INTRODUCTION

Impaired serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) neurotransmission has been implicated in the etiology of a wide range of psychiatric disorders including depression, anxiety, and impulse-control-related disorders. Findings common to some or all of these disorders include reduced cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of the 5-HT metabolite, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, reduced hormonal responses to 5-HT drug challenge, and altered brain 5-HT receptor numbers (Mann et al, 1995; O'Keane et al, 1992; Coccaro et al, 1995; Dolan et al, 2001; Drevets et al, 1999; Stockmeier, 2003). In addition, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) have proved effective in their treatment, an action that is believed to be due to the enhancement of 5-HT neurotransmission (Bell and Nutt, 1998; Mann et al, 2001; Jetty et al, 2001; Nemeroff, 2002).

We, and others, have proposed that alterations in fundamental processes underlying the brain's response to the inhibition of behavior, reinforcement (reward and punishment), and environmental threats could be associated with the development of impulsivity, depression, and anxiety disorders (Deakin and Graeff, 1991; Elliott et al, 2000a). An understanding of the neural basis of these normal neuropsychological processes could prove a useful tool for investigating the abnormal processes involved in psychiatric disorders. Advances in neuroimaging techniques have added substantially to the understanding of neurobiology of emotional processing in humans. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) in conjunction with cognitive activation paradigms, it has been possible to visualize the functional neuroanatomy of specific components of emotional processing such as response to reward and punishment (Elliott et al, 2000b), fear conditioning (Morris et al, 2001), emotion recognition (for a review, see Phan et al, 2002), and impulse control (Casey et al, 1997). Cognitive activation paradigms combined with neuroimaging also offer the opportunity to investigate the role of 5-HT in these processes by studying the modulation of neural responses by drugs (pharmacoMRI or pMRI).

Behavioral inhibition of ongoing behavior is an appropriate response to ‘stop’ signals including aversive stimuli. The inability to inhibit appropriately behavioral responses is a component of impulsivity, which also includes impaired capacity to plan ahead and rapid information processing. In a recent study, we have shown that patients with the diagnosis of borderline and antisocial personality disorder (which is associated with lack of impulse control) activate more widespread prefrontal and temporal cortical areas than healthy controls in the Go/No-go task (Völlm et al, 2004), a neuropsychological task requiring behavioral inhibition (Casey et al, 1997). The Go/No-go task has been widely used as a cognitive activation paradigm with normal volunteers, and it produces consistent prefrontal activation (Horn et al, 2003; Völlm et al, 2004; Rubia et al, 2003; Anderson et al, 2002; Menon et al, 2001; Liddle et al, 2001; Kawashima et al, 1996). The lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), particularly on the right, is a common area activated by No-go compared with Go conditions in these studies and appears to be specifically involved in the inhibition of responses (Aron et al, 2003; Rubia et al, 2003; Menon et al, 2001; Liddle et al, 2001). The right middle frontal gyrus, including dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), also appears to be an important component of behavioral inhibition circuitry (Menon et al, 2001; Garavan et al, 2002). The error detection and processing involved in No-go performance has been associated with the activation of more medial areas of the prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate cortex (Rubia et al, 2003; Garavan et al, 2002; Liddle et al, 2001; Menon et al, 2001; Hester et al, 2004). We have previously shown enhanced neuronal responses in the lateral OFC to Go/No-go performance after the administration of m-chlorophenypiperazine (mCPP), a 5-HT2C agonist (Anderson et al, 2002), consistent with a direct modulatory effect of 5-HT on the neural circuits involved in behavioral inhibition.

