INTRODUCTION

Multiple studies have demonstrated that PTSD patients complain of recurrent nightmares and sleep continuity disturbances, which are listed separately in the re-experiencing and hyperarousal clusters in the DSM-IV criteria. Roszell et al (1991) found in a study of 116 Vietnam veterans receiving treatment in a PTSD specialty clinic that disturbed sleep, separate from nightmares, was the most frequently reported symptom (90% of subjects). Hyperarousal during sleep, a well-established finding in mood and psychotic disorders, is demonstrable on polysomnography by increased awakenings and decreased sleep depth as measured by the levels of slow wave or delta sleep. In healthy subjects, delta sleep activity is maximal in the first half of the night and is temporally associated with peak growth hormone release and the nadir of cortisol output (Van Cauter et al, 2000). Many studies in subjects with major depression have documented reduced delta sleep, increased cortisol release, and reduced growth hormone output as reviewed by Riemann et al (2001). These studies along with an extensive literature in healthy subjects support the hypothesis that delta sleep is in part mediated by hormonal factors and is associated with the homeostatic and physical restorative function of sleep (Borbely and Achermann, 2000; Feinberg, 1974; Vgontzas et al, 1999). Hence it is surprising that only two studies have shown that PTSD, a putative hyperarousal disorder, is associated with decreased visually scored delta sleep (Glaubman et al, 1990; Kramer and Kinney, 1988). This contrasts with a larger number of studies of PTSD that have found no differences in visually scored delta sleep (Brown and Boudewyns, 1996; Dagan et al, 1991; Dow et al, 1996; Hefez et al, 1987; Hurwitz et al, 1998; Klein et al, 2002; Lavie et al, 1979; Mellman et al, 1995a,1995b,1995c,1997; Ross et al, 1994a,1994b; Woodward et al, 1996a,1996b,1996c). This might suggest that individuals with PTSD misperceive their sleep duration and depth, or that mediators of delta sleep, such as nocturnal hormone activity, differ in PTSD compared to other hyperarousal disorders such as depression.

Another possible explanation for failing to find reduced delta sleep in PTSD relates to the poor sensitivity of visual sleep staging, or sleep macroarchitecture, in quantifying delta sleep (reviewed in Uchida et al, 1999). Visual scoring is based on arbitrary criteria that produce a crude single category score (eg stage 4) for each epoch of sleep, which obscures the complexity of the sleep electroencephalogram (EEG). Further, visually scored delta sleep typically represents a small fraction of total sleep time and most studies lack the power to detect significant group differences. Woodward and colleagues have published the only study that employed quantitative analysis of the sleep EEG in PTSD subjects. In this study, the distinguishing feature in a sample of 56 unmedicated Vietnam combat veterans was that subjects with PTSD compared to controls had significantly decreased delta spectral power during nonrapid-eye-movement (NREM) sleep (Woodward et al, 2000). For this reason, studies utilizing quantitative measures of the sleep EEG may be more informative in studies of PTSD. Further, challenge paradigms that manipulate delta sleep, such as sleep deprivation or administration of certain drugs, may be needed in order to better characterize delta sleep regulation in PTSD.

A large body of literature suggests that elevations in both hypothalamic (neurohormonal) and extrahypothalamic (neurotransmitter) corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) release is associated with decreased delta sleep activity. The evidence supporting the role of increased extrahypothalamic CRF being associated with decreased delta sleep includes studies in which intracerebroven-tricular administration of CRF in adult rats results in a potent reduction in delta sleep (Ehlers et al, 1986; Chang and Opp, 2001). Further, intracerebroventricular blockade of central CRF receptors in freely behaving rats reduces spontaneous waking (Chang and Opp, 1998). The evidence supporting the role of increased hypothalamic CRF being associated with decreased delta sleep includes the following: (1) Hypercortisolemic depression is associated with increased hypothalamic CRF and decreased delta sleep (reviewed in (Gold et al, 1988a,1988b)). (2) There is a strong inverse relation between delta sleep and pulsatile cortisol release (see Vgontzas et al, 1999; Born et al, 1991 for a review). The apparent relation between peripheral cortisol levels and delta sleep is likely to be driven by the activity of hypothalamic CRF, because exogenous cortisol infusion, which reduces CRF in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN), increases delta sleep (Friess et al, 1994; Bohlhalter et al, 1997). (3) Metyrapone administration, which leads to an increase in hypothalamic, but not extrahypothalamic CRF (Kalin et al, 1987), causes a decrease in delta sleep (Jahn et al, 1996).

