Abstract
Cultured hippocampal neurons were used as a model system to address experimentally the spatial and temporal sequence leading to the appropriate sorting of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic terminals to different cellular target domains and the role of neural activity in this process. By using antibodies against glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 (GAD65) and synaptophysin, we examined the development and segregation of GABAergic and non-GABAergic synaptic terminals on single neurons. Electron microscopy confirmed that GAD65-labeled swellings observed using light microscopy corresponded to synaptic boutons. From the time at which GABAergic terminals first appeared, they developed at a more rapid rate on neuronal somata than non-GABAergic terminals did, such that by 18 d in culture, 60% of the total boutons on somata were GABAergic. By contrast, the majority (70%) of boutons on dendrites were non-GABAergic. These data suggest that inhibitory synaptic terminals are targeted preferentially to or maintained on cell somata at the expense of excitatory terminals. Interestingly, non-GABAergic terminals were not inhibited from forming synapses on cell somata, because in the absence of GABAergic terminals they attained the same total somatic terminal density seen in the presence of GABAergic terminals. Chronic blockade of neuronal activity did not affect the differential targeting of GABAergic and non-GABAergic axons; however, it did reduce the extent of dendritic arborization. Our findings support a two-step model for synaptic segregation whereby the majority of terminals is initially targeted in an activity-independent manner to the appropriate cellular domains, but an additional developmental mechanism serves to further restrict and refine the original synaptic distribution.
Excitatory and inhibitory synaptic terminals are differentially distributed on their postsynaptic target cells. On hippocampal and neocortical pyramidal cells, excitatory synapses outnumber inhibitory synapses, but they predominate on distal dendrites and dendritic spines (Gray, 1959; Andersen et al., 1966; Beaulieu and Colonnier, 1985). In contrast, inhibitory synapses predominate on cell somata and dendritic shafts and, although numerically fewer, are positioned near the site of action-potential generation to strongly influence neuronal output (Gray, 1959; Blackstad and Flood, 1963;Hamlyn, 1963; Andersen et al., 1964; Uchizono, 1968). Such spatial segregation of functionally distinct synapses is critical for normal integrative properties of synaptic neurotransmission (Rall, 1970). The sequence through which such functionally distinct types of synaptic terminals become properly sorted on their target neurons is unknown, nor is it known what factors play a role in regulating synaptic sorting, such as chemical or electrical signals.
Several lines of evidence indicate that axons in both vertebrates and invertebrates find their target areas by following pathways defined by gradients of chemical signals (Jessell, 1988; Glanzman et al., 1991;Broadie, 1994; Goodman, 1994; Tessier-Lavigne, 1994). The signaling molecules can be attractants or repellents, secreted by targets, or bound to substrates. Although some axonal growth cones may be directed precisely by specific signals to their final synaptic targets (Bate, 1976; Ready and Nicholls, 1979; Bastiani and Goodman, 1986; Zoran et al., 1990), many axons terminate in initially widespread arbors, which in response to competition from overlapping inputs are pruned and remodeled (Goodman and Shatz, 1993; Hall and Sanes, 1993). It is not known whether axons arising from excitatory and inhibitory neurons precisely target different cellular domains or whether they innervate their targets nonselectively and undergo subsequent rearrangement. Distinguishing between these two developmental strategies is a critical first step in determining the mechanisms underlying synaptic sorting.
One obvious way in which excitatory and inhibitory axon terminals might be distinguished from one another is by the different neurotransmitters they use. In the developing CNS, competition between afferents and the resultant highly ordered connections are often mediated by activity-dependent neurotransmitter release (Stryker and Harris, 1986;Shatz and Stryker, 1988; Fox et al., 1996). Thus, activity-dependent excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission with subsequent interactions between different neurotransmitters and their specific receptors may play a role in the segregation of glutamatergic and GABAergic terminals to relatively separate cellular domains.