From an evolutionary perspective, it has been proposed that the neural substrates involved in the behavioral response to acute and chronic stress, punishment and threat, could be implicated in the neurobiology of anxiety and depressive disorders, with a central role for 5-HT in the modulation of these mental processes (Deakin and Graeff, 1991; Deakin, 2003). We have previously shown that a simple financial reward task activates the amygdala and striatum (Elliott et al, 2003), while financial loss is associated with increased neuronal response in the hippocampus (Elliott et al, 2000b). The OFC has also been implicated in recognition and processing of reward and punishment stimuli, with some findings suggesting an anatomical division of its functions with lateral areas associated with loss/punishment and medial areas more with reward (Bechara et al, 1994; O'Doherty et al, 2001; Elliott et al, 2003). It has been argued that lateral regions of the OFC are critical in responding to punishments signalling a need for behavioral change (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). Depressed patients show abnormal OFC responses to financial rewards and losses (Longe et al, 2003). Previous work in our unit has shown that reducing brain 5-HT function using acute tryptophan depletion increases depressive and anxiety reactions to laboratory stress (Miller et al, 2000; Anderson et al, 2003), and there is preliminary evidence showing 5-HT modulation of the processing of reward and punishment cues involved in decision making (Rogers et al, 2003). Little, however, is known about the functional neuroanatomy of 5-HT involvement in basic processes of reinforcement in humans.

The ability to identify facial expressions and emotions is an important component of social functioning and adaptation. There is evidence for an involvement of dissociable, although interconnected, neural substrates that mediate face emotion processing, with a central role for the amygdala (Haxby et al, 2002). The amygdala has been one of the most consistently activated areas in studies relating to fearful faces (eg Morris et al, 1996; Hariri et al, 2000; Phillips et al, 2001), a response that is lateralized in some studies (eg Morris et al, 1996), possibly relating to different rates of habituation with one study finding faster habituation in the right than left amygdala to repeated exposure (Morris et al, 2001). There are less consistent findings with other emotions such as anger (eg Blair et al, 1999; Hariri et al, 2002) and disgust (Phillips et al, 1997; Winston et al, 2003), perhaps reflecting a stronger reaction to fear than other emotions rather than a truly selective response (Whalen et al, 2001). Previous imaging studies using emotional face stimuli have also shown activation of medial and orbital prefrontal cortices, including anterior cingulate, insula and regions of the occipital cortex, particularly the fusiform gyrus (Sprengelmeyer et al, 1998; Blair et al, 1999; Surguladze et al, 2003; Abel et al, 2003; Keightley et al, 2003). Depressed and anxious patients have been shown to have altered neural responses to negative emotional faces (Lawrence et al, 2004; Thomas et al, 2001; Sheline et al, 2001). Antidepressant treatment has been shown to normalize increased amgydala responses to fearful faces in depressed patients (Sheline et al, 2001; Fu et al, 2004). Acute modulation of 5-HT function alters facial expression recognition in healthy volunteers (Harmer et al, 2003a, 2003b; Attenburrow et al, 2003), but as yet it is unclear as which to neural substrates mediate this effect.

The aim of this study was to determine the acute effects of the SSRI, citalopram, on brain activation during cognitive activation tasks tapping into fundamental neuropsychological processes that could be involved in the pathophysiology of psychiatric diagnosis (behavioral inhibition, reinforcement processing, and covert aversive face emotion recognition).

Specifically, on the basis of the literature reviewed above, we hypothesized that performance of the cognitive tasks would be associated with distinct but overlapping patterns of activation as follows:

  1. 1

    Go/No-go task: Our specific hypotheses concerned prefrontal cortical regions. We predicted that in line with the previous studies discussed, No-go relative to Go blocks would be associated with increased BOLD response in lateral OFC, anterior cingulate, right middle frontal gyrus, and DLPFC.

  2. 2

    Loss task: We hypothesized that the main effect of loss compared to no loss would be associated with neural responses in the lateral OFC and hippocampus.

  3. 3

    Covert recognition of aversive faces: For this task, we predicted increased BOLD response in the amygdala, fusiform gyrus, OFC, and anterior cingulate gyrus.

A response in the OFC was predicted for all three tasks. This is a region that mediates a variety of social, cognitive, and emotional functions, and, as discussed above, appears to play a critical role in all three of the functions assessed here. For each of the tasks, we further predicted that citalopram would modulate the neuronal responses associated with task performamance. Based on previous results, and the clinical profile of the drug, we hypothesized that citalopram would enhance neuronal activation induced by these tasks. We term this approach to imaging the effects of drugs on cognitive activation as ‘indirect’ pharmacological fMRI (pMRI) to distinguish it from direct neuronal activation by drugs (‘direct’ pMRI).