Metyrapone administration is used diagnostically to test the integrity of the entire HPA axis. It blocks the enzymatic conversion of 11-deoxycortisol to cortisol resulting in a stimulation of hypothalamic CRF and ACTH and subsequent accumulation of 11-deoxycortisol (Fiad et al, 1994). Metyrapone acutely reduces cortisol levels and eliminates cortisol-mediated feedback inhibition of the HPA axis. Metyrapone effectively exposes the underlying drive of the PVN to release CRF and arginine vasopressin (Akil and Morano, 1995). Metyrapone administration in rats resulted in EEG activation and reduced sleep time. In this study, the metyrapone effect on sleep was mediated by the rise of hypothalamic CRF as opposed to the drop in cortisol (Burade et al, 1996). EEG activation and loss of sleep time was reversed by anti-CRF antibodies.

PTSD may be associated with increases in both extrahypothalamic and hypothalamic CRF activity. Two studies have found higher levels of CRF in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) in PTSD (Baker et al, 1999; Bremner et al, 1997). CSF reflects mainly extrahypothalamic sources (Kalin et al, 1987; Vythilingam et al, 2000). Increased hypothalamic CRF in PTSD is thought to explain, in part, the blunted ACTH response to CRF in PTSD found in some (Heim et al, 2001; Smith et al, 1989) but not all studies (Kellner et al, 2002; Rasmusson et al, 2001). Increased hypothalamic CRF has also been inferred from a number of studies that have found elevated cortisol levels in PTSD (De Bellis et al, 1999; Lemieux and Coe, 1995; Liberzon et al, 1999; Maes et al, 1998; Pitman and Orr, 1990). Despite possible elevated hypothalamic CRF activity, PTSD subjects in a large number of studies have been found to have either normal (Baker et al, 1999) or decreased 24-h urinary cortisol (Mason et al, 1986; Yehuda et al, 1995; Yehuda et al, 1990), normal (Kellner et al, 2002) or decreased plasma cortisol (Jensen et al, 1997; Yehuda et al, 1996b), increased lymphocyte glucocorticoid receptors (Yehuda et al, 1991), normal (Kosten et al, 1990) or increased suppression of cortisol in response to dexamethasone (Goenjian et al, 1996; Grossman et al, 1996; Stein et al, 1997; Yehuda et al, 1993), and a buffered ultradian pattern of cortisol release (Yehuda et al, 1996b). Most of these findings are consistent with the hypothesis that PTSD is associated with enhanced negative feedback of the HPA axis, or reduced adrenal output, or a combination of these two mechanisms (for a review, see Yehuda, 2002). The combination of increased hypothalamic CRF activity with normal or low levels of cortisol in PTSD appears to be a unique phenomenon in psychiatric disorders (Kasckow et al, 2001). However, more studies are needed to further characterize hypothalamic CRF activity in PTSD. The sleep EEG response to metyrapone is a measure of the brain response to increases in hypothalamic CRF and thus has potential advantages over other challenge paradigms, such as examining the pituitary response to exogenous CRF. Hence, examining sleep in the context of a metyrapone challenge may provide more information about the brain response to a hypothalamic CRF challenge in PTSD.

