Abstract
Spontaneous rhythmic bursting induced by coapplication of strychnine (1 μm) and bicuculline (20 μm) was observed with electrophysiological recording from pairs of lumbar ventral roots (usually L5) in an isolated preparation of the neonatal rat spinal cord. Bursting was insensitive to exogenously applied GABA or glycine, confirming that it was attributable to block of glycine and GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition. NMDA accelerated bursting in a dose-dependent manner. Complete coronal spinal transection at L3 or L6 level did not block bursting recorded from L5 or L2 roots, respectively. Gradual cutting of the cord along the midline through a sagittal plane preserved bursting activity in both disconnected sides but led to loss of synchronicity. Once the spinal cord was fully separated into left and right halves, regular bursting persisted on each side with no phase-coupling between the two preparations. Section along a frontal plane to remove dorsal horns and much of the central canal area did not affect burst frequency or left-to-right synchronicity, whereas it reduced burst duration. A quadrant preparation containing mainly a single ventral horn displayed enhanced burst frequency while bursts became very short events. Bath application of 5-hydroxytryptamine (30 μm) or NMDA (5 μm) increased burst frequency and decreased burst duration in all types of preparation except the isolated quadrants, in which brief bursts were accelerated but not shortened by these chemical agents. These results suggest that bursting induced by strychnine and bicuculline apparently relied on distinct mechanisms for burst triggering and intraburst structure. The first required a relatively smaller neuronal network that was confined to a ventral quadrant. Intraburst structure was dependent on a larger circuitry comprising either both ventral horns or one side of the spinal cord including more than two segments.
Investigating the rhythmic motor patterns of the spinal cord in vitro provides a useful tool for understanding the more complex behavior of functionally intact spinal networks. In the in vitro neonatal rat spinal cord, rhythmic patterns of motoneuronal activity can be induced by several agents, including 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT), NMDA, and acetylcholine (Cazalets et al., 1992; Cowley and Schmidt, 1994), which are known to modulate neuronal membrane properties (Hultborn and Kiehn, 1992). Each one of these drugs can elicit rhythmic activity consistent with locomotion (i.e., alternating bursts in flexor and extensor motoneurons on each side of the cord, referred to as fictive locomotion) (Rossignol and Dubuc, 1994), although patterns incompatible with locomotor activity (for instance, left–right alternation but in-phase activation of flexor and extensor nerves within each hindlimb) can also be manifested (Cowley and Schmidt, 1994). We have shown recently that in this preparation regular rhythmic bursts occur spontaneously in motoneurons after applying pharmacological blockers of GABAA and glycine receptors (Bracci et al., 1996). Such bursts, characterized by several intraburst oscillations, take place simultaneously in left and right ventral roots with a frequency of ∼2 bursts/min and are accelerated by 5-HT in a dose-dependent manner similar to that previously described for fictive locomotion (Cazalets et al., 1992). Recently, Cowley and Schmidt (1995) showed that after pharmacological block of inhibition drug-induced fictive locomotion is also converted into synchronized bursting at lower frequency. Furthermore, even organotypic cultures of the rat spinal cord display synchronized bursting in the presence of strychnine and bicuculline (Streit, 1993). These findings raise the possibility that spinal rhythmogenesis could result from cellular mechanisms that do not require synaptic inhibition, even if inhibition is certainly necessary to ensure temporal phasing of motor patterns.