METHODS

Subjects

In all, 12 healthy (10 right handed) male volunteers, aged 19–36 years (mean±SD 24.7±5.8 years) were recruited from students, staff, and the general public, through advertisement.

Subjects were screened using the Overview Module from Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition (SCID-NP; First et al, 2002) and the Mini International Neuropsychiatry Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al, 1998). Exclusion criteria included any serious general medical condition or one that could interfere in the interpretation of results, current or past psychiatric disorder, use of medication within the last 2 weeks, illicit drug use, excessive consumption of alcohol (>21?U/week), caffeine (>8 cups of coffee/day), or cigarette consumption (>10 cigarettes/day), and involvement in concurrent research or in research involving taking an experimental drug in the previous 2 months.

The study was approved by the local Research Ethics Committee and written informed consent was obtained from each volunteer.

Procedures

Subjects were tested on two occasions with placebo (normal saline) or intravenous citalopram 7.5?mg, infused over 7.5?min, separated by at least 3 days (range 3–28 days, mean±SD=11.1±6.9 days), in a randomized, balanced order, single-blind design. The choice of citalopram was based on its high selectivity for 5-HT, evidence from animal and human studies for serotonergic effects, its good tolerability, and its availability as an intravenous preparation (see Discussion). Subjects were cannulated outside the scanner at least 30?min before they received the drug. They then underwent a 22.5?min fMRI scan during which they received the drug infusion in the middle 7.5?min in order to examine the direct effects of citalopram. These results are not presented here. At 5?min after the end of this scan (ie 12.5?min after the end of the drug infusion), they received the first of three further scans during which they undertook the neuropsychological tasks detailed below.

Neuropsychological Tasks

Three tasks of a block-design lasting 6?min each were performed inside the scanner in the order Go/No-go, Loss/No-loss, and covert face emotion recognition.

Go/No-go

This task was based on one reported by Casey et al (1997) and previously used in our Unit (Horn et al, 2003; Anderson et al, 2002). Subjects were presented with a series of letters displayed for 500?ms every 1.73?s on a screen and instructed to respond, using a button box, to all letters apart from ‘V’ by pressing a button. Eight blocks, of 45?s each, were presented in an ABABABAB design, A being the Go condition, with no V's and B the No-go condition, with 50% V's.

Loss/No-loss

This task consisted of six blocks, three associated with loss (2nd, 4th and 6th) and three with no loss (1st, 3rd, and 5th). Subjects were presented with a series of colored squares at a rate of one every 1.33?s. They were instructed to respond only to blue and green squares by pressing a button as fast as they could. Volunteers were instructed that if they were not quick enough to respond on blue squares, they would lose money but they would not lose money on green squares. Losses occurred for 65% of the blue targets and were signalled by pound signs with a cross through them.

Covert Face Emotion Recognition

Subjects were presented with pictures of faces from the Pictures of Facial Affect Series (Ekman and Friesen, 1976). The following types of affect were presented: neutral (A), anger (B), disgust (C), and fear (D). Eight pictures of each emotion was presented for 3?s with an interval of 0.75?s between presentations. Each block lasted 30?s, presented in an ABACADABACAD design. Using a button box, subjects were asked to identify the gender of the faces not to describe or focus on the emotion pictured.

fMRI Scanning

Images were acquired using a Philips (Eindhoven, Holland) 1.5?T Gyroscan ACS NT retrofitted with Powertrak 6000 gradients, operating at a software level 6.1.2 T2*-weighted volumes were acquired using a single-shot echo-planar (EPI) pulse sequence. Each volume comprised 40 contiguous axial slices, (TR/TE=5000/40?ms, 64 × 64 data matrix, 3.5?mm thickness with an in-plane resolution of 3 × 3?mm). A T1-weighted structural scan was also acquired for each subject, for coregistration and to exclude any structural abnormality. No abnormality was reported for any of the 12 subjects.

