Trends in Neurosciences
Volume 29, Issue 9, September 2006, Pages 496-505
Journal home page for Trends in Neurosciences

Opinion
Different molecular cascades in different sites of the brain control memory consolidation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2006.07.005Get rights and content

To understand cognition, it is important to understand how a learned response becomes a long-lasting memory. This process of memory consolidation has been modeled extensively using one-trial avoidance learning, in which animals (or humans) establish a conditioned response by learning to avoid danger in just one trial. This relies on molecular events in the CA1 region of the hippocampus that resemble those involved in CA1 long-term potentiation (LTP), and it also requires equivalent events to occur with different timings in the basolateral amygdala and the entorhinal, parietal and cingulate cortex. Many of these steps are modulated by monoaminergic pathways related to the perception of and reaction to emotion, which at least partly explains why strong and resistant consolidation is typical of emotion-laden memories. Thus memory consolidation involves a complex network of brain systems and serial and parallel molecular events, even for a task as deceptively simple as one-trial avoidance. We propose that these molecular events might also be involved in many other memory types in animals and humans.

Introduction

One-trial avoidance has been widely used for the study of memory consolidation. There have been many successful inferences from this task to other forms of memory, especially those of fear-motivated learning and spatial tasks. One-trial avoidance corresponds to many important examples of learning in humans, such as not to stick your fingertips into electric plug sockets, to look right rather than left when crossing a street in London, and to refrain from entering a suspicious-looking neighborhood.

The sequence of molecular mechanisms underlying long-term potentiation (LTP) in the CA1 region of the hippocampus has been proposed to underlie memory consolidation in that region 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8. This was shown to be indeed the case for one type of memory: one-trial avoidance learning 9, 10. However, at the same time, different sequences of molecular changes in other brain regions are also crucial for memory consolidation in this and other tasks 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18. The main difference between the molecular changes in the hippocampus and the other structures (the basolateral amygdala, and the entorhinal and parietal cortex) is their timing and sequential order 9, 10, 11. Timing among its diverse underlying molecular events is a key factor in the build-up of LTP maintenance 1, 4, 10 and, as shown elsewhere 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14 and discussed here, in the build-up of consolidation. The sheer number of connections within and among various brain structures in LTP and in memory formation is the reason why these processes are so complex 1, 4, 9, 10, and why it is difficult to schematize them. Attempts have been made to devise schemes or blueprints for LTP 1, 4 and memory 2, 3, 9, 10. However, by the end of this article we hope to have persuaded readers that memory consolidation requires not only LTP (or a process very much like LTP) in the hippocampal CA1 sub-area but also different sequences of the same or of related signaling pathways in other cortical areas and the amygdala 9, 10, 17.

Section snippets

What is memory consolidation?

Most neuroscientists view consolidation as a process lasting a few hours through which memories are transformed from a labile into a more stable state 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14. However, several authors favor the view that there is also another form of consolidation that lasts for many days or months, or even a lifetime [18]. No doubt memories can accrue or lose information over prolonged periods 9, 10. However, over days, months or years there are oscillations in the rate

Why one-trial avoidance

One-trial step-down or step-through inhibitory (‘passive’) avoidance in rodents 9, 10, 18, or one-trial peck avoidance in young chicks 19, 20, has long been a favorite model for biochemical and pharmacological studies of memory. In the rodent models, the animals learn that stepping down from a platform or stepping through a door is followed by a footshock; on subsequent exposures to the task they will stay much longer on the safe platform or on the safe side of the door before eventually

Main molecular events in CA1 LTP and in the consolidation of one-trial-avoidance memory

The main events in CA1 during consolidation of one-trial avoidance involve, like those in CA1 LTP 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 29, the initial activation of AMPA, metabotropic and particularly NMDA glutamate receptors 2, 5, 7, 9, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36. This causes an increase of intracellular Ca2+ concentration ([Ca2+]i) 1, 5, followed by an enhancement of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent kinase II (CaMKII) activity in the first 60 min 7, 37, 38, 39, which leads to phosphorylation of AMPA receptors 39, 40 and a

Main events in memory formation of one-trial learning in the basolateral amygdala, entorhinal cortex and posterior parietal cortex

Table 2, Table 3, Table 4 summarize the main published findings on memory consolidation in the basolateral amygdala, entorhinal cortex and posterior parietal cortex, respectively. There have been a few scattered findings on the posterior parietal cortex (a prolonged amnesic effect of the ERK1/2 inhibitor U0126 and of the GABA receptor agonist muscimol when given up to 3 h post-training [14]). Findings in other structures (e.g. the insular lobe and prefrontal areas) have been less consistent or

What forms of plasticity are involved in each structure?