Hence, given the remaining uncertainties about the status of delta sleep and hypothalamic CRF in PTSD, and the lack of any study that has examined the relation between the two, we employed a metyrapone challenge to test indirectly if hypothalamic CRF activity is associated with delta sleep in PTSD. We report here the effect of metyrapone administration on different domains of sleep disturbances, particularly delta sleep; and endocrine variables, particularly 11-deoxycortisol and ACTH, in a sample of male subjects with and without chronic PTSD. We hypothesized that metyrapone would result in a decrease in delta sleep as measured by a period amplitude analysis. Further, we predicted that the metyrapone-induced decrease in delta sleep would be associated with the endocrine (increased 11-deoxycortisol and ACTH) response. Finally, we predicted that PTSD subjects, compared to controls, would have lower levels of delta sleep at baseline and a greater decreases in delta sleep induced by metyrapone.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Medically healthy male subjects were recruited from web-based and newspaper advertisement and from the San Francisco Veterans Affairs Medical Center (SFVAMC) PTSD Outpatient Program. All subjects were given details of the study and asked to sign a written informed consent form if they wished to participate. The study protocol and consent form was approved by the Committee on Human Research at the University of California, San Francisco (UCSF). In all, 24 PTSD subjects and 18 controls completed three nights of polysomnography on the General Clinical Research Center at UCSF. Subjects were included if they met DSM-IV criteria for current PTSD as assessed by the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; Blake et al, 1995). The mean age for the PTSD subjects was 49.4 (SD=5.7) and control subjects was 47.8 (SD=9.3). The mean CAPS score for the PTSD subjects was 64 (SD=17) compared to 2 (SD=4) in controls. Only six of the PTSD subjects met current criteria for major depression. All control subjects were negative for lifetime major depression. Subjects were excluded if they met criteria for alcohol or substance abuse within the past 2 years, lifetime criteria for organic mental disorder, schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, bipolar disorder, panic disorder, or obsessive compulsive disorder as assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-P; Spitzer et al, 1992). Medical exclusion criteria included any history of brain disease, current systemic illness affecting central nervous system function, or use of any medication affecting the brain. All subjects were free of any psychiatric medications for at least 2 months prior to participation. An oximeter (Respironics Cricket) was used to screen for obstructive sleep apnea (OSA). The cutoff criterion for apnea was 10 desaturation events per hour in bed, which has been shown to have a sensitivity of 98% and specificity of 48% in detecting OSA (Sériès et al, 1993). Subjects who screened positive for OSA were excluded. All subjects were alcohol free and restricted to having one optional cup of caffeinated coffee each morning for 2 weeks prior to the three nights of sleep recordings. Ambulatory polysomnography using an Oxford MR95 digital recorder was used to monitor three nights of sleep. Subjects adhered to a stable sleep–wake schedule at their habitual times. Subjects filled out self-report ratings of subjective sleep quality, depression, PTSD symptoms, and mood states utilizing a sleep diary (100 mm visual analog scale), the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al, 1961), the Impact of Event Scale-Revised (Weiss, 1997), and the Profile of Mood States (McNair et al, 1992) each morning after the sleep recordings. The first night of polysomnography was used as an adaptation night and was not utilized in the analyses. On the morning after the second night, blood was collected by venipuncture and plasma was assayed for ACTH, 11-deoxycortisol, and cortisol. Subjects received 750 mg of metyrapone every 4 h for a total of four doses along with 30 cm3 of an antacid. The timing of the doses was adjusted so that the last dose occurred at the subject's self-reported habitual bedtime. The following morning, 8–9.5 h after the last metyrapone dose, another blood sample was collected to measure ACTH, 11-deoxycortisol, and cortisol. Subjects were all NPO for 9 h prior to the two morning blood draws.

Objective sleep quality was measured with ambulatory polysomnography (Oxford MR95 recorder). The parameters recorded included an electroencephalogram (EEG) at leads C3 and C4, left and right electro-oculograms (EOG), and submental electromyogram (EMG), and electrocardiogram (EKG) in accordance with standardized guidelines (Rechtschaffen et al, 1968). The EEG and EOG leads were referenced to linked mastoids. All sleep was imported into Pass Plus (Delta Software) analytic software and visually scored in 30-s epochs in accordance to Rechtschaffen and Kales (1968).

Sleep architecture was delineated as the percentage of time spent asleep in NREM stages 1–4 and stage REM. Sleep continuity was measured by calculating sleep maintenance defined as the ratio of total time spent asleep divided by the total recording period between sleep onset and offset. An awakening was defined by EEG arousals lasting 30 s or longer. REM periods were defined by at least 3 min of consecutive REM sleep with no less than 30 min of NREM sleep separating two REM periods. REM measures included: REM percent, REM latency (minutes from sleep onset to first REM period), REM activity (number of rapid eye movements), and REM density (REM activity/minutes REM sleep).