Analysis

Imaging data were analyzed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM2, Friston, The Welcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK), with a random effects model. Images were realigned to correct for motion artifacts using the first scan as a reference and normalized into the Talairach and Tournoux stereotactic space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) using Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) templates. Images were smoothed with a 10?mm Gaussian kernel to facilitate intersubject averaging. After this spatial preprocessing, first-level analysis was performed on each subject using the general linear model with a delayed boxcar waveform to model blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal changes to generate a single mean image corresponding to each experiment. These images were combined in a second-level, random effects, analysis, using a one-way ANOVA to investigate the main effect of the task. This main effect was used as an inclusive mask to examine which of the regions responsive to the task showed a significant modulation by the drug relative to placebo (ie an interaction between task and drug, masked by the main effect of task). It is possible for significant interactions to occur outside areas where a main effect is seen, but it is then difficult to interpret their functional significance.

For the three tasks, neural responses in the control blocks were subtracted from those in the active task blocks, to reveal areas of increased signal associated with performance of the task, irrespective of drug pretreatment. The active minus control comparisons were No-Go minus Go, Loss minus No-loss, and aversive emotion minus neutral in the three tasks, respectively. In the Loss/No-loss task, the reverse subtraction was also examined. In the analysis of the covert face emotion recognition task, the combined aversive faces analysis compared with neutral faces is presented in order to identify brain areas responding to external threat in a generic sense rather than specific emotions. Within areas showing a main effect of task, neural responses on placebo were subtracted from those on citalopram to identify areas where citalopram enhanced neural activation to the task, with the reverse subtraction used to identify where citalopram attenuated responses. Statistical maps were thresholded at p<0.001 uncorrected (Z-score>3.09) with only cluster sizes of 10 or more contiguous voxels being reported. Small volume corrections (Worsley et al, 1996) were applied to a priori regions of interest, as specified in the introduction. These regions, and the volume of correction, were chosen on the basis of previous studies reported by ourselves and others (eg Morris et al, 1996; Hariri et al, 2000; Phillips et al, 2001; Elliott et al, 2003; Horn et al, 2003). In addition, lower levels of significance are reported when bilateral activations were seen. We also report regions where activation was observed significant at p<0.001 uncorrected. However, these regions are included for descriptive purposes only and are not discussed further.

RESULTS

Citalopram administration was very well tolerated and there were no significant differences between visual analog self-ratings of mood, discomfort, nausea, and sedation/alertness taken before and after the scanner tasks. Behavioral results for one subject were not available because of a technical problem.

Go/No-go Task

There were no differences between occasion in the number of omission errors (not responding to letters apart from V; mean±SD: 0.3±0.5% for placebo, 0.2±0.8% for citalopram, p=0.9) or commission errors (responding to V's; 11.2±4.8% for placebo, 10.7±6.6% for citalopram, p=0.7). Subjects were slightly slower to respond on the citalopram occasion (reaction time on placebo 387±28?ms, 406±24?ms on citalopram, p=0.05).

The main effects of task and their modulation by citalopram are represented in Table 1 and Figure 1. During the No-go blocks (behavioral inhibition) significant neuronal responses after small volume correction on the basis of prior hypothesis were observed in the right DLPFC, OFC bilaterally, and right middle frontal gyrus. Additional responses, significant at p<0.001 uncorrected, were observed in right temporal cortex, bilateral supramarginal gyri, right precuneus, and right thalamus. Citalopram enhanced the activation of DLPFC, right lateral OFC, and middle temporal gyrus. Attenuations were seen in OFC, more on the left, bilateral supramarginal gyri, and right superior temporal gyrus.

Table 1 Maximally Activated Voxels in Areas in which Significant Evoked Activity was Related to No-go Block Compared to Go Block Levels
Figure 1
figure 1

Modulation of task-related activations by citalopram. (a) Enhancement of right BA47 activation in No-go compared with Go blocks. (b) Attenuation of left>right BA11 activation in No-go compared with Go blocks. (c) Attenuation of right BA11 activation in No-loss compared with Loss blocks. (d) Attenuation of right BA47 and right amygdala activation in Aversive compared with Neutral face blocks.

Loss/No-loss Task

Subjects performed near ceiling on this task with very few commission or omission errors and no difference in reaction time between the two occasions (data not shown).