Cultured hippocampal neurons, which consist only of pyramidal (glutamatergic) neurons and GABAergic interneurons, provide a relatively simple system in which to examine the strategies used to sort excitatory and inhibitory synapses to different cellular domains (Benson et al., 1994; Craig et al., 1994). In culture, basket-like GABAergic terminals form on somata of pyramidal neurons, and non-GABAergic terminals form on dendritic spines, strongly suggesting that excitatory and inhibitory synapses segregate appropriately (Craig et al., 1993; Benson et al., 1994). Synapses identified by the immunocytochemical localization of synaptic vesicle proteins can be analyzed and quantified by light microscopy (Fletcher et al., 1991;Benson et al., 1994). Those synapses that are GABAergic can be identified by antibodies against glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 (GAD65) (Benson et al., 1994), an isoform of the GABA-synthesizing enzyme GAD, which is targeted to synaptic vesicles (Kaufman et al., 1991; Reetz et al., 1991; Shi et al., 1994).
In the present study, we used cultured hippocampal neurons to determine the spatial and temporal sequence through which GABAergic and non-GABAergic synaptic terminals become targeted differentially to their proper cellular domains, and we investigated the role of neural activity in initiating this process.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Cultures. Hippocampi were dissected from 18-d-old, fetal Sprague Dawley rats, and cell cultures were prepared as described previously (Banker and Cowan, 1977; Goslin and Banker, 1991). Cells were dissociated by treatment with 0.25% trypsin for 15 min at 37°C, followed by trituration through a fire-polished Pasteur pipette. The cells were plated at a density of 50,000 cells per 60 mm plastic Petri dish on poly-l-lysine-coated glass coverslips in minimum essential media (MEM) (Biotechnologies, Grand Island, NY) containing 10% horse serum. After the cells had attached (about 4 hr), the coverslips were transferred to dishes containing a monolayer of cortical astroglia in MEM containing N2 supplements (Bottenstein and Sato, 1979), sodium pyruvate (1 mm) (Sigma, St. Louis, MO), and ovalbumin (0.1%) (Sigma). After 3 d, cytosine arabinoside (5 μm) (Sigma) was added to inhibit proliferation of glial cells. Neurons were maintained for up to 5 weeks by exchanging one third of the media once a week.
Immunocytochemistry. Cell cultures were fixed at 4, 11, and 18 d in vitro with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS, pH 7.2, containing 0.12 M sucrose, for 30 min at 37°C. Cells were permeabilized with 0.25% Triton X-100 for 5 min, and nonspecific binding sites were blocked with a solution of 10% bovine serum albumin (BSA) in PBS. Cells were simultaneously incubated in two primary antibodies diluted in 1% BSA in PBS at 4°C overnight: anti-GAD-6, a mouse monoclonal antibody that recognizes the 65 kDa form of GAD [Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, Iowa City, IA (Chang and Gottlieb, 1988)], and anti-synaptophysin, an affinity-purified rabbit polyclonal (Navone et al., 1986) (kindly provided by P. De Camilli, Yale University Medical School) (diluted 1:8000). Antibody binding was visualized by incubating cells with a biotinylated anti-mouse secondary antibody, followed by fluorescein-labeled streptavidin (both from Vector, Burlingame, CA) (diluted 1:500), and an anti-rabbit rhodamine-labeled secondary antibody (Boehringer Mannheim Biochemicals, Indianapolis, IN) (diluted 1:200), a combination that yielded the highest signal-to-noise ratio for both antibodies. For analysis of dendritic arbors, some sister cultures were incubated with an antibody against MAP2 (AP14; Binder et al., 1986) (kindly provided by E. Torre, University of Virginia Medical School). For all of the immunocytochemical procedures, control cultures were incubated with 2% nonimmune serum or 1% BSA in place of primary antibody. In addition, when two antibodies were used simultaneously, staining was compared with that obtained in cultures incubated with anti-GAD65 or anti-synaptophysin alone.
Tetrodotoxin (TTX) exposure. Cultured hippocampal neurons were exposed continuously to the sodium channel blocker TTX (Sigma) (1 μm) for 15 d, beginning from day 3 (just before the formation of synapses). When renewed every 3 d, this concentration of TTX has proven effective in eliminating action potentials during prolonged periods in neurons cultured according to the protocol used in the present study (Van Huizen et al., 1985; Ramakers et al., 1993;Craig et al., 1994; Verderio et al., 1994; D. Benson, unpublished observations).
