Abstract
Little is known about intrinsic variation from animal to animal in the periodicity of columnar systems within various regions of the mammalian cerebral cortex. To address this issue, complete mosaics of the ocular dominance columns were reconstructed from flat-mounts of the left and right striate cortex (V1) in six normal adult macaques (Macaca fascicularis). To identify the columns, we enucleated the right eye and subsequently processed striate cortex for cytochrome oxidase (CO) activity. Average column areas for the intact eye and the missing eye were nearly equal, confirming that monocular enucleation in adult macaques produces negligible column shrinkage. The contralateral eye’s columns occupied more territory than the ipsilateral eye’s columns, even in the central visual field representation (0° to 8°), where they predominated by 52 to 48%. The column mosaics showed remarkable variation in periodicity. The number of column pairs along the V1/V2 border ranged from 101 sets in one monkey to 154 sets in another. Average column width along the V1/V2 border ranged between 670 and 395 μm, a nearly twofold difference. The widest columns were found in the foveal representation. This high degree of innate variability should be taken into account when considering the effects of various sensory manipulations (e.g., strabismus, anisometropia), which have been reported to alter the periodicity of ocular dominance columns. We found pronounced intrinsic variation in the width and number of ocular dominance columns in a sample of six M. fascicularis, indicating that the number of hypercolumns within a given cortical area can range widely among normal members of the same species.
In many species, the geniculocortical afferents serving the right and left eyes terminate in a pattern of dovetailed stripes in layer IVc of striate cortex (V1) called ocular dominance columns (Hubel and Wiesel, 1969; Wiesel et al., 1974). In 1985, LeVay and colleagues reconstructed the entire mosaic formed by these columns in macaque striate cortex. They found that the columns give rise to an elaborate system of roughly parallel bands, laid out in a stereotypic fashion with respect to the retinotopic map in V1. Because reconstruction of the complete pattern of columns was such an arduous task, they examined only a single animal. Limiting their study to just one monkey left unanswered the question of how much the column pattern varies from animal to animal. Is the layout of the columns governed by the same rules in all macaques? How much natural variation occurs among animals in the geometry and periodicity of the columns? What might this variation tell us about the process responsible for the formation of the columns, or about the purposes they serve?
Since the first report by LeVay and colleagues (LeVay et al., 1985), subsequent studies have described the complete pattern of ocular dominance columns in a variety of primate species (Rosa et al., 1988,1992; Horton et al., 1990; Florence and Kaas, 1992). In each of these reports, as in LeVay’s original study, complete reconstruction of the columns was accomplished in only a single specimen. Moreover, investigators differed to some extent in how they fixed the tissue, labeled the ocular dominance columns, and reconstructed their pattern. For these reasons, the findings in these studies are difficult to compare. They leave unsettled the basic question of how much intrinsic variation occurs in the pattern of columns from animal to animal in a single species.
Our interest in this problem was stimulated by several reports that the periodicity of ocular dominance columns is affected by raising animals with strabismus, anisometropia, or alternating monocular occlusion (Löwel, 1994; Roe and Ts’o, 1995; Tieman and Tumosa, 1996). To aid in the interpretation of these studies, we thought it might be pertinent to report our experience gained from the study of ocular dominance columns in normal animals. Over the past 3 years, we have prepared complete flat-mounts of the ocular dominance columns in six normal adult macaques (Macaca fascicularis). We have been struck by a remarkable variability in the periodicity of the columns, although their general layout is quite consistent from animal to animal. The purpose of this paper is to illustrate the degree of intrinsic variation seen in the periodicity of ocular dominance columns within a small group of normal adult macaques from the same species and to point out certain features of the columns that differ from animal to animal.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experimental animals. Six normal adult cynomolgus macaques (M. fascicularis) were used for these experiments (Table 1). Five shared a common genetic heritage, coming from a feral colony on the island nation of Mauritius. Macaques were introduced to Mauritius in the early 17th century, when they escaped from Dutch sailors keeping them as pets aboard ships bound from Java (Houghton, 1988). With no natural predators, they have proliferated to a population of 50,000–60,000, destroying rare indigenous species and raiding croplands. The minimum ages listed in Table 1 were based on dates of capture, because birthdates were unknown. A single monkey came from a feral colony in the Philippines.