As shown in Table 2, the main effects of the Loss compared with No-loss blocks in prehypothesised regions were observed bilaterally in lateral OFC. Additional responses significant at p<0.001 uncorrrected were observed in bilateral frontopolar prefrontal cortex, and parietal cortex, predominantly left temporal cortex and right fusiform gyrus. Citalopram decreased the response in right fusiform gyrus (Table 2).

Table 2 Maximally Activated Voxels in Areas in which Significant Evoked Activity was Related to Loss and No-Loss Blocks Levels

In the No-loss compared with Loss blocks, increased hemodynamic responses were seen in bilateral medial OFC, right anterior cingulate gyrus, right insula, mainly left temporal cortex, left occipital cortex, predominantly right parahippocampal gyrus, right hippocampus, and uncus. Citalopram increased the activation in right insula and left occipital cortex. The activation of right medial OFC was attenuated by citalopram (Table 2, Figure 1c).

Covert Face Emotion Recognition

Subjects performed near ceiling on this task with very few incorrect responses or omission errors and no difference in reaction time between the two occasions (data not shown).

Table 3 shows that aversive faces, when compared with neutral faces, caused bilateral neuronal activation in hypothesized regions of lateral OFC, fusiform gyri, and amygdala/amygdaloid complex. Additional responses, significant at p<0.001 uncorrected, were observed in DLPFC, superior temporal cortices, occipital gyri, and thalami. Citalopram tended to enhance the activation in fusiform gyri and thalami bilaterally, whereas it attenuated the activation in the right lateral OFC and right amygdala (Figure 1d).

Table 3 Maximally Activated Voxels in Areas in which Significant Evoked Activity was Related to the Covert Recognition of Aversive Faces Blocks Compared to the Recognition of Neutral Faces Blocks

DISCUSSION

We have studied regional neuronal activation, and its modulation by 5-HT, in three paradigms selected to probe neuropsychological processes related to impulsivity, depression, and anxiety, namely behavioral inhibition, loss, and viewing of aversive faces. The main effects of the tasks in this study (ie independent of drug modulation) have confirmed the involvement of prefrontal and subcortical regions, which in general are consistent with other findings in the literature (Casey et al, 1997; Rubia et al, 2003; Horn et al, 2003; Haxby et al, 2002). Acute infusion of the SSRI, citalopram, modulated brain responses in a task-specific manner with only modest overlap. This supports a role for 5-HT in these process, but one that is crucially dependent on the nature of the psychological task involved.

Go/No-go Task

Some studies have shown impaired performance in this task related to poor impulse control (Dolan and Park, 2002), although some negative results have also been reported (Dinn and Harris, 2000). Given the inverse relationship that has been found between 5-HT function and impulsivity (eg Dolan et al, 2001), it is of interest that subjects tended to respond to targets more slowly under citalopram; however, there was no effect on the number of commission errors. We found that citalopram enhanced right OFC activation during No-go blocks, a similar finding to our previous result with the 5-HT2C agonist, mCPP (Anderson et al, 2002), supporting a modulatory role for 5-HT in this region, probably 5-HT2C receptor mediated. Citalopram also increased activation of other right-sided areas including DLPFC, an area that has been implicated in working memory load in this task (Mostofsky et al, 2003). In contrast citalopram attenuated more medial OFC and superior temporal cortex/supramarginal gyrus. These findings are consistent with a role for 5-HT in the effective inhibition of unwanted behaviors and suggest a direct involvement in the circuitry involved in withholding responses. The attenuation by citalopram of activation in areas that may be involved in error processing and attention/spatial awareness (Karnath, 2001) suggest a different role for 5-HT in this circuitry.

Loss/No-loss Task

Understanding the results obtained in the Loss/No-loss task is complicated by features of the task itself that became apparent after the study. Although the No-loss blocks were designed as neutral blocks, given the subtraction design in relation to the Loss bocks, they could be perceived as associated with not losing, or relative reward. In addition, within the Loss Block, there was an expectation of losing for about 2 3 of the time, and also the experience of avoiding loss for the rest of the time. Learning theory has long regarded omission of anticipated punishment as functionally equivalent to reward. Indeed, subjects might have consciously experienced omission of loss as mildly relieving or as a pleasurable consequence of their own quick reactions.