Light microscopy. Immunostaining was visualized using a Zeiss Axiophot microscope equipped for the visualization of fluorescence and integrated with an image analysis system (NeuroZoom, Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, and Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, NY). Neurons were visualized through a 40× or 63× objective, and images were captured using an Optronix CCD camera and transferred to either a DEC or Macintosh computer workstation. The localization of presynaptic specializations labeled by antibodies against synaptophysin or GAD65 were mapped directly onto the image, and positions were stored as coordinates. Most neuronal processes in culture were thin enough to be visualized in a single optical plane and could therefore be captured in a single frame, but the relative thickness of the cell soma frequently necessitated the collection of images from several focal planes. In this way the total number of presynaptic terminals and the percentage of terminals containing GAD65 were determined. To ensure that we were counting all labeled terminals, we carried out pilot studies using a confocal laser scanning microscope (Zeiss LSM410). Although focus was sharper with the confocal microscope, the same number of labeled terminals was clearly visible when a conventional microscope was used.
A modified Scholl analysis (Scholl, 1953) was used to compare quantitatively the relative positions of non-GABAergic and GABAergic presynaptic terminals. A line was traced around the cell soma, and two concentric circles of increasing radius were drawn around it. Because neurons of increasing age were also increasing in size, the radii of the concentric circles were adjusted systematically such that the dendritic tree was divided into three roughly equal regions: proximal, middle, and distal. The distance between the circles was determined by measuring rays emanating from the edge of the soma to the most distal terminal branch for each primary dendrite, taking the average length, and dividing by 3 (Fig. 1). The percentage of total dendritic length contained within proximal regions averaged 34% (±2), within the middle regions, 29% (±2), and within the distal regions, 37% (±2). The number of synapses that fell within each region was counted. Dendritic arbors were mapped using synaptophysin staining and phase-contrast microscopy or sister cultures that were immunostained for MAP2 (Benson et al., 1994). For each neuron, we determined the number of primary branches arising from the cell soma, the total length and number of dendritic segments, and the number of terminal branches. At least 10 cells per experiment and at least 30 cells per age were examined in this manner, unless noted otherwise. Significant differences were detected by ANOVA, and the sources of differences were detected using Scheffé’s F procedure for post hoccomparisons.
Electron microscopy. To assess GAD and GABA immunolocalization, we used a method modified from that of Caserta and Barker (1994). Eleven- and 18-d-old neural cultures were fixed in a 2% paraformaldehyde/0.12 M sucrose/0.1% glutaraldehyde/PBS solution. Cells were then treated with 0.5 mg/ml sodium borohydride, permeabilized with 0.02% saponin in PBS, and blocked with 10% BSA in PBS. Cells were incubated overnight at 4°C in undiluted anti-GAD65 supernatant. Labeling was detected using the Vector “Elite” avidin–biotin kit with diaminobenzidine as chromogen. The cells were postfixed in 1% glutaraldehyde and washed in PBS. The cells were then treated with 2% osmium tetroxide, dehydrated through graded ethanols, and embedded using propylene oxide and epon. Once the epon polymerized the cells, they were removed from the cover glass using liquid nitrogen. Ultrathin sections were cut and mounted on copper grids and then stained by uranyl acetate and lead citrate.
RESULTS
Normal maturational sequence of cultured hippocampal neurons
Neurons that were cultured for 4, 11, and 18 d were used for most of the experiments described in this study. Each of these ages represents a different developmental stage with regard to the formation of synapses (Fletcher et al., 1991). At 4 d, axons have emerged from cell somata, dendrites are beginning to form, and synaptogenesis has just begun. At 11 d, synapses are forming rapidly and dendrites are significantly more mature, but dendritic spines have not yet formed. By 18 d in culture, neurons have well developed axonal and dendritic arbors, and pyramidal-type neurons have elaborated dendritic spines.
Identification of GABAergic and non-GABAergic terminals
To identify GABAergic and non-GABAergic synaptic terminals, cultures were immunostained simultaneously with antibodies against GAD65 and synaptophysin (Fletcher et al., 1991; Benson et al., 1994). Synaptophysin immunolabeling identified the total number of presynaptic terminals, whereas GAD65 identified those that were also GABAergic. By light microscopy, GAD65-containing boutons apposed to dendrites were in precise register with synaptophysin-labeled boutons. Electron microscopy confirmed that in both 11- and 18-d-old cultures, GAD65-labeled boutons apposing dendritic membranes were presynaptic. Labeled axon terminals that contained clusters of vesicles were apposed to a slightly thickened postsynaptic density (Fig. 2). GAD65-labeled synaptic boutons were never observed to appose dendritic spines.