Surgical procedures. Before use, each animal was screened for any preexisting eye disease using standard ophthalmological technique. The pupils, eye movements, and eye alignment were normal. To test eye alignment, we relied on centration of the corneal light reflex in both pupils of the alert, fixating monkey (Hirshberg test). This method can detect strabismus greater than 3° to 4°, but not microstrabismus. After ocular motility and alignment were assessed, each animal was sedated (ketamine HCl, 20 mg/kg, i.m.) for cycloplegic refraction and fundus examination. No animal had any media opacity, abnormal or unequal refractive error, or strabismus (again, with the proviso that a microstrabismus could not be excluded).
To remove one eye, each monkey was placed under general endotracheal anesthesia with 2.5% isoflurane and N2O/O2(50:50). Following a protocol approved by the University of California, San Francisco Committee on Animal Research, the right eye was enucleated using sterile technique. After surgery, the animal was treated with buprenorphine HCl (0.02 mg/kg, i.m.) every 8 hr for 2 d to ensure analgesia.
Ocular dominance columns appear within 24 hr of monocular enucleation in cytochrome oxidase (CO)-stained sections of striate cortex. However, maximum contrast in CO activity is obtained by waiting 3 weeks. Therefore, all animals in this study survived at least 3 weeks (Table1). Some survived even longer, because they were used for an unrelated study of cortical myelin patterns induced by monocular enucleation (Horton and Hocking, 1997). These differences in survival times do not affect the pattern of ocular dominance columns in adult animals (LeVay et al., 1980) and, therefore, are unrelated to the variation in column periodicity that we found.
Flat-mounting of striate cortex. After receiving a lethal dose of sodium pentobarbital intraperitoneally, each animal was perfused through the left ventricle with 1 l of normal saline followed by 1.5 l of 2% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 mphosphate buffer. The occipital lobes were removed immediately and placed into 0.1 m phosphate buffer. Striate cortex was dissected from each occipital lobe, unfolded, and flattened (Fig.1). To achieve further flattening, the pial surface of the tissue was placed against a glass slide. A sheet of soft foam was applied to the white matter side, followed by a weight (∼8 gm/cm2). The tissue sandwich was left to bathe overnight in 1% paraformaldehyde plus 30% sucrose for cryoprotection and additional fixation. To flash-freeze the block for sectioning, the pial surface was placed on a glass slide covered with Saran Wrap while a bag of powdered dry ice was pressed against the white matter. Serial sections were cut at 30 μm, and alternate sections were reacted for CO activity (Wong-Riley, 1979). We have shown previously by combining [3H]proline transneuronal autoradiography with CO histochemistry in the same animal that the pale columns labeled in this manner represent the ocular dominance columns serving the enucleated eye (Horton, 1984).
Data analysis. To reconstruct the ocular dominance columns in each animal, every other CO section was photographed with Plus X-Pan 35 mm film (Eastman Kodak, Rochester, NY) and developed with Microdol-X (Eastman Kodak). The negatives were scanned into the computer, contrast-reversed, and analyzed using Photoshop 3.0 (Adobe Systems, Mountain View, CA). To start a montage, the single image containing the largest portion of layer IVc was chosen as the “base section.” We then outlined layer IVc in the next section with the “lasso” tool. The layer IVc fragments of the column pattern from this section were copied and pasted into a layer overlying the base section. They were rendered semitransparent to align them with the base section by matching prominent blood vessels. They were then restored to normal density and “flattened” onto the base section, extending the mosaic of columns in layer IVc. This process was repeated with each alternate section through the entire series until the montage was complete. The contrast and brightness of the final figures were adjusted to match the montages from each animal, but no other Photoshop features were used to retouch or enhance the images. The seams between each fragment in the montages are hardly visible because they can be seen only at high power.
To make black and white drawings, we created a transparent layer overlying the final montage and traced the borders of the dark columns at high magnification on the monitor using the “pencil” function at a 1 pixel width setting. The dark columns were then flood-filled with black, copied, and pasted onto a white background. To quantify column areas, the black and white drawings were transferred to Mocha (Jandel Scientific, San Rafael, CA). A thresholding function assigned pixels to left eye columns (black, level of gray = 1) or right eye columns (white, level of gray = 256). Intermediate levels of gray were used to quantitate the unreconstructable areas, blind spots, and monocular crescents.