Given the caveats and this interpretation of the No-loss condition, the areas showing main effects of task are compatible with areas identified in the literature. As predicted by previous studies of financial loss (eg O'Doherty et al, 2001), activation of lateral areas of OFC was more pronounced during the Loss blocks than during the No-loss blocks. The opposite subtraction showed medial OFC and right anterior cingulate activation. This was not a planned comparison and therefore we did not have a prior hypothesis concerning this effect. However, the findings are consistent with what we would have predicted based on the interpretation that No-loss is relative rewarding compared with Loss. O'Doherty et al (2001) found activation of the medial OFC to financial reward. Other studies have also reported medial OFC responses to rewarding stimuli (see Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004, for a review).

In a previous study, involving financial loss in a choice paradigm, Elliott et al (2000b) observed bilateral hippocampal activation and we expected to see the same. However, we observed no hippocampal activation; indeed, right hippocampus was activated in the No-loss condition. The explanation is not obvious, but may relate to different contexts of the tasks.

Citalopram attenuated response to loss in right fusiform gyrus (see above). A possible implication is that 5-HT decreases attention to, or processing of, visual stimuli that predict an aversive outcome that cannot be avoided. Some enhancement of the right OFC response was observed, as in the No-go task, but this fell short of statistical significance (z=2.38). Our expectation had been that citalopram might modulate prefrontal and temporal effects of loss given the experimental evidence for a role of 5-HT in anxiety and responses to punishment (Graeff, 2002) as well as clinical relevance in treating mood and anxiety disorders (Bell and Nutt, 1998; Anderson, 2001). It is possible that the loss aspect of this task was insufficiently potent or relevant to individuals to engage circuits involved in emotion, or that the task itself was confounded as discussed above.

Citalopram attenuated right medial OFC activations in the No-loss blocks suggesting a possible modulation of circuits involved in reward. Citalopram enhanced occipital activation, which is consistent with 5-HT enhancement of early-stage visual information processing as suggested by a previous finding that fenfluramine increased critical flicker fusion threshold (Andrews and Anderson, 1998).

Covert Face Emotion Recognition

Since first described by Darwin (1872), facial expressions of basic emotions have been considered a key component of emotion processing. A considerable number of imaging studies involving pictures of face emotion have been conducted with evidence to support the idea that different emotions processes involve separate neural systems. Nevertheless, there are also findings suggesting a common pathway to facial expression processing, involving amygdala and prefrontal areas, especially the lateral OFC. We do not report separate analyses for different negative expressions in this paper, but concentrate on the common effects of negative facial emotions since we have speculated that 5-HT modulates responses to aversion.

Consistent with others' findings we found activation of the parahippocampal gyrus bilaterally extending into the amygdala in response to aversive faces. Amygdala activation has been reported in a number of studies to be activated by a range of emotional faces (Winston et al, 2003; Yang et al, 2002) and by novel compared with familiar faces (Schwartz et al, 2003), suggesting that the amygdala may be activated by stimuli of potential significance or threat to the individual.

Aversive faces also activated posterior OFC on the right, lateral frontal cortex, and temporal poles on both sides, occipital and extrastriate regions, particularly fusiform gyrus, again broadly consistent with other studies (Keightley et al, 2003; Lange et al, 2003; Gur et al, 2002; Abel et al, 2003; Surguladze et al, 2003; Sprengelmeyer et al, 1998).

Acute citalopram decreased activation in the right lateral OFC and right parahippocampal/amygdala region (with small volume correction). Increases in fusiform gyri and thalamic responses were bilateral, but individually below the threshold for significance.