Delta sleep activity was analyzed by period amplitude analysis (PAA) using the Pass Plus (Delta Software) analytic software. Integrated amplitude of 0.3–4.0 Hz activity per 30 s epoch was analyzed by NREM/REM cycles across all recorded sleep following the technique described by Feinberg et al (1987),(1991) and Travis et al (1991). PAA analyses were conducted on all epochs of NREM and REM sleep. Epochs scored as wake were not included in these analyses. Movement artifact was visually tagged and not included in the analyses.

STATISTICAL ANALYSES

Descriptive data provide means and standard deviations of the symptom levels and subjective and objective sleep variables pre- and postmetyrapone. The effect of metyrapone on symptom levels, subjective sleep quality visually scored sleep stages, quantitative delta sleep as indexed by period amplitude analysis, and plasma cortisol, ACTH, and 11-deoxycortisol was analyzed by a repeated measures ANOVA with condition (metyrapone administration) as a within-subject factor and group membership as a between-subject factor. The relations among the changes in delta sleep, cortisol, 11-deoxycortisol, and ACTH were analyzed by two-tailed Pearson correlations.

RESULTS

Metyrapone administration was generally well tolerated in both the PTSD and normal control subjects. The most common side effect was dizziness that occurred in three PTSD and two control subjects. Nausea was reported by two control subjects, both of whom felt relief from the use of an antacid. No subjects dropped out of the study because of adverse effects of metyrapone. The repeated measures ANOVA of subjective sleep quality, the Beck Depression Inventory, Impact of Event Scale-Revised, and the Profile of Mood States showed that metyrapone did not have a significant effect on subjective sleep quality, depression, PTSD symptoms, or mood states when measured the morning after each sleep recording.

The effect of metyrapone on visually scored sleep stages and quantitative delta sleep as indexed by period amplitude analysis is presented in Table 1. PTSD subjects had significantly more stage 1 sleep, and less delta sleep as indexed by stages 3 and 4, and total delta integrated amplitude. As predicted, metyrapone administration had an arousing effect on the sleep EEG as indexed by a significant increase in awakenings and associated decrease in sleep maintenance. Period amplitude analysis of delta sleep showed a marked decrease in total integrated amplitude, time in the delta band, and number of delta waves as indexed by baseline crossings. There was a significant group by condition effect for total delta integrated amplitude, demonstrating that the effect of metyrapone was significantly greater in controls than PTSD. Finally, by restricting the analysis to delta integrated amplitude to the first three NREM, we found highly significant effects for condition, group, and group-by-condition, suggesting that the impact of metyrapone, as expected, occurs in the first portion of the sleep period.

Table 1 Effect of Metyrapone on Visually Scored Sleep and Period Amplitude Analysis of Delta Sleep in PTSD and Controls

The effect of metyrapone on AM cortisol, 11-deoxycortisol, and ACTH in PTSD and controls is presented in Table 2. There is a strong effect of metyrapone administration on the 11-deoxycortisol and ACTH response. There is also a marginally significant increase in cortisol postmetyrapone. The next day postmetyrapone cortisol was not suppressed because the samples were obtained in the morning 8–9 h after the last dose of metyrapone. There is a significant effect of PTSD status on the ACTH response, in that PTSD subjects had less of an ACTH rise compared to controls.

Table 2 Cortisol, 11-Deoxycortisol, and ACTH Responses to Metyrapone in PTSD and Controls

The relation among the changes in delta sleep, cortisol, 11-deoxycortisol, and ACTH is presented in a Pearson correlation matrix of change scores in Table 3. In each measure, the change score was calculated as Night 3 (postmetyrapone) minus Night 2 (premetyrapone). The full matrix is presented in order to highlight the relations both within and across the sleep and endocrine measures. The effect of metyrapone on reducing delta-integrated amplitude was significantly associated with the magnitude of increase in both 11-deoxycortisol and ACTH. As expected, changes in visually scored delta sleep were significantly associated with changes in delta-integrated amplitude. However, the only significant association with the endocrine response and changes in visually scored sleep stages was found with stage 3 and 11-deoxycortisol.