The issue of localization of the rhythmogenic activity is a complex one and not resolved even for the fictive locomotor pattern. In the case of the chick embryo preparation, Ho and O’Donovan (1993) have shown that both caudal and rostral segments of the lumbosacral cord can generate this rhythmic activity and that neurons localized in the ventrolateral part of the ventral horns are sufficient for the generation of motoneuronal bursting. Moreover, they showed that during spontaneous motor episodes, two different mechanisms are responsible for burst frequency and burst duration and that these mechanisms can be affected separately by experimental lesions. This kind of investigation has not yet been performed on the mammalian spinal cord. In the rat, the localization of the locomotor network is still controversial; in fact, using Vaseline barriers applied to the surface of the rat spinal cord, Cazelets et al. (1995) have proposed that this network is localized to the upper lumbar segments, whereas other investigators suggest that the rhythmogenic neural substrate is distributed over a large number of segments (for discussion, see Katz, 1996). The present work investigated, in the rat spinal cord, the topography of the networks responsible for the rhythmic activity generated by simultaneous application of GABAA and glycine antagonists. To address this issue, lesions of the spinal cord were performed, including removal of segments, progressive hemisectioning, and ablation of dorsal horn tissue. Furthermore, the block of GABA or glycine receptors was functionally assessed and the sensitivity of patterned bursting to agents able to induce fictive locomotion was tested.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experiments were carried out on spinal cord preparations isolated from neonatal rats (5–12 d old) under urethane anesthesia [6 ml of 10% (w/v) solution, i.p.] as described previously (Fisher and Nistri, 1993; Ballerini et al., 1995). Spinal cords (from the mid-thoracic region to the cauda equina) were continuously superfused with oxygenated Krebs solution of the following composition (in mm): NaCl 113, KCl 4.5, MgCl2 1, CaCl2 2, NaH2PO4 1, NaHCO3 25, glucose 11, pH 7.4, at 7.5 ml/min. Dorsal root stimulation was performed via a miniature suction electrode using stimuli of 0.1 msec duration, 1.5–2× threshold intensity (2–5 V) for monosynaptic ventral root responses, and applied once every 8 sec. For root recordings, lumbar ventral roots (predominantly L5, although L2 or L3 were also used) were connected to miniature suction electrodes filled with Krebs solution. For intracellular recording, motoneurons (functionally identified on the basis of their antidromic spike) were impaled with 3 m KCl-filled microelectrodes. Responses were amplified with an Axoclamp 2A unit, stored on videotape for further analysis, digitized at 1–10 kHz with the pClamp program (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA; version 6.2), and displayed on a chart recorder. Ventral root responses were recorded with either DC-coupled (usually with 30 kHz lowpass filter) or AC-coupled (100 Hz to 10 kHz passband) amplification. An additional 20 Hz lowpass filter was used occasionally. Details concerning the definition of bursts and their measurement (duration and frequency) are as reported previously (Bracci et al., 1996); cycle period was measured from the start of a burst to the beginning of the next one, and intraburst oscillation frequency was measured as 1/t, where t is the interval between the peaks of two successive waves. Data are expressed as mean ± SD with statistical significance evaluated by ANOVA. Drugs were applied via the bathing solution. For experiments aimed at localizing the bursting network mechanisms, lesions were performed with a small piece of razor blade after observing control responses: data were normally collected after a 15 min stabilization period after each lesion. Various types of lesion were produced: they consisted of (1) gradual cutting along the midline (sagittal plane cutting, thus yielding two hemisected preparations), (2) transverse sectioning (coronal plane cutting, thus eliminating spinal cord segments), or (3) cutting through a frontal plane yielding hemicords with ventral horns only. The latter preparation was further divided along a sagittal plane to produce a quadrant in which only one ventral horn was present. At the end of each experiment, preparations were fixed with tissue-freezing medium (Cryogel, Jung), frozen, and sectioned (60 μm) with a cryostat. After staining with toluidine blue (1% in distilled water), sections were examined with a Zeiss Axiovert 135 microscope (5× magnification).