Clearly, in CA1 the form of plasticity involved in memory consolidation is one closely similar to LTP, if not actual LTP 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10. In the other structures the sequences of molecular processes are different from those described for LTP. However, the receptors involved (particularly the NMDA receptor) 9, 10, 11, 14, 34, 36, 83 and the function of the enzymes involved (particularly their relationship to receptor activation, CREB phosphorylation, gene expression, protein

Why are emotionally-rich memories usually better consolidated?

The post-training hippocampal PKA activation and CREB phosphorylation, and memory consolidation itself, are highly sensitive to dopamine receptor agonists and to antagonists acting on D1 receptors, β-adrenoceptors and 5HT1A receptors [50] (Table 1, Table 3, Table 4). The pathways ending on dopamine D1 receptors and β-adrenoceptors in the hippocampus and neocortex are well known to mediate alertness, emotion and anxiety levels. D1 receptors and β-adrenoceptors enhance, and 5HT1A receptors

Endogenous state dependency revisited

Table 1, Table 2, Table 3, Table 4 show that memory consolidation is accompanied and regulated by different neuromodulatory and molecular ‘states’. So is retrieval [89]. Memories, particularly those of an emotional type, have been suggested to rely on an endogenous state-dependent process 90, 91, 92, 93.

Both consolidation and retrieval of one-trial avoidance require PKA and ERK activity in the hippocampus, the entorhinal, parietal and cingulate cortex and the basolateral amygdala, and both are

A note on the parallel processing of immediate, short-term and long-term memory

STM is now known to last ∼3–6 h and is processed in parallel to the consolidation of LTM 21, 22. Before that, and as a precondition for both STM and LTM formation, working memory (WM) handles the information that is to be saved while it is being acquired and/or for a few seconds afterwards 9, 10, 11. The mechanisms of WM and STM are in general not directly related to those of LTM formation, and the interested reader is referred to Ref. [11] for a recent review of this topic.

Several of the

A caveat

The list of brain areas that participate in memory consolidation is certainly longer than the one we have discussed in this review. If CA1 and the entorhinal cortex participate, then surely the dentate gyrus and CA3 must be involved [33]. Recent evidence supports this [94]. The same stands for other molecules not specifically discussed here. For example, recent data point to a key role for the ubiquitin–proteasome system [96] and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) 97, 98, 99 in STM and

Can a neural network be proposed?

The sequence of events in CA1 LTP has been worked out by several authors 1, 3, 4, 5, 6. However, beyond the early NMDA receptor activation, and the subsequent increase in [Ca2+]i, activation of CaMKII and enhancement of AMPA receptor, PKC and PKA activity, things become blurred and it is difficult to be precise, especially about timing [44]. There could also be alternative connections between metabolic routes, particularly those involving signaling pathways and gene activation [7]. Indeed, LTP

Concluding remarks

Different chains of molecular events involving glutamate receptors, CaMKII, PKA, PKC and ERK1/2 are necessary for LTM formation in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, entorhinal cortex, parietal cortex and cingulate cortex. The time course of the events in CA1 is similar but not identical to that which underlies LTP in this area. The timings of the molecular events in the other structures differ from each other and from those in CA1. Several of the biochemical events

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from FAPERGS/CNPQ, Brazil, through the National Program for Nuclei of Excellence (PRONEX), by CONICET and ANPCyT, Argentina, and by NIH grant R21-TW007800-01.

References (110)

  • L.R.M. Bevilaqua

    Memory consolidation induces N-methyl-d-aspartic acid-receptor- and Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II-dependent modifications in α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionic acid receptor properties

    Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • M. Cammarota

    Inhibitory avoidance training induces rapid and selective changes in 3[H]AMPA receptor binding in the rat hippocampal formation

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (1995)
  • M. Cammarota

    Reversible changes in hippocampal 3[H]-AMPA binding following inhibitory avoidance training in the rat

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (1996)
  • P.A. Colley et al.

    Long-term potentiation as synaptic dialogue

    Brain Res. Rev.

    (1993)
  • G. Paratcha

    Involvement of hippocampal PKCβI isoform in the early phase of memory formation of an inhibitory avoidance learning

    Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • Y.Y. Huang

    cAMP contributes to mossy fiber LTP by initiating both a covalently mediated early phase and macromolecular synthesis-dependent late phase

    Cell

    (1994)
  • Y.Y. Huang et al.