Behavioral studies with acute citalopram and tryptophan depletion and supplementation (Harmer et al, 2003a, 2003b; Attenburrow et al, 2003) report increased recognition of fearful faces associated with increased 5-HT function, in women. Subchronic citalopram administration in contrast was associated with decreased recognition of fearful faces (Harmer et al, 2004). These findings are consistent with the proposal that acute increases in 5-HT can be associated with increased anxiety (Mortimore and Anderson, 2000; Hetem et al, 1996), and that decreased anxiety and therapeutic efficacy result from adaptive changes in the 5-HT system with continued treatment (Bell and Nutt, 1998). Our findings suggest that 5-HT attenuates the processing of emotion in faces in limbic structures. However, this further implies that the heightened detection of fear in faces induced by citalopram (Harmer et al, 2003a) is not mediated by 5-HT modulation of amygdala function. Our results suggest it may be due to enhancement of visual processing in fusiform cortex and thalamic attentional mechanisms. However, as discussed above, the equation of activation with functional activity is not straightforward and we did not record performance on face emotion identification. Therefore, we do not know whether our subjects would have behavioral data comparable with Harmer et al (2003a). Indeed, the latter study involved female subjects, whereas the present study was carried out in males. Attenburrow et al (2003), using tryptophan supplementation, found increased sensitivity to recognition of fearful faces in women but not in men, suggesting there may be a differential sex effect. There is also evidence of an inverse relationship between activation in hippocampal/amygdala regions compared with prefrontal areas dependent on the degree of conscious identification of emotion (Hariri et al, 2002; Lange et al, 2003), with conscious labelling increasing prefrontal activation. It is therefore possible that 5-HT is involved in processes related to the bringing of aversive emotions to conscious awareness.

Of interest, a recent study in depressed patients reported increased left hemisphere responses to sad faces compared with controls including amygdalar/parahippocampal area, ventral striatum, caudate, thalamus, and dorsal cingulate gyrus. Treatment for 8 weeks with the SSRI fluoxetine led to a reduction in responses, that did not, however, correlate with symptomatic improvement, although an increase in range of haemodynamic response in the anterior cingulate gyrus was associated with response (Fu et al, 2004). While not directly comparable with our paradigm, these results do support a modulation of amydgala responses to face emotion by 5-HT.

Comparison of Citalopram Modulation in Different Task-Related Activations

It is striking that right lateral OFC activations were seen in all three tasks in this experiment. This area receives polymodal sensory inputs and is involved in the lateral orbitofrontal–basal ganglia loop (Zald and Kim, 2001). Our findings are in keeping with the proposal that right BA47 is engaged in situations were a behavioral strategy requires reappraisal and behavioral inhibition because outcomes change or become aversive (Elliott et al, 2000a; Wallis et al, 2001). Acquired lesions of this region in humans interferes with response inhibition in the Stop paradigm, a type of No-go task (Aron et al, 2003). Only right-sided lesions have this effect and this is compatible with more pronounced right-sided effects in our study and enhancement of right BA47 activation by citalopram, both of which suggest a genuine lateralization of function.

It is interesting that citalopram enhanced this activation in the Go/No-Go task but attenuated the response to aversive faces. We also observed a tendency to enhance the response in the loss task which failed to reach significance. One possible explanation is that 5-HT promotes activity in this region when a behavioral response is required, such as response inhibition, but when this response selection system is engaged by aversive processes, without behavioral implications, citalopram attenuates the response.

Medial OFC (BA11) activation was seen in both the Go/No-go tasks and the No-loss condition of the monetary task; however, there was little overlap between the activated voxels. This general region is part of the medial orbitofrontal–basal ganglia loop with connections with the anterior cingulate cortex, ventral striatum, medial temporal structures including the amygdala, and hypothalamus (Zald and Kim, 2001). It appears to be engaged by reinforcing stimuli when there is a stimulus discrimination component to the task, and this may have a functional similarity with error-checking in the Go/No-go task. In both tasks, citalopram attenuated the response, but the implications for the role of 5-HT are not obvious.

We designed our tasks to be very simple to minimize performance differences or effects of difficulty, which could have confounded interpretation of results if citalopram had affected these. We acknowledge that this can also be seen as a limitation of the study as it was not designed to explore to what extent these might explain the drug effects seen, or differences between tasks. While we believe this is unlikely in the current study, a challenge for future pMRI studies will be to explore the relationship between drug effects on neuronal activation and neuropsychological aspects such as subjective state, perceived difficulty and performance.