RESULTS
Data were collected from 37 spinal cord preparations. Previous work (Bracci et al., 1996) has shown that after application of pharmacological blockers of synaptic inhibition to the spinal cord a regular pattern of network-driven bursts appeared in motoneurons. Figure 1A shows an example of bursting activity recorded intracellularly (in current-clamp conditions): bursts consisted of large amplitude depolarizations appearing at regular intervals. The average cycle period of burst episodes (measured from the start of a bursting event to the beginning of the next one) was 29 sec; average burst duration was 4.9 sec. Individual bursts were characterized by several intraburst oscillations (with frequency between 2 and 5 Hz), as shown by the faster timescale trace on the right side of Figure 1A. When motoneuronal population activity was monitored through ventral root recording, bursts and intraburst oscillations were also clearly detected with similar average frequency and duration (Fig. 1B). These events appeared nearly synchronously on both sides when recordings were taken from a pair of homolateral ventral roots (see examples in Figs.3A, 4A). This finding implies that both bursts and intraburst oscillations took place almost simultaneously in a large number of motoneurons on both sides of the cord, because even small differences in phase between motoneurons would have resulted in a less organized pattern. DC-coupled tracings from ventral roots closely resembled intracellular recordings in their slow time course (Fig.1B, top row) during which clusters of action potentials were present during bursting activity (unless records were lowpass-filtered; compare expanded records in Fig.1B). AC-coupled and/or 20 Hz lowpass tracings were also able to monitor bursts and intraburst oscillations (Fig.1C) with loss of slow potential changes or fast transients. These results indicate that the main characteristics of bursting were detected with extracellular recordings (either DC- or AC-coupled) which, therefore, were much more suitable to study the effects of lesions on this preparation. Extracellular recording also allowed us to assess that bursting activity took place simultaneously in ventral (ipsi- or contralateral) roots of lumbar segments (L2–L5) of the isolated spinal cord (data not shown).
Is synaptic inhibition blocked during rhythmic bursts?
Even if strychnine and bicuculline are known to block inhibitory synaptic transmission in rat motoneurons in vitro (Wu et al., 1992), it remained to be ascertained whether in the present experimental conditions of spontaneous bursting, inhibitory synaptic processes were blocked throughout the spinal network; this was assessed by extracellular recording. In our previous study (Bracci et al., 1996), 20 μm bicuculline methiodide and 1 μm strychnine were used to block GABAA and glycine receptors, respectively. In the present investigation, the same concentrations of these antagonists were first tested for their ability to prevent the inhibitory action of exogenously applied glycine or GABA (in concentrations known to strongly inhibit motoneurons; Wu et al., 1992) on polysynaptic reflexes (elicited from L5 dorsal root by electrical stimulation of the homologous ipsilateral dorsal root). Dorsal root stimulations were continuously delivered at 8 sec intervals, so as to avoid excessive depression of the reflex (Lev-Tov and Pinco, 1992). Glycine (0.5 mm) caused a strong reduction in reflex peak amplitude and area (Fig.2A). On average, in three preparations such a glycine application significantly (p < 0.0001) decreased polysynaptic reflex peak amplitude by 37 ± 21% and area by 64 ± 18%. These effects were readily reversed on washout (Fig. 2A). Subsequent application of 1 μm strychnine caused an increase in reflex peak amplitude (37 ± 15%) and area (52 ± 25%), with the appearance of time-locked oscillations at 4–8 Hz (see example in Fig.2A). In the presence of strychnine, application of 0.5 mm glycine failed to affect reflex amplitude, area, or oscillations. The effects of GABA application on dorsal root-evoked reflex before and after application of bicuculline were tested in the presence of 10 μm CGP 52,432 so as to avoid involvement of GABAB receptors. Under these conditions (n = 3), 1 mm GABA significantly (p < 0.0001) and reversibly reduced reflex peak amplitude by 54 ± 11% and area by 88 ± 19% (see example in Fig. 2B). Subsequent application of 20 μm bicuculline increased reflex peak amplitude by 64 ± 13% and reflex area by 440 ± 110% but, unlike strychnine, it did not elicit slow, rhythmic oscillations. In the presence of bicuculline, 1 mm GABA failed to affect reflex peak amplitude or area (Fig. 2B).
After induction of spontaneous bursting by coapplication of strychnine and bicuculline to the same preparations, 0.5 mm glycine failed to affect burst frequency or duration (Fig. 2C; similar data were obtained on three preparations). GABA (1 mm) was equally ineffective on bursting frequency or duration in the presence of strychnine, bicuculline, and CGP 52,432 (n = 2; Fig. 2D). Prolonged application (>30 min) of CGP 52,432 (10 μm) per se did not block bursting induced by strychnine and bicuculline in 4 of 4 preparations (data not shown).