    Modulation of both the early and the late phase of mossy fiber LTP by the activation of β-adrenergic receptors

    Neuron

    (1996)
  • J.D. Sweatt

    Mitogen-activated protein kinases in synaptic plasticity and memory

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2004)
  • J.D. English et al.

    A requirement for the mitogen-activated protein kinase cascade in hippocampal long term potentiation

    J. Biol. Chem.

    (1997)
  • C. Pittenger

    Reversible inhibition of CREB/ATF transcription factors in region CA1 of the dorsal hippocampus disrupts hippocampus-dependent spatial memory

    Neuron

    (2002)
  • M. Alonso

    Aversive experiences are associated with a rapid and transient activation of ERKs in the rat hippocampus

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2002)
  • J.H. Medina et al.

    Retrograde messengers, long-term potentiation, and memory processes

    Brain Res. Rev.

    (1995)
  • F.D. Lubin

    Kainate mediates nuclear factor-κB activation in hippocampus via phosphatidylinositol-3 kinase and extracellular signal-regulated protein kinase

    Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • M. Cammarota

    Learning-associated activation of nuclear MAPK, CREB and Elk-1, along with Fos production, in the rat hippocampus after a one-trial avoidance learning: abolition by NMDA-R blockade

    Mol. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • J.D. English et al.

    Activation of p42 mitogen activated protein kinase in hippocampal long-term potentiation

    J. Biol. Chem.

    (1996)
  • E.D. Roberson et al.

    Transient activation of cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase during hippocampal long-term potentiation

    J. Biol. Chem.

    (1996)
  • A. Abeliovich

    Modified hippocampal long-term potentiation in PKCγ-mutant mice

    Cell

    (1993)
  • R. Bourtchuladze

    Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein

    Cell

    (1994)
  • I. Izquierdo

    Neurotransmitter receptors involved in post-training memory processing by the amygdala, medial septum and hippocampus of the rat

    Behav. Neural Biol.

    (1992)
  • M. Cammarota

    Learning twice is different from learning once or from learning more

    Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • L. Cahill

    The neurobiology of learning and memory: some reminders to remember

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2001)
  • H.T. Blair

    The lateral amygdala processes the value of conditioned and unconditioned aversive stimuli

    Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • E.A. Phelps

    Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2004)
  • D.M. Barros

    Molecular signaling pathways in the cerebral cortex are required for retrieval of one-trial avoidance learning in rats

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • I. Izquierdo

    Different forms of posttraining memory processing

    Behav. Neural Biol.

    (1989)
  • C.R. Bramham et al.

    BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: the synaptic consolidation hypothesis

    Prog. Neurobiol.

    (2005)
  • P.T. Pang et al.

    Regulation of late-phase LTP and long-term memory in normal and aging hippocampus: role of secreted proteins tPA and BDNF

    Ageing Res. Rev.

    (2004)
  • T.V. Bliss et al.

    A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus

    Nature

    (1993)
  • R.C. Malenka et al.

    Long-term potentiation – a decade of progress?

    Science

    (1999)
  • E.R. Kandel et al.

    Neuroscience: breaking down scientific barriers to the study of brain and mind

    Science

    (2000)
  • S.J. Martin

    Synaptic plasticity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • R.C. Malenka

    The long-term potential of LTP

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • R.G. Morris

    Elements of a neurobiological theory of the hippocampus: the role of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity in memory

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.

    (2003)
  • I. Izquierdo et al.

    Behavioural pharmacology and its contribution to the molecular basis of memory consolidation

    Behav. Pharmacol.

    (2000)
  • L.A. Izquierdo

    Molecular pharmacological dissection of short- and long-term memory

    Cell. Mol. Neurobiol.

    (2002)
  • P. Ardenghi

    Late and prolonged post-training memory modulation in entorhinal and parietal cortex by drugs acting on the cAMP/protein kinase A signalling pathway

    Behav. Pharmacol.

    (1997)
  • J.S. Bonini

    AMPA/kainate and group-I metabotropic receptor antagonists infused into different brain areas impair memory formation of inhibitory avoidance in rats

    Behav. Pharmacol.

    (2003)
  • J.I. Rossato

    Retrograde amnesia induced by drugs acting on different molecular systems depends on how long after training these drugs are infused in selected brain areas

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • B. Sacchetti

    Auditory thalamus, dorsal hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, and perirhinal cortex role in the consolidation of conditioned freezing to context and to acoustic conditioned stimulus in the rat

    J. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • J.D. Brioni

    Role of GABA during the multiple consolidation of memory

    Drug Dev. Res.

    (1993)
  • Cited by (387)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text