At 4 d in culture, the light microscopic localization of both GAD65 and synaptophysin immunolabeling was most prominent in a region of cytoplasm adjacent to the nucleus that is most likely to be the Golgi apparatus. GAD65 label also extended diffusely into the surrounding cytoplasm, but was rarely observed in brightly stained granules that might be indicative of vesicles (Fig.3A). By contrast, synaptophysin label was observed in granules that have been demonstrated to be vesicles by electron microscopy (Fletcher et al., 1991). Labeled vesicles extended into axons and clustered infrequently in presynaptic terminal boutons (Fig. 3B). By 7 d in culture, GAD65 and synaptophysin immunolabeling were prominent in bouton-like structures, and synaptophysin was no longer prominent in the soma (not shown). At 11 and 18 d in culture, presynaptic terminals were clearly labeled with both GAD65 and synaptophysin staining (Fig. 3D–E). These data closely concur with what has been reported previously (Fletcher et al., 1991; Benson et al., 1994; Solimena et al., 1994).
Rate of formation of synapses
At 4 d in culture, there was an average of 22 synaptophysin-labeled boutons on the soma and dendrites of neurons receiving synaptic input, but many neurons had none. The number of synaptophysin-labeled terminals did not correlate with somal size, which was approximated by area. The number of boutons per micrometer increased linearly over time and reached an average of 6.1/10 μm at 18 d (Fig. 4A). The average rate of synaptophysin-labeled bouton development was similar to that of the overall increase in dendritic length, although the rate of dendritic outgrowth exceeded that of synapse development between 11 and 18 d in culture (p < 0.04; Fig. 4). Dendritic length increased more by addition of new branches than by lengthening of existing branches.
The development of GAD65-labeled boutons followed that of synaptophysin. At 4 d in culture, >93% of the neurons were not contacted by GAD65-labeled boutons, but two of the neurons examined had two or three GAD65-positive boutons. At 11 d in culture, ∼68% of the neurons were contacted by GAD65-labeled boutons, with an average of 1.6 boutons/10 μm. At 18 d in culture, all but 15% of the neurons were contacted by GAD65 boutons, and there was an average of 1.8 boutons/10 μm; however, the neurons lacking GAD65 boutons do not seem to be incapable of receiving such inputs, because by 4 weeks in culture, all neurons were contacted by GAD65-labeled boutons. Although GAD65-labeled terminals were found throughout the somatodendritic domain, they appeared more clustered around cell somata.
Segregation of terminals to cellular domains
To determine how GAD65-positive and -negative synaptic terminals were distributed along the somatodendritic domain, we mapped the location of GAD65- and synaptophysin-containing terminals using computer software that would assign each identified bouton anx,y,z coordinate. The resulting synaptic terminal maps were divided into four areas: the first was the cell soma, and the latter three—proximal, middle, and distal—represented relatively equal divisions of the dendritic tree (see Materials and Methods).
At 4 d in culture, there were extremely few GABAergic terminals on any neuron, and these were mostly on dendrites. In addition, synaptophysin-immunoreactive terminals were evenly distributed across somal, proximal, middle, and distal regions. By 7 d in culture, the numbers of both GABAergic and non-GABAergic terminals had increased over all regions of their target neurons. In comparing the distribution of GABAergic and non-GABAergic terminals, there was a trend toward having a greater percentage of GABAergic terminals on cell somata (38%) than on dendrites (18%), although the difference was not statistically significant (n = 10, p < 0.3). By 11 d in culture, the percentages of synaptic terminals on cell somata that were GABAergic or non-GABAergic were approximately equal (51 vs 49%), whereas in all dendritic regions, the percentage of terminals that were GABAergic lagged significantly behind that which were nonGABAergic (Fig. 5A). By 18 d in culture, the percentage of synaptic terminals that were GABAergic had increased on cell somata such that it exceeded the percentage of non-GABAergic somatic terminals by 33%. On all dendritic regions, the percentage of synaptic terminals that were GABAergic was less than half that which were non-GABAergic (Fig. 5B). The percentage of total synaptic terminals that were GABAergic decreased sharply over a proximal-to-distal gradient whereas the percentage that was non-GABAergic increased.