Once rhythmic bursts were induced by 1 μm strychnine and 20 μm bicuculline, progressive increases in strychnine concentration up to 10 μm had no effect on burst frequency or duration. On the other hand, when the noncompetitive GABAA antagonist picrotoxin (100 μm) was coapplied with 1 μm strychnine (omitting bicuculline), rhythmic bursts similar to those elicited by bicuculline and strychnine were always observed. Under these conditions (n = 4), burst frequency, burst duration, and their coefficients of variation were not significantly different from the preparations treated with strychnine and bicuculline. Similarly, during the course of the experiment replacing bicuculline with picrotoxin (in the continuous presence of strychnine) did not alter the bursting pattern established with bicuculline and strychnine at the start of the recording session (n = 2). When coapplied with strychnine, 1 mm penicillin (another GABAA antagonist) was also able to induce rhythmic bursts similar to the one elicited by bicuculline and strychnine (n = 2).
All together, these results strongly suggest that in the rat isolated spinal cord GABAA and glycine receptors were blocked by the concentrations of strychnine and bicuculline used in the present study and that, under these conditions, rhythmogenesis should be based on mechanisms independent of these receptors. Furthermore, GABAB receptors were not necessary for rhythm generation.
Sensitivity of bursting activity to NMDA and carbachol
In our previous study (Bracci et al., 1996), the NMDA receptor antagonists CPP and APV slowed down bursting cycle period or completely suppressed bursting; in the latter case, pharmacological block of AMPA receptor desensitization reestablished bursting. It seemed interesting, therefore, to test the role of NMDA receptors more directly by applying NMDA and comparing it with another rhythmogenic substance such as carbachol (Cowley and Schmidt, 1995). After rhythmic bursts were induced by strychnine and bicuculline, application of NMDA was able to increase burst frequency and to decrease burst duration in a dose-dependent manner. In the experiment of Figure 3, NMDA concentrations were cumulatively applied at ∼10 min intervals after rhythm stabilization. The largest NMDA dose compatible with bursting was 10 μm (cycle period was 1.3 ± 0.1 sec and burst duration 0.13 ± 0.02 sec; see also Fig. 3B). As shown in the right panels of Figure 3A, intraburst oscillations were preserved with 2.5 and 5 μm NMDA, even if their number progressively decreased and ultimately disappeared with 10 μm NMDA, which converted bursts into single-discharge events. On a sample of four preparations, 5 μm NMDA (in the presence of strychnine and bicuculline) significantly decreased cycle period (by 75 ± 10%) and burst duration (by 59 ± 18%). The cholinergic agonist carbachol (10 μm) also significantly decreased cycle period (by 70 ± 16%) and burst duration (by 55 ± 19%) in the presence of strychnine and bicuculline (n = 3; data not shown). These data indicate that the bursting activity induced by bicuculline and strychnine was sensitive to NMDA and carbachol, two agents that are able to induce rhythmic activity in the isolated spinal cord (Cowley and Schmidt, 1995).
Ablation of rostral or caudal segments
It has been proposed recently that in the neonatal rat spinal cord the networks responsible for generation of locomotor-like activity are localized to L1/L2 lumbar segments (Cazelets et al., 1995). To investigate whether the networks localized at this level are necessary for the rhythmic activity evoked by strychnine and bicuculline, we recorded bilaterally from L5 ventral roots before and after ablation of the spinal cord segments rostral to the exit of L3 ventral roots (the lesion is schematically shown in Fig.4A, left). Figure 4 shows that bursting persisted in both L5 ventral roots, and it was also maintained in the L2 ventral root (which is above the transection level; data not shown). In 6 of 6 preparations, rhythmic bursts (with intraburst oscillations) persisted at the same frequency after this lesion (compare in Fig. 4A, top andbottom traces, taken from the same spinal cord before and after surgical ablation). The average values for cycle period from 6 preparations confirmed the lack of change after removal of the upper spinal segments (Fig. 4B, left). Burst duration became slightly shorter, as indicated by the right records at a faster time sweep (Fig. 4A). This phenomenon, however, was not statistically significant, as shown by the histograms from 6 preparations (Fig. 4B, right). It seemed also of interest to explore the minimum number of segments necessary to support bursting. In four cases, the portion of the cord localized caudal to L6 was subsequently removed without change in burst frequency and duration (data not shown). Furthermore, a tissue block containing only L4 and L5 segments (n = 3) generated bursts (recorded from L5 ventral root) of a more irregular nature, as indicated by their elevated coefficient of variation for the cycle period (on average 75 vs 12% in the intact cords). Burst duration was on average decreased by 59% with respect to the intact preparations. A block of tissue comprising the L5 segment only (n = 3) displayed irregular and infrequent spontaneous bursts occurring on average at 7.3 ± 3.7 min intervals and lacking intraburst oscillations (burst duration 3.3 ± 2.6 sec).