Table 3 Correlation of Changes in Delta Sleep, Cortisol, 11-Deoxycortisol, and ACTH

DISCUSSION

Prior to metyrapone administration, we found evidence of decreased delta sleep in PTSD compared to controls, which is similar to the Woodward study (Woodward et al, 2000). We confirmed our prediction that metyrapone would lead to a decrease in delta sleep in both PTSD and controls, although this effect was unexpectedly greater in the control group. Further, we confirmed our prediction that the decrease in delta sleep would be significantly associated with the endocrine (increased 11-deoxycortisol and ACTH) response. This is supportive evidence for the fact that the delta sleep response to metyrapone is a measure of the brain response to a hypothalamic CRF challenge. This model is summarized schematically in Figure 1. The effect of metyrapone on delta sleep is predicted both by the rise in 11-deoxycortisol, which is most proximal to the adrenal blockade of cortisol synthesis, and by the magnitude of rise in ACTH, which presumably is stimulated by an increase in hypothalamic CRF release secondary to the reduction of feedback regulation by cortisol. These correlations are strong evidence that the effect of metyrapone on sleep is mediated by the response of the HPA axis as opposed to a possible nonendocrine effect of metyrapone. There is also a stronger relation between the ACTH response and delta-integrated amplitude as opposed to visually scored delta sleep. This is not surprising given that visual sleep stages are insensitive measures of delta sleep. Period amplitude analysis in this study increased our power to detect significant correlations between the delta sleep and endocrine responses to metyrapone.

Figure 1
figure 1

Model of sleep and endrocrine response to metyrapone. CRF=Corticotropin-releasing factor, PVN=paraventricular nucleus, ACTH=adrenocorticotropic hormone

We were surprised to see a reduced endocrine and delta sleep response in PTSD compared with controls. In the context of possible increased hypothalamic CRF levels in PTSD, the baseline normal cortisol and ACTH levels in our study suggest that the central response of ACTH to CRF is downregulated in PTSD. This would account for a diminished ACTH response to metyrapone in PTSD. However, the lower rise in ACTH levels in PTSD in this study differs from a study of metyrapone administration by Yehuda et al (1996a), who found an increased response. An alternate explanation for the difference in the two studies, as well as our finding of decreased endocrine and sleep responses in PTSD, is that the same dose of metyrapone did not produce the same degree of adrenal suppression of cortisol synthesis. Thus, the reduced endocrine and sleep response to metyrapone in PTSD suggests either that PTSD subjects have downregulated central responses to CRF or that PTSD subjects had less of a CRF response because of less adrenal suppression of cortisol. The latter possibility is supported by the decreased rise in 11-deoxycortisol in PTSD compared to controls.

The endocrine responses to metyrapone in this study do not support a model of reduced adrenal capacity in PTSD as has been suggested by several investigators (Heim et al, 2000; Kanter et al, 2001). Reduced adrenal capacity would be expected to yield a large ratio of ACTH to cortisol release. The mean ACTH/cortisol ratio prior to metyrapone administration is no different in PTSD vs controls (PTSD=1.83, controls=1.86; t=−0.1, p=0.95). The mean ACTH/cortisol ratio after metyrapone is actually lower, although nonsignificantly, in PTSD compared with controls (PTSD=11.20; controls=24.31; t=−1.4, p=0.16), which is evidence against reduced adrenal capacity.

The metyrapone-induced decrease in delta sleep in this study differs from a study of metyrapone and sleep by Gillin et al (1974b), who did not show an activating effect in a small sample (N=5) of young men. It is possible that the relatively young age of the subjects in the Gillin et al study (age 20–25 years) may have had some influence on the results. Both HPA function and sleep are strongly influenced by maturational changes that may explain, for example, why adolescents with major depression do not have similar HPA and sleep abnormalities as depressed adults (Gillin et al, 1981; Rao et al, 1996). This possible age effect is also consistent with the results from Vgontzas et al (2001), who showed that young men did not have an activating response to CRF infusion in contrast to middle age men, who showed a prominent decrease in delta sleep Metyrapone administration in our current study yielded similar results as the Jahn study (Jahn et al, 1996) in that there was a robust decrease in delta sleep.