To calculate the sample standard deviations (Table 2), values were averaged for the right and left hemisphere of each animal and then treated as a single datum (i.e., n = 6). This was done because parameters were highly correlated between hemispheres for any given animal. However, the alternative approach of analyzing each flat-mount as an independent datum (i.e., n = 12) resulted in a very similar sample standard deviation for each parameter.
RESULTS
Variability in column periodicity
The main purpose of this paper is to show the range in periodicity of ocular dominance columns seen in a consecutive sample of sixM. fascicularis examined in our laboratory. To make this point, we have selected for illustration the animal with the widest columns (Figs.2A,D), an animal with intermediate columns (Figs.2B,E), and the animal with the thinnest columns (Figs. 2C,F). Black and white drawings of these photomontages are shown in Figure3. Measurements derived from the drawings are provided in Table 2.
The periodicity of ocular dominance columns can be assessed by various methods, including Fourier analysis (Obermayer and Blasdel, 1993). In the belief that the pictures of the column mosaics speak for themselves, we have kept our approach simple. In each animal, we counted the number of pairs of ocular dominance columns that intersected the perimeter of striate cortex (V1/V2 border; vertical meridian). For example, in Monkey 1 there were 102 column pairs along the V1/V2 border, whereas in Monkey 3 there were 150 column pairs. Next, we measured the perimeter of V1, excluding the monocular crescent, to calculate the average column width along the V1/V2 border. It ranged from 670 μm (Monkey 1, right V1) to 395 μm (Monkey 3, right V1). Van Essen et al. (1984) have shown that macaque V1 surface area varies enormously, from 690 to 1560 mm2, with no obvious correlation with body weight. One might expect animals with a larger V1 to have fatter columns or more sets of columns. The monkeys in our series happened to be similar in V1 area, close to the means of 1195 and 1069 mm2 reported in macaques (Van Essen et al., 1984; Purves and LaMantia, 1993). Thus, fluctuation in striate area does not account for the range of column periodicity in Figures 2 and3.
Looking at Figures 2 and 3, it is apparent that the periodicity of the columns correlates with the intricacy of their mosaic. The widest columns were found in Monkey 1. Its columns formed a relatively simple pattern of nearly continuous, smooth stripes that swept across the operculum from the vertical meridian to the horizontal meridian. By contrast, Monkey 3 had the thinnest columns. They gave rise to a more complex pattern, with shorter stripes and more frequent fractures, bifurcations, and islands. Despite these differences in column periodicity and mosaic complexity, the ocular dominance columns were laid out according to the same general plan in all animals. The details of this pattern have been described eloquently by LeVay et al. (1985)and will not be reiterated here. It is worth mentioning that although column periodicity varied greatly from monkey to monkey, in any given animal the left and right cortices were virtually reciprocal images, with remarkable similarity in size, shape, column periodicity, and mosaic pattern. This resemblance included even minor idiosyncratic features, such as the appearance of the blind spot representation on each side. However, the precise details of the mosaic formed by the columns on each side were not identical.
Larger foveal columns
Columns were wider along the vertical meridian than along the horizontal meridian for any given eccentricity. This effect was visible in the original column reconstruction illustrated by LeVay et al. (1985; see their Fig. 6A), although they did not mention it. The period of a column pair also varied with eccentricity: columns were wider within the central field than the peripheral field, as noted previously (LeVay et al., 1985). As a result of these two combined gradients, the foveal representation contained the widest column pairs in each mosaic. This feature was more striking in some animals than in others. Monkey 4, chosen for illustration because it had the most obvious enlargement of foveal columns, is shown in Figures4 and 5. In a previous study, we pointed out that some animals have unusually large foveal columns (see Fig. 4, Horton and Hocking, 1996). We doubt that expansion of foveal columns is an artifact of cortical stretching during flat-mounting, because the foveal representation is located in a smooth area of opercular cortex that flattens easily with little distortion (Fig. 1). We do not understand the significance of these large foveal columns, or why they are more pronounced in some animals.
Do columns shrink after monocular enucleation in adults?