Sagittal sectioning of the spinal cord
Because the present pattern consists of simultaneous bursting of homologous ventral roots, we tested whether side-to-side neural connections at different segmental levels were necessary to maintain synchronicity. To this end, we first performed progressive hemisectioning from the caudal part of the cord (as indicated bydotted lines at the top of Fig.5A) while recording from left and right L5 ventral roots. Figure 5A shows that, even when the cord was bisected up to L4 or L2 (termed type I or II lesion, respectively), rhythmic bursts still took place simultaneously in L5 ventral roots. Similar results were obtained in four preparations. In the example of Figure 5A, sagittal hemisectioning also produced shortening of individual burst duration. This phenomenon, however, was not statistically significant when data from four preparations were pooled together (see below). The average values observed for burst frequency and burst duration are illustrated in the plots of Figure6A, in which the type I and II lesions are grouped as caudal hemisection protocol (open columns). Although burst frequency and burst duration were not significantly affected by these lesions, the coefficient of variation of the cycle period (but not the one of burst duration) was largely increased (+168%) after type II lesion. Thus, such a lesion affected the regularity of burst onset without any concomitant change in burst duration regularity. When the cord was further split up to L1 level, complete synchronicity was maintained in two of four preparations. Further splitting up to thoracic level resulted in burst desynchronization in all cases (data not shown).
Analogous results were observed when the hemisection was performed starting from the rostral end of the cord (rostral hemisection protocol; n = 4). In this case, bursts still took place simultaneously in L5 ventral roots even when the cord was split down to L6 (type III lesion) or S2 (type IV lesion), as shown in Figure5B. The effects of these lesions on the average values of cycle period and burst duration are plotted in Figure6B. Even in this case, no significant changes were observed in these values, but the coefficient of variation of the cycle period was largely increased after lesions III and IV (+531 and +590%, respectively). Moreover, burst duration appeared to decrease after each lesion, although this effect was not statistically significant. Further splitting of the spinal cord resulted in partial or complete loss of synchronicity in all cases. Thus, side-to-side connections localized more than three segments apart from L5 level (either rostrally or caudally) are sufficient to maintain rhythmic bursts synchronous in L5 ventral roots.
Complete hemisection of the spinal cord was performed on four intact preparations. In all cases, rhythmic bursting in left and right L5 ventral roots persisted without synchronicity. In two cases the two halves displayed different burst frequency and duration (see Fig.5C), whereas in the other two cases burst frequency and duration were similar for the left and right sides, although no longer temporally locked together. Plots of Figure 6C (obtained by pooling all data from both sides of each preparation) show that, in addition to a strong increase in the coefficient of variation of cycle period, complete hemisection caused a significant reduction in burst duration. These data demonstrate that each side of the spinal cord possessed the neural substrate necessary to produce rhythmic activity in the presence of strychnine and bicuculline.
Effects of dorsal horns ablation
After dorsal horn removal from the chick embryo spinal cord, the remaining network, localized in the ventral horns, is still able to perform coordinated rhythmic activity (Ho and O’Donovan, 1993). To test whether in the neonatal rat spinal cord ventral horn networks were able to generate rhythmic activity in the absence of synaptic inhibition, we recorded from L5 ventral roots before and after a frontal section that removed the dorsal horns and the central canal area from spinal cord preparations comprising lumbosacral segments. In five such preparations, histological examination of stained sections confirmed that only the laminae localized ventrally to the central canal had been left (see example in Fig. 7B). In all of these cases, rhythmic bursts were still observed in lesioned preparations, as illustrated by a representative example in Figure7A. Whereas the cycle period was unaffected by dorsal horn removal, a significant reduction in burst duration was consistently observed even if intraburst oscillations were still detectable. Average values of cycle period and burst duration before and after dorsal horns ablation are plotted in Figure 7C.