Methodological Considerations

It is important to consider difficulties inherent in the interpretation of BOLD signal modulation following drug administration. These include the effects of the drug on 5-HT function, nonspecific effects of the drug on BOLD signal, and the interpretation of increased or decreased ‘activation’ identified by altered BOLD signal.

Microdialysis studies in animal have consistently shown acute increases in cortical extracellular 5-HT following acute SSRI administration (David et al, 2003; Felton et al, 2003; Moret and Briley, 1996). However, animal studies have typically given doses 10–100 times higher than the low-dose (about 0.1?mg/kg) we administered (eg Felton et al, 2003). Our dose was determined by considerations of safety and tolerability in acute intravenous infusion because we also wanted to examine the direct neural effects of citalopram (direct pMRI, data not presented here). There is evidence that increases in extracellular 5-HT following SSRIs are greater in the raphe nuclei than the cortex (Bel and Artigas, 1992). It is therefore possible that acute low dose citalopram could preferentially inhibit 5-HT reuptake in cell body, rather than terminal, areas. This would lead to reduced neuronal firing rate through somatodendritic autoreceptor activation (eg Gartside et al, 1995), and hence lower terminal 5-HT release and lower synaptic concentrations. However, two lines of evidence led us to believe that this dose was sufficient to increase terminal synaptic 5-HT concentrations. First, neuroendocrine challenge with intravenous citalopram at the low dose of 5?mg has been shown to result in plasma hormonal responses (Attenburrow et al, 2001), and in this study, we also found significant increases in prolactin and cortisol concentrations (data not presented). Second, the effect of acute oral citalopram on face emotion recognition is opposite to that seen with acute tryptophan depletion and similar to that seen after tryptophan supplementation (Harmer et al, 2003a, 2003b; Attenburrow et al, 2003). Nevertheless, we acknowledge that there remains some uncertainty about the net effect of acute SSRI administration in our study, which must be considered in interpreting the data. It is possible that higher citalopram doses than used here might have resulted in a wider, or different, pattern of modulation. In addition, the effect of postsynaptic 5-HT receptor stimulation is often inhibitory, particularly when 5-HT1A receptors are involved, so it is also difficult to know whether enhanced (or reduced) 5-HT function will lead to regional neuronal activation or inhibition.

The possibility of nonspecific effects of citalopram also need to be considered (eg increasing blood flow in activated regions), but we would argue that our findings of areas of decreased BOLD signal, and opposite effects in the same region depending on task, argue against this and for a task-specific modulation by 5-HT. A further question in interpreting pharmacological modulation of neuropsychological tasks using fMRI is what the changes in brain activation mean in terms of the underlying neuronal processes or circuitry. Increases in BOLD signal are believed to be an indirect measure of increased neuronal metabolism, but does an increase reflect neurones being more activated because the task is being performed better, or because more cells have to be recruited to achieve the same level of function? Other uncertainties lie in the interpretation of block design analyses where the comparison is based on a subtraction of BOLD signal in the two conditions. Relative increase in BOLD signal in one type of block compared with another could be due to the activation in that block or attenuation in the comparator block. In addition, block designs are insensitive to, and may be confounded by, changes in response over the time of testing or between blocks. For example, rapid habituation of amygdala activation to faces is well described (Fischer et al, 2003; Thomas et al, 2001) and could lead to activation following initial stimulus presentation not being detected because it is outweighed in the overall analysis by lack of activation subsequently.

Bearing these limitations in mind, we believe that the results do start to build a picture of 5-HT modulation of neuronal circuitry involved in basic psychological processes.

CONCLUSIONS

We were interested in both similarities and differences in the effect of citalopram on activations in the three tasks, which were chosen to probe aspects of psychological functioning that might underlie areas of psychopathology in which 5-HT is believed to be involved. The tasks did show different patterns of activation, although there was a common theme of OFC involvement. Our results support the involvement of 5-HT in each of these processes, although the activations occurring in Loss/No-loss task are difficult to interpret in a straightforward way. A major conclusion from our study is the task-specific nature of citalopram modulation of neuronal activation. This is consistent with the complexity implied by the widespread innervation by 5-HT neurones and the involvement of 5-HT in a wide variety of different functions.