At 18 d in culture, of the total number of GABAergic terminals, 12% were on somata and 88% were on dendrites. In contrast, 4% of non-GABAergic terminals were on somata, and 96% were on dendrites (p < 0.0001 by χ2analysis).
Comparison of the rates of GAD65-positive and GAD65-negative terminal innervation of somata revealed that the number of GAD65-positive terminals increased more rapidly than that of GAD65-negative terminals (Fig. 6A). A similar comparison restricted to dendritic trees (that is, excluding cell somata) showed that GAD65-positive terminals formed at a much slower rate than did GAD65-negative terminals (Fig. 6B).
We next examined the distribution of somatic boutons on relatively old cultures (31 d in vitro) to assess the stability of the enhanced GABAergic innervation on somata. The proportion of somatic GABAergic synapses did not change significantly over this time.
Do GABAergic terminals affect the formation of non-GABAergic terminals?
We compared the total number of synaptophysin-labeled terminals on the somata of neurons receiving GABAergic terminals with those that were devoid of GABAergic terminals. The total number of synaptophysin-labeled terminals did not vary between these two groups [2.54 boutons/10 μm2 ± 0.19 and 2.20 boutons/10 μm2 ± 0.26, respectively (n = 20)]. The presence and extent of GAD innervation of somata correlated precisely with a concomitant reduction in the number of GAD-negative terminals in somata.
Effects of activity blockade on the segregation of synapses
Neuronal activity was chronically blocked by exposing neuron cultures to 1 μm TTX from the third day after plating. TTX was renewed every 3 d, a frequency that has been demonstrated to eliminate evoked potentials (Craig et al., 1994; Verderio et al., 1994). Even after a 15 d exposure, neurons appeared healthy and qualitatively indistinguishable from untreated neurons. By labeling cultures with antibodies against GABA, which more completely labels cell somata than GAD65 does, we determined that 6% of the neurons in TTX-treated cultures were GABAergic. This percentage did not differ significantly from that in sister cultures or from untreated cultures analyzed previously (Benson et al., 1994). The effect of TTX on dendritic outgrowth and maturation was assessed by counting the number of primary and terminal dendritic branches and by measuring individual branch lengths in cultures immunocytochemically labeled with MAP2. Prolonged treatment with TTX reduced the overall extent of dendritic arbors primarily by decreasing the numbers of terminal branches and the lengths of primary dendrites (Table 1). By contrast, TTX treatment had no effect on the distribution or proportion of GAD65-labeled terminals on cell somata or on proximal, middle, and distal dendrites, nor did TTX treatment affect the number of synaptophysin boutons per micron (Table 1).
DISCUSSION
In the present study, we used cultured hippocampal neurons to examine the spatial and temporal sequence leading to the proper sorting of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic terminals to different cellular domains, and the role of neural activity in this process. There were three major findings. (1) From the time at which they began to form, GABAergic synaptic terminals developed more rapidly and eventually outnumbered non-GABAergic terminals on neuronal somata, whereas non-GABAergic terminals predominated on dendrites. (2) In the absence of GABAergic synaptic terminals, non-GABAergic terminals were not inhibited from forming synapses on cell somata. (3) These initial stages of synaptic terminal segregation occurred normally in the absence of neural activity, although dendritic arborization was inhibited. Collectively, these data rule out the hypothesis that GABAergic and non-GABAergic terminals initially are distributed randomly and only through a later process rearrange to their adult-like pattern of relatively restricted distribution. Rather, the data support a model whereby from the earliest stages of GABAergic terminal formation, inhibitory synaptic terminals are maintained preferentially on cell somata. Interestingly, however, this does not imply that non-GABAergic terminals are inhibited from forming contacts with cell somata, because they readily achieved a normal total synaptic density on somata lacking GAD terminals. Rather, it may be the case that GABAergic terminals inhibit or repulse non-GABAergic ones, or they compete more successfully for some attractant molecule or trophic factor.