Rhythmic activity in isolated ventral horns
In four preparations exposed to strychnine and bicuculline, rhythmic bursting was studied after surgical removal of dorsal horns plus one ventral horn that left an isolated spinal cord quadrant. Such a lesion, performed on a preparation consisting of four to five lumbosacral segments, was confirmed histologically on transverse slices of the tissue (see example in Fig.8B). Figure 8Aillustrates recordings from L5 ventral root before and after this procedure. Rhythmic bursts were still present after lesion; their frequency was larger than in control, whereas their duration decreased. Similar results were observed in the other preparations. The average values for cycle period and burst duration are plotted in Figure8C. In all cases, burst frequency increased after this lesion, with a concomitant decrease in burst duration. Both effects were statistically significant (Fig. 8C). The oscillatory structure of bursts, typical of intact preparations, was always lost after this lesion, and bursts were converted from multicomponent events (lasting on average ∼7 sec) to shorter (1–3 sec) single discharges (Fig. 8A).
Sensitivity of lesioned preparations to 5-HT and NMDA
In fully hemisected spinal cords and in preparations consisting of isolated ventral horns, application of 5-HT (10 μm) or NMDA (5 μm) in the presence of strychnine and bicuculline always resulted in a concomitant increase of burst frequency and decrease of burst duration, similar to that observed in intact spinal cord [see Fig. 3 and related text for NMDA effects; see also Bracci et al. (1996) for 5-HT effects on intact preparations]. In the presence of 5-HT, cycle period and burst duration were significantly (p < 0.0001) decreased by 40 ± 11 and 53 ± 15%, respectively, in hemisected cords (n = 3) and by 59 ± 10 and 37 ± 9%, respectively, in dorsal horn-ablated preparations (n = 3). In the presence of NMDA, cycle period and burst duration were significantly (p < 0.0001) decreased by 45 ± 16 and 56 ± 10%, respectively, in hemisected cords (n = 2) and by 62 ± 9 and 43 ± 13%, respectively, in dorsal horn-ablated preparations (n = 3). In isolated ventral quadrants (in which bursts were reduced to single-discharge events), only cycle period was significantly (p < 0.0001) decreased by NMDA or 5-HT (62 ± 21 and 55 ± 18%, respectively; n = 3). In these preparations, however, burst duration was not significantly affected by these agents. This situation is illustrated in the example of Figure9A: in the presence of strychnine and bicuculline alone, regular rhythmic bursts were recorded from L5 ventral root of an isolated ventral horn. These events were <2 sec in duration and lacked intraburst oscillations, which were replaced by a plateau with sustained action potential firing (as indicated by thickening of tracing during burst; right panels of Fig.9A). Application of 5 μm NMDA produced a strong increase in burst frequency (310%) but failed to affect burst duration significantly (see plots of Fig. 9B). The faster tracings of individual bursts shown in the right panels of Figure9A also indicate that there was no alteration in intraburst structure in the presence of NMDA. These results suggest that two different mechanisms could underlie burst triggering and burst structure. The second mechanism was probably lost after surgical isolation of a single ventral quadrant, resulting in brief and unstructured bursts, while the mechanism responsible for burst onset was still present. As a consequence, further pharmacological manipulations with 5-HT or NMDA were able to affect burst triggering only.
DISCUSSION
The principal finding of this study is that spontaneous rhythmic bursts induced in the neonatal rat spinal cord by strychnine and bicuculline were present in the following spinal structures even after their surgical isolation from the rest of the cord: (1) spinal segments caudal to L3; (2) longitudinally hemisected spinal cord; (3) isolated ventral horn preparations; and (4) single ventral quadrant.