Our data are consistent with the hypothesis that delta sleep activity is mediated in part by hypothalamic CRF. Delta sleep occurs when CRF is minimally active (Friess et al, 1995). CRF pulses increase after 4–5 h of sleep and reach peak activity at the beginning of the wake period (Gallagher et al, 1973). Many studies have found an inverse relation between delta sleep and pulsatile cortisol release (see Vgontzas et al, 1999; Born et al, 1991 for a review). Normal aging is associated with a concurrent rise in secretory HPA activity and a reduction in delta sleep (Dodt et al, 1994; Feinberg et al, 1967; Gillin et al, 1981; Van Cauter et al, 1996). Aging is also associated with increased cortisol response and more decreases in delta sleep in response to mild laboratory stressors (Prinz et al, 2000). Several studies in which subjects were forced to sleep in the second half of the night when HPA activity is enhanced have shown that delta sleep emerges only during troughs of cortisol activity (Follenius et al, 1992; Orth et al, 1967; Spath-Schwalbe et al, 1992,1993; Van Cauter et al, 1991; Weibel et al, 1995; Weitzman et al, 1974,1983). In the most sophisticated set of studies of peripheral cortisol and delta sleep to date, Gronfier and colleagues calculated secretory rates of cortisol measured every 10 min using a deconvolutional procedure. Delta sleep activity, as measured by spectral analysis, was inversely correlated with cortisol release and the time delay between delta sleep and cortisol pulses was suggestive of the presence of an unmeasured generator, which they hypothesized to be CRF, in the regulation of these rhythms (Gronfier et al, 1998). Finally, Vgontzas et al (2001) demonstrated that peripheral CRF infusion in healthy middle-aged men results in a greater decline in delta sleep than in healthy young men. Other studies of CRF infusion in young men have found either a decrease in delta sleep (Holsboer et al, 1988) or no effect on sleep (Born et al, 1989). Exogenous CRF does not cross the blood–brain barrier (Martins et al, 1996) but can affect periventricular structures, particularly the hypothalamus. Peripheral administration of CRH antagonists that do not cross the blood–brain barrier have been found to reduce spontaneous waking (Chang and Opp, 1999). Collectively, these studies are strong evidence that neurohormonal CRF, separate from neurotransmitter CRF, has a role in sleep–wake regulation.

CRH neurons in the PVN project via the median eminence to the hypothalamo-hypophyseal portal system, which leads to an increase in pituitary ACTH. Theoretically, the decrease in delta sleep could be related to changes in pituitary ACTH or adrenal cortisol release. However, ACTH infusion has been shown to suppress REM but not slow-wave sleep, which argues against the primary role of ACTH in decreasing delta (Born et al, 1989; Gillin et al, 1974a). Further, many studies show that cortisol infusion increases delta sleep (Bohlhalter et al, 1997; Born et al, 1991,1989; Fehm et al, 1986; Hartog, 1964). Thus, the metyrapone effect on delta sleep is not likely to be related to changes in ACTH and cortisol release. One possible mechanism to explain the effect of metyrapone on delta sleep is that the increase in PVN CRF release affects other brain areas involved in sleep or arousal. This possibility is supported by studies that have shown that some PVN CRF neurons project to the locus ceruleus (Aston-Jones et al, 1986; Valentino et al, 1992). For example, Valentino et al (1992) showed that retrograde labeling of CRF terminals in the locus ceruleus originated in the PVN. Support for this mechanism also comes from a study which showed that metyrapone administration results in c-fos induction in the locus ceruleus (Rotllant et al, 2002). CRF projections from the PVN to the locus ceruleus may be a point of integration between neurohormonal and neurotransmitter CRF systems (Valentino et al, 1992). It is possible that stimulation of the locus ceruleus by way of CRF neurons originating in the PVN is of sufficient magnitude to affect a sensitive measure of sleep depth, delta-integrated amplitude, but not of sufficient magnitude to induce anxiety or panic.