Development of GAD-containing terminals and synaptic inhibition
The appearance of GAD-labeled boutons in the present study correlates closely with the reported appearance of functional synaptic inhibition in cultured hippocampal neurons (Basarsky et al., 1994) as well as in hippocampus of approximately the same age in vivo(Zhang et al., 1991). GABAA receptors are present even before GAD-labeled boutons (Killisch et al., 1991). Therefore, at least some GAD-labeled boutons are likely to provide inhibition at the earliest stages examined, although it is possible that neurotransmitter release from some of the GAD-labeled boutons results in depolarizing potentials (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Fiszman et al., 1990; Xie and Smart, 1991). The fact that the numbers of GAD-labeled boutons continue to increase over the entire neuron suggests that such boutons are not transient and will eventually, if not immediately, contribute to synaptic inhibition.
Development of non-GABAergic terminals
Hippocampal neurons, when cultured as described, are composed only of interneurons and pyramidal neurons (Goslin and Banker, 1991). The vast majority of interneurons are GABAergic, and their synaptic terminals will contain GAD (Benson et al., 1994). Because pyramidal neurons are glutamatergic (Streit, 1984), terminals lacking GAD are probably excitatory.
At all ages, most terminal boutons on cultured hippocampal neurons lacked GAD. This is not surprising, because pyramidal neurons greatly outnumber interneurons (16:1) in these cultures (Benson et al., 1994). Most of the non-GABAergic boutons terminate on dendrites, an observation that parallels the distribution of asymmetric synapses in the adult hippocampus in vivo (Andersen et al., 1966). The rate at which non-GABAergic terminals form on dendrites increases more rapidly than that of GABAergic terminals between 11 and 18 d in culture, a period coinciding with the development of dendritic spines (Banker and Waxman, 1988; Papa et al., 1995; Benson et al., 1996). Dendritic spines are the principal sites for excitatory synaptic terminals (Horner, 1993; Harris and Kater, 1994) and have extremely few GABAergic terminals in vivo and in vitro (Seress and Ribak, 1990; Craig et al., 1993; present study). It is not known how dendritic spines are formed, but an appealing hypothesis is that excitatory synaptic terminal proliferation may serve to induce incipient dendritic spines.
Segregation of GABAergic and non-GABAergic terminals
How do GABAergic and non-GABAergic terminals become selectively distributed on their targets? The present data show that the rate of formation of GABAergic terminals exceeds that of non-GABAergic terminals on somata, but it lags behind non-GABAergic terminals on dendrites. This supports the notion that attractive and/or repulsive forces may be used for localized synaptic terminal targeting on cellular domains, much like what has been described for guiding axons to more distant targets (Goodman, 1996). One possibility is that the greater number of somatic GABAergic terminals may arise by localized repulsion of non-GABAergic terminals from somata; however, our data suggest that this is unlikely. In the absence of GABAergic input, non-GABAergic terminals achieve the same total synaptic terminal density on somata observed in the presence of GABAergic input. Rather, cell somata seem to provide a particularly permissive environment for the formation of GABAergic terminals. Conditions that favor GABAergic innervation could be generated by a number of mechanisms. GABAergic axons might express a unique receptor corresponding to a guidance molecule that has an enhanced somatic distribution; GABAergic axons may compete more effectively for a soma-localized trophic factor; or GABAergic axons may inhibit or repel non-GABAergic axons.
On dendrites, non-GABAergic terminals form more rapidly than GABAergic terminals. They may respond to an attractive molecule(s) emanating from dendrites, but given that non-GABAergic neurons greatly outnumber GABAergic neurons, an attractive force would not be necessary to achieve a greater density of synaptic terminals. Likewise, because GABAergic axons are capable of forming distally located synapses, it is unlikely that they are selectively repulsed from dendrites, or at least from dendritic shafts; however, they may be repulsed from dendritic spines. We found no GABAergic synaptic terminals on dendritic spines, and GAD-immunoreactive terminals do not seem to colocalize with the dendritic spine-localized glutamate receptor subunit GluR1 in cultured hippocampal neurons (Craig et al., 1994).