Block of synaptic inhibition
Because many models of spinal rhythmogenesis rely on synaptic inhibition (Grillner et al., 1991; Dale, 1995; Roberts et al., 1995), it was important to investigate whether inhibition was actually suppressed during the present rhythm. Although on neonatal rat motoneurons strychnine and bicuculline are known to block GABAA and glycine receptors, respectively (Wu et al., 1992), the present experiments were undertaken to validate this notion throughout the spinal cord networks. GABA (in the presence of CGP 52,432) or glycine failed to affect strychnine- and bicuculline-induced bursts. Furthermore, when the strychnine concentration was progressively elevated from 1 to 10 μm, no additional change in bursting pattern became apparent, thus suggesting that 1 μm strychnine had already saturated glycine receptors in accordance with the high affinity of this antagonist (Becker et al., 1988). These data indicate that inhibitory receptors were effectively blocked by the two alkaloids. The noncompetitive GABAAantagonists picrotoxin or penicillin produced effects that were indistinguishable from the ones caused by bicuculline, suggesting that bicuculline actually exerted its action through block of GABAA receptors. Because CGP 52,432 did not block bursts, it is also apparent that GABAB receptor activation was not required for such a rhythm.
Whereas strychnine or bicuculline largely increased dorsal root-evoked reflex activity, only strychnine caused the appearance of 4–8 Hz time-locked neuronal oscillations. These oscillations are similar to the ones intrinsic to the spontaneous bursts in strychnine and bicuculline solution and, because they were absent during the irregular bursting elicited by bicuculline alone (Bracci et al., 1996), presumably imply a particular role for glycinergic inhibition in the decay phase of polysynaptic excitatory responses. Similarly, Cowley and Schmidt (1995) have shown that application of strychnine (but not of bicuculline) during fictive locomotion evokes 4–8 Hz packets of synchronized intraburst discharges. Thus, it seems likely that these oscillations specifically arose from block of glycinergic inhibition.
Local networks supporting rhythmic bursting
Lesions of the neonatal rat spinal cord showed the following. (1) Removal of segments rostral to L3 had no effects on burst frequency and duration, although more than two segments were necessary to preserve regular rhythmicity. (2) Gradual splitting of the spinal cord from the caudal end increased the variability of burst frequency but not of burst duration. (3) Gradual splitting from the rostral end had similar effects on burst variability and tended to reduce burst duration. (4) Bilateral synchronicity between the activity of the two L5 ventral roots persisted even when crossed connections remained intact only at a distance of several segments (either rostrally or caudally). (5) After complete separation of the spinal cord into left and right hemicords, both sides continued to independently perform rhythmic bursting at different rates, even if this lesion increased burst frequency variability and decreased burst duration. (6) Ablation of the tissue dorsal to the central canal did not affect burst frequency but resulted in reduction of burst duration. (7) Although the characteristic structure of bursts with several oscillations was usually retained after the above-mentioned lesions, in the case of the ventral quadrant preparation bursts were converted into simple single-discharge events at higher frequency. Thus, it became apparent that the minimal neuronal network necessary for this rhythmic activity was contained within a single ventral quadrant spanning over a few lumbar segments.
On isolated spinal cords or lesioned preparations in which intraburst oscillations were still present, 5-HT or NMDA always produced a concomitant decrease in cycle period and burst duration. Conversely, in the case of isolated ventral quadrants these drugs enhanced burst frequency without changing burst duration. This finding suggests that the rhythmic onset of bursts and their prolonged oscillations were due to two different mechanisms. In support of this view, Streit (1996)observed that, on organotypic cultures of the rat spinal cord, lesions could selectively affect either duration or frequency of bursting induced by block of synaptic inhibition. In the present study, each type of lesion left the mechanism responsible for rhythmic burst onset intact, while the mechanism responsible for burst oscillations was impaired by hemisection and dorsal horns ablation and was lost after ventral quadrant isolation. It seems likely, therefore, that burst oscillations depended on the number of cells left in the network and that such a number was insufficient in the case of a single ventral quadrant, which was able to generate rhythmicity but incapable of supporting long duration bursts.
The cellular and network mechanisms supporting the observed bursting activity are still to be clarified. Because bursting appeared in the absence of synaptic inhibition and was not crucially dependent on NMDA receptor activity (see Bracci et al., 1996), the present results cannot be explained by models used for other rhythmogenic patterns such as fictive locomotion, which are based on reciprocal inhibition and NMDA receptor-driven pacemaker neurons (for review, see Grillner et al., 1995). In the present case, one can speculate that bursts originated via an autoregenerative mechanism (relying on AMPA receptor activity) (Ballerini et al., 1995; Bracci et al., 1996) within a network comprising excitatory connections only and were terminated perhaps by a combination of synaptic depression (Streit, 1993) and activation/inactivation of voltage-dependent conductances of premotoneurons (Bracci et al., 1996).
