003). Indeed, during SWS, the membrane potential of cortical neurons oscillates between depolarized and hyperpolarized levels with a periodicity of about 1 s. This cyclic alternation is reflected in the electroencephalogram (EEG) by a slow (<1 Hz) oscillation: cortical neurons are depolarized and fire spikes during depth-negative EEG waves, while they are hyperpolarized during depth-positive EEG waves (Steriade et al., 2000, Timofeev et al., 2001). Lastly, some factors independent of the sleep process per se modulate sensory processing during sleep: to take only one example in human, differences in stimulus processing were observed between the first and second part of the night for the same sleep stage (Plihal, Weaver, Mölle, Fehm, & Born, 1996).

Section snippets

Sensory representations during sleep: Neuronal activity in sleeping animals

From which literature can it be inferred that sensory representations are or are not maintained during sleep? If we except the animal studies which, in the 1960s, examined how evoked potentials along sensory pathways varied with the sleep-wake states (for review of that literature, see Coenen, 1995, Hall and Borbély, 1970, Velluti, 1997; see also Meeren, van Cappellen van Walsum, van Luijtelaar, & Coenen, 2001), two sets of data are available: single unit electrophysiological recordings in

Sensory integration during sleep: Event-related brain potentials and functional neuroimaging in sleeping humans

Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) have been widely used to assess the extent of information processing during sleep in humans. We only present a small facet of that literature; for a more complete vision, see the following reviews (Atienza et al., 2001, Atienza et al., 2002, Bastuji and García-Larrea, 1999, Bastuji et al., 2002, Campbell et al., 1992, Cote, 2002).

In general, the earlier the component of the evoked potentials, the more it is affected by the physical characteristics of the

Representation of significant stimuli: Expression during sleep of learning-induced neuronal plasticity

The ability of sleeping subjects to discriminate between relevant and irrelevant auditory stimuli is general knowledge, based on numerous anecdotal observations and experimental evidence (review in Bonnet, 1982). We are all familiar with such phenomena as mothers waking up to their baby’s cry but not to irrelevant sounds, even when louder. Sleeping mothers are even able to discriminate their own baby’s cry from the cries of other babies: on the first three postnatal nights at the hospital, 58%

Dynamics of memory representations during sleep

The idea that sleep has a beneficial effect on memory formation is not new (Heine, 1914, Jenkins and Dallenbach, 1924), and it has received strong experimental support from an important stream of research that started at the end of the 1960s (review in Bloch et al., 1979, Ekstrand et al., 1977, Fishbein and Gutwein, 1977, Hennevin and Leconte, 1977, McGrath and Cohen, 1978, Pearlman, 1979, Smith, 1985; for more recent reviews see Giuditta et al., 1995, Hennevin et al., 1995, Smith, 1995). At

Conclusions

From the single cell to human behavior, studies converged and their results agreed to support the notion that neural representations, or at least parts of neural representations, can be in an active form during sleep. First, environmental stimuli do penetrate the sleeping organism: Although impoverished, the messages sent by sensory neurons provide information about the external world which is accurate enough to allow detection of behaviorally relevant stimuli; event-related brain potentials in

References (345)

  • H. Bastuji et al.

    Semantic analysis of auditory input during sleep: studies with event-related potentials

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2002)
  • W. Bialek et al.

    Reliability and information transmission in spiking neurons

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (1992)
  • C.R. Bramham et al.

    Synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus is modulated by behavioral state

    Brain Research

    (1989)
  • M. Brazdil et al.

    Intracerebral event-related potentials to subthreshold target stimuli

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2001)
  • J. Brualla et al.

    Auditory event-related potentials to semantic priming during sleep

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1998)
  • G.T. Buracas et al.

    Gauging sensory representations in the brain

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (1999)
  • G. Buzsáki

    Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for noisy brain states

    Neuroscience

    (1989)
  • L. Cahill et al.

    Is the amygdala a locus of conditioned fear? Some questions and caveats

    Neuron

    (1999)
  • B.E. Cairns et al.

    Eye movement-related modulation of trigeminal neuron activity during active sleep and wakefulness

    Brain Research

    (2003)
  • N. Censor et al.

    A link between perceptual learning, adaptation and sleep

    Vision Research

    (2006)
  • A.M.L. Coenen

    Neuronal activities underlying the electroencephalogram and evoked potentials of sleeping and waking: Implications for information processing

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (1995)
  • A.M.L. Coenen et al.