Comparative aspects of synaptic terminal distribution in vivo and in vitro
The preference that GABAergic terminals show for somata and that non-GABAergic terminals show for dendrites is a pattern that resembles the distribution of GABAergic or symmetric and asymmetric terminals during early postnatal development of hippocampus and neocortex (Blue and Parnavelas, 1983; Bähr and Wolff, 1985; Seress et al., 1989). In adult rat hippocampus, however, nearly all somatic synapses are symmetric and presumably inhibitory (Gottlieb and Cowan, 1972), a state never attained by GAD-labeled terminals in the cultured neurons examined in the present study. It is possible that if neurons could be grown in culture indefinitely, they might eventually display an adult-like synaptic distribution pattern either by synaptic remodeling or elimination or by previously unlabeled terminals becoming immunoreactive for GAD. If this were the case, we would predict the proportion of GABAergic terminals on neuronal somata to increase over time in culture, but we found the proportion to be relatively stable after 18 d in culture. Furthermore, it is unlikely that unlabeled terminals would become GABAergic, because there was no evidence of either a change in the total number of neurons expressing GAD or GABA or in the intensity of GAD labeling. It is more likely that the final exclusion of non-GABAergic synapses from cell somata requires the presence of an interaction not available in culture.
The observations in the present study support a two-step model for the segregation of excitatory and inhibitory synapses on postsynaptic neurons. In the first step, excitatory and inhibitory synapses are sorted differentially to separate cellular target domains, but some overlap remains on cell somata. In the second step, remaining excitatory terminals are eliminated from cell somata, but the original distribution pattern is maintained on dendrites. Such a two-step process is similar to what has been described for the regional restriction of synapses in several areas of vertebrate brain where weaker and/or inappropriate connections are eliminated and stronger and/or appropriate connections are maintained (Debski et al., 1990;Goodman and Shatz, 1993; O’Leary and Koester, 1993). It follows that the observations made in the present study apply to the early acquisition of synapses rather than to their final organization.
Terminal segregation and neuronal activity
A number of studies have demonstrated that neurotransmitters can directly affect growth cone movement (Haydon et al., 1984;Michler-Stuke and Wolff, 1987; McCobb et al., 1988), and given the putative neurotrophic role for GABA (Hansen et al., 1984; Taylor et al., 1990), GABA release is an obvious candidate for initiating preferential interactions with cell somata or for repelling non-GABAergic growth cones. However, unlike the activity-dependent pruning of synaptic terminals that is often observed in vertebrates (Goodman and Shatz, 1993), the present data demonstrate that segregation of excitatory and inhibitory terminals occurs normally in the absence of activity-evoked neurotransmitter release. Thus, if GABA serves as the signaling molecule, small amounts released spontaneously must be sufficient to engage somatic GABA receptors.
In cultured rat cerebral cortex, long-term exposure to TTX depletes GABA (Ramakers et al., 1994). Thus, it is possible that GABA levels were reduced in our cultures; however, there were no overt decreases in the intensity of GAD immunofluorescence, in the number of GABA-immunoreactive neurons, or in the number of GAD-containing boutons after TTX treatment. The apparent absence of any change in GAD immunocytochemistry concurs with what has been observed in the cerebral cortex in vivo. After monocular deprivation in adult monkeys, levels of immunocytochemically localized GABA and GAD as well as GAD67 mRNA are decreased in the eye-dominance columns of primary visual cortex corresponding to the deprived eye relative to the normal eye columns (Hendry and Jones, 1986; Benson et al., 1991); however, the monocular crescent representation in visual cortex that receives information solely from the contralateral, deprived eye shows no change in GABA or GAD immunoreactivity (Hendry and Carder, 1992). Changes in the levels of GABA and GAD seem to be restricted to regions where an imbalance in the levels of activity arising from the two eyes occurs. In the present study, all synaptic interactions are affected equally by TTX, and thus no imbalance in activity exists. Therefore, GAD levels would not be expected to change, consistent with the present observations.
The present results, however, demonstrated that chronic activity blockade did lead to decreased dendritic outgrowth. A number of studies have suggested that synaptogenesis may be retarded when neural activity is absent (Zanka and Jones, 1982; Van Huizen et al., 1985), but our results suggest instead that a decrease in synapse number may be attributable to a decrease in available synaptic sites.
Footnotes
This research was supported by National Science Foundation Grant IBN-9419900 (D.L.B.). We thank Robert Woolley, Dariel L. S. Cobb, and Marina B. Rubin for their technical assistance, and Dr. G. W. Huntley for his comments on this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Deanna L. Benson, Fishberg Research Center for Neurobiology, Box 1065, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, One Gustave L. Levy Place, New York, NY 10029.