Comparison with other investigations
The relation between the present bursting pattern, observed in the absence of synaptic inhibition, and fictive locomotion (induced by 5-HT and/or NMDA) has yet to be clarified. In this sense, three possibilities can be envisioned. (1) These patterns are produced by two different and separated spinal networks whose behavior is unrelated. (2) The pattern observed in the presence of strychnine and bicuculline is produced by networks structurally overlapping the ones involved in fictive locomotion, but these patterns are produced by different cellular mechanisms. (3) These patterns are attributable to the same basic rhythm generator, and the differences in terms of spatiotemporal phasing are attributable to additional networks, comprising inhibitory processes, that ensure alternation in the presence of synaptic inhibition. Although further investigations are required to discriminate between these hypotheses, it is important to note that indirect support to the third hypothesis arises from the fact that agents traditionally used to elicit fictive locomotion (the frequency of which depends on drug concentration; Cazelets et al., 1992) were also able to accelerate the pattern induced by strychnine and bicuculline in a similar dose-dependent manner. Cazalets et al. (1995)have proposed that in the neonatal rat spinal cord fictive locomotion is produced by a rhythm-generating network localized in the rostral lumbar region at L1–L2 level. Such a rostrolumbar localization is compatible only with the first hypothesis because the rhythm-generator processes responsible for the pattern in the absence of inhibition remained after removal of segments rostral to L3. However, in the chick embryo spinal cord, Ho and O’Donovan (1993) have shown that even the caudal lumbosacral segments can perform rhythmic bursting similar to fictive locomotion, although they are less rhythmogenic than the rostral segments. These results, if applicable to the rat spinal cord, would allow using hypotheses two and three to explain the present data. In particular, within the framework of the third hypothesis, the rat caudal segments might possess locomotor rhythm generators that are unmasked by strychnine and bicuculline more easily than by 5-HT and NMDA.
In the Xenopus embryo spinal cord, rhythmic activity persists after the left and right sides have been separated (Kahn and Roberts, 1982) or in the presence of saturating concentrations of strychnine (Soffe, 1989); numerical models based on reciprocal inhibition between left and right pools hardly account for these findings (Dale, 1995). Thus, it seems likely that, as in the case of the neonatal rat, even in the Xenopus embryo spinal cord there is a rhythmogenic mechanism that does not require synaptic inhibition for its operation.
Rhythmic activity has been observed in other CNS preparations during block of synaptic inhibition. Brainstem respiratory networksin vitro have been shown to produce spontaneous rhythmic activity whose frequency is not changed by block of chloride-mediated synaptic inhibition (Feldman and Smith, 1989). In hippocampal slices, block of GABAA receptor results in rhythmic bursts characterized by a primary discharge followed by several oscillations (Miles et al., 1984; Traub et al., 1993; Taylor et al., 1995). In this case, two mechanisms have been proposed to be responsible for the primary discharge (attributed to activation of AMPA receptors at recurrent excitatory synapses) and for late oscillations (attributed to dendritic calcium spikes; Traub et al., 1993).
Concluding remarks
Because block of synaptic inhibition biases synaptic processes toward excitation in the spinal cord, this pharmacological tool provides a large experimental simplification of the neuronal circuitry to investigate the properties of the spinal networks. The findings that the collective behavior of spinal neurons, far from becoming chaotic under this conditions, turns into an extremely regular pattern, which can be strongly modulated by several chemical agents and is preserved after ablation of large portions of neuronal tissue, represent an important constraint to be included in spinal network models.
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants from the European Union (Human and Capital Mobility Programme), Telethon Foundation (Project #823), and Istituto Nazionale di Fisica della Materia to A.N. We thank Ciba-Geigy for kindly donating CGP 52,432 and Marco Beato for his participation in some coronal section experiments.
Correspondence should be addressed to E. Bracci at SISSA, Via Beirut 4, 34013 Trieste, Italy.