    Animal models for information processing during sleep

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2002)
  • K.A. Cote

    Probing awareness during sleep with the auditory odd-ball paradigm

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2002)
  • K.A. Cote et al.

    P300 to high intensity stimuli during REM sleep

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1999)
  • A. Cowey et al.

    The cortical representation of the retina in squirrel and rhesus monkeys and its relation to visual acuity

    Experimental Neurology

    (1969)
  • M. Czisch et al.

    Altered processing of acoustic stimuli during sleep: Reduced auditory activation and visual deactivation detected by a combined fMRI/EEG study

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • S.P.A. Drummond et al.

    Functional imaging of the sleeping brain: review of findings and implications for the study of insomnia

    Sleep Medicine Reviews

    (2004)
  • J.-M. Edeline

    Learning-induced physiological plasticity in the thalamo-cortical sensory systems: evaluation of receptive field plasticity, map changes and their potential mechanisms

    Progress in Neurobiology

    (1999)
  • B.R. Ekstrand et al.

    The effect of sleep on human long-term memory

  • J.M. Ellenbogen et al.

    Interfering with theories of sleep and memory: sleep, declarative memory, and associative interference

    Current Biology

    (2006)
  • I. Fagioli

    Mental activity during sleep

    Sleep Medicine Reviews

    (2002)
  • M.S. Fanselow et al.

    Why we think plasticity underlying Pavlovian fear conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala

    Neuron

    (1999)
  • W. Fishbein et al.

    Paradoxical sleep and memory storage processes

    Behavioral Biology

    (1977)
  • S.L. Foote et al.

    Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of the locus coeruleus in alerting, orienting, and attending

    Progress in Brain Research

    (1991)
  • M. Abeles et al.

    Spatiotemporal firing patterns in the frontal cortex of behaving monkeys

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1993)
  • M. Abeles et al.

    Detecting spatiotemporal firing patterns among simultaneously recorded single neurons

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1988)
  • A.M. Aertsen et al.

    Dynamics of neuronal firing correlation: modulation of effective connectivity

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1989)
  • E. Ahissar et al.

    Dependence of cortical plasticity on correlated activity of single neurons and on behavioral context

    Science

    (1992)
  • R.A. Andersen et al.

    Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements

    Annual Review of Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • D. Ari-Even Roth et al.

    A latent consolidation phase in auditory identification learning: time in the awake state is sufficient

    Learning and Memory

    (2005)
  • M. Atienza et al.

    Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep

    Psychophysiology

    (2000)
  • M. Atienza et al.

    Posttraining sleep enhances automaticity in perceptual discrimination

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • H. Bastuji et al.

    Brain processing of stimulus deviance during slow-wave and paradoxical sleep: a study of human auditory evoked responses using the oddball paradigm

    Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1995)
  • W. Baust et al.

    Changes in the auditory input in wakefulness and during the synchronized and desynchronized stages of sleep

    Archives Italiennes de Biologie

    (1964)
  • H.C. Beh et al.

    Discrimination and conditioning during sleep as indicated by the electroencephalogram

    Science

    (1965)
  • O. Benoit et al.

    Seuils d’excitabilité réticulaire et sommeil profond chez le chat

    Journal de Physiologie (Paris)

    (1960)
  • O. Benoit et al.

    Patterns of spontaneous unitary discharge in thalamic ventro-basal complex during wakefulness and sleep

    Experimental Brain Research

    (1973)
  • V. Bloch et al.

    Relationship between paradoxical sleep and memory processes

  • M. Bonnet

    Performance during sleep

  • A. Borst et al.

    Information theory and neural coding

    Nature Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • Cited by (94)

    • Sleep and its regulation: An emerging pathogenic and treatment frontier in Alzheimer's disease

      2021, Progress in Neurobiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, there is only preliminary and conflicting evidence for acute effects of sleep on Aβ levels in humans. For symptomatic AD patients, sleep (and SWS in particular) is important to target with interventions in view of the extensive literature demonstrating the important role of sleep for attention, executive control, and declarative and non-declarative memory (Hennevin et al., 2007; McCoy and Strecker, 2011; Oudiette and Paller, 2013; Rasch and Born, 2013; Scullin and Bliwise, 2015), and that specific sleep stages are critical for hippocampal- and non-hippocampal-dependent memory functions and neuroplasticity (Havekes et al., 2016; Kent and Mistlberger, 2017; Kreutzmann et al., 2015). SWS disruption has even been proposed as a critical pathway by which Aβ disrupts hippocampal-dependent memory consolidation (Mander et al., 2015).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text