In this study, metyrapone caused objective sleep disturbance in the absence of any measurable change in self-report arousal or anxiety. Interestingly, unmedicated patients with Addison's disease have fragmented sleep, which is reversed with glucocorticoid replacement (Garcia-Borreguero et al, 2000). Adrenalectomy leads to increases in CRF activity in the PVN, but decreases in extrahypothalamic areas such as the central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nuclei of the stria terminalis (Watts and Sanchez-Watts, 1995). Patients with Addison's disease who are off glucocorticoids would be expected to have increased CRF activity in the PVN but decreases in extrahypothalamic sites. Hence, Addison's disease is associated with EEG activation but patients are in general not highly symptomatic with anxious arousal. Similarly, metyrapone activates hypothalamic but not extrahypothalamic CRF and the use of this drug is associated with EEG arousal in the absence of increased anxiety. The fact that both neurohormonal and neurotransmitter CRF systems affect sleep and arousal have important implications for understanding insomnia in the context of neuropsychiatric disorders. It is possible for sleep to be disrupted in the absence of anxious arousal states.

In depression, sleep disturbance is highly associated with increased hypothalamic HPA activity (Rush and Weissenburger, 1994). Multiple studies have found that nonsuppression of cortisol with the dexamethasone suppression test (DST) is associated with reduced delta sleep and short REM latency (Asnis et al, 1983; Giles et al, 1987; Hubain et al, 1998; Kerkhofs et al, 1986; Rush et al, 1982). Many studies have reported on the relation between REM latency and DST nonsuppression (reviewed in Rush et al, 1997) without commenting on delta sleep. Several investigators have commented that short REM latency can arise from a deficit in delta sleep (Feinberg and Koresko, 1965; Giles et al, 1998; Kupfer and Ehlers, 1989). The association between hypercortisolemia and reduced delta sleep, frequently observed in depression (Gold et al, 1988a,1988b), is consistent with the hypothesis that increased hypothalamic CRF causes a decrease in delta sleep.

The low baseline delta sleep in PTSD is consistent with a model of increased hypothalamic CRF activity in this group. However, decreased delta sleep can have multiple causes independent of CRF. Delta sleep is regulated by a complex interaction between ascending arousal systems, intrinsic sleep promoting brain areas, and GABAergic inhibitory inputs, which allow for reduced arousal and electrical synchrony of cortical neurons (for a review see Jones, 2000). Decreased delta sleep is also found in schizophrenia, depression, normal aging, and brain atrophy (for a review see Neylan et al, 2003). Although, we suggest that decreased baseline delta sleep in PTSD may in part be related to increases in basal levels of both hypothalamic and extrahypothalamic CRF, it is likely that other mechanisms contribute to this finding.

As previously mentioned, there are several alternative interpretations of the data not involving CRF that should be considered. One possibility that explains both the diminished endocrine and sleep response to metyrapone in PTSD was that there were group differences in the pharmacokinetics of metyrapone. For example, if PTSD subjects for any number of reasons had less bioavailability of metyrapone, then the diminished sleep and endocrine response may be related to less CRF release as opposed to downregulation of CRF receptors. Although there were no significant differences in body mass index in our PTSD and control subjects, it is at least theoretically possible that there were group differences in bioavailability of metyrapone. As previously discussed, the endocrine response to metyrapone was assessed by measuring ACTH and 11 deoxycortisol the morning after metyrapone administration given at habitual sleep onset time. In order to fully understand the effect of the endocrine challenge on delta sleep in PTSD and controls, and address any issues regarding bioavailability of metyrapone, it will be necessary to confirm comparable cortisol suppression across groups by repeated sampling of nocturnal hormone activity during the sleep recordings. In addition, given the known inhibitory effect of CRF on the somatotropic axis (Holsboer et al, 1988; Steiger et al, 1992) and the known relation between delta sleep and growth hormone release (Van Cauter et al, 2000), it will be necessary to measure growth hormone responses in order to understand fully the effect of metyrapone on delta sleep.

In summary, the results from this study demonstrate that metyrapone administration causes an increase in awakenings and marked decrease in quantitative measures of delta sleep, significantly greater in controls compared to PTSD. The decline in delta sleep was significantly associated with the magnitude of increase in both 11-deoxycortisol and ACTH. This suggests that the delta sleep response to metyrapone is a measure of the brain response to a hypothalamic CRF challenge. PTSD subjects demonstrated a diminished endocrine and delta sleep response to metyrapone, which suggests either that PTSD had less CRF release induced by metyrapone or are less responsive to CRF.