In young macaques, monocular enucleation during the critical period of cortical plasticity causes shrinkage of the eye’s ocular dominance columns (Hubel et al., 1977). By contrast, no shrinkage of ocular dominance columns is thought to occur after monocular enucleation in adults. This conclusion is based on a single adult macaque studied by LeVay et al. (1980). Four months after monocular enucleation, this animal received an injection of [3H]proline into the remaining eye. The ratio of column areas serving the normal and missing eye was 1.0:1 in each hemisphere. However, using CO histochemistry, subsequent investigators have obtained conflicting results. Hess and Edwards (1987) reported a dark/light ratio in column widths of 60:40 in opercular cortex 8.5 months after monocular enucleation in adult Cebus apella. In the same species, 4–7 months after monocular enucleation, Rosa et al. (1992) found a “close to 50%” ratio of dark and light stripes in opercular cortex. In the macaque, Trusk et al. (1990) noted that “darkly-stained bands in layer 4Cβ were significantly wider than lightly-stained bands” after monocular enucleation. Finally, in a single M. fascicularis, Florence and Kaas (1992)reported that “CO-dark bands represent 57% of the paired column width contralateral to the intact eye, and 55% ipsilateral to the intact eye.” However, the age at enucleation was unknown, raising the possibility that their monkey could have been immature and, therefore, vulnerable to column shrinkage.
In view of these varied reports, we measured the areas of the dark columns (normal left eye) and light columns (missing right eye) in our mosaics. For opercular cortex, the dark columns averaged 50.8% and the light columns averaged 49.2% (Table 2). For peripheral cortex, the areas were 51.2% dark columns and 48.8% light columns. Taken at face value, these figures suggest slight shrinkage of the enucleated eye’s columns, especially in peripheral cortex. However, there are several reasons we mistrust them. First, the columns are not perfectly sharp, and we might have tended to favor the dark columns when drawing their boundaries. Second, the light columns become difficult to see in peripheral cortex ipsilateral to the enucleated eye, where they appear as small pale islands in a dark sea. By contrast, dark islands in a pale sea on the other side stand out much better. We suspect that this biased our drawings and, therefore, our area measurements in peripheral cortex. Third, the diaminobenzidine reaction product accumulates some distance from the CO catalyst, potentially expanding the dark columns beyond their true width. Fourth, a continuous sheet of dark CO activity remains at the base of layer IVcβ after monocular enucleation (Horton, 1984). If the montage grazes the base of layer IVcβ, it will favor the dark columns. Because of these factors, we believe that negligible shrinkage of ocular dominance columns occurs after monocular enucleation in adults. This conclusion is consistent with a previous study of human striate cortex, showing nearly equal dark and light CO column areas as long as 10 and 23 years after monocular enucleation (Horton and Hedley-Whyte, 1984). Whatever shrinkage occurs is probably caused by transneuronal atrophy in layer IVc, which gives rise to columns visible with Nissl stain (Haseltine et al., 1979), rather than sprouting of axon terminals serving the intact eye.
Imbalance between columns serving the contralateral and ipsilateral eye
LeVay et al. (1985) observed that from the blind spot to the monocular crescent (18° to 60°), the ipsilateral eye’s columns become increasingly attenuated and fragmented. This effect is most evident along the horizontal meridian, where they break into islands (see open arrow, Fig. 3A). As a result, columns driven by the contralateral eye occupy a greater fraction of peripheral cortex than columns of the ipsilateral eye. This imbalance was also seen by Florence and Kaas (1992) and Rosa et al. (1992). In our mosaics, from the “hinge” to the periphery (∼8° to 60°), the contralateral eye/ipsilateral eye ratio of column areas was 63:37. This figure was derived from Table 2 by averaging the percentages for column areas serving the contralateral eye and the ipsilateral eye in the periphery.
We were curious whether this imbalance in contralateral/ipsilateral eye columns extended onto the operculum. Averaging the percentages of opercular column areas, we found a ratio of 52:48, indicating mild predominance of the contralateral eye, even for the central field representation (0° to 8°). The difference was modest and was not evident in every specimen. For several reasons, however, we were inclined to regard it as genuine. First, the confounding issue of bias in favor of the dark columns (reviewed above) cancels out, because the contralateral eye’s columns are dark in half of the cases and light in half of the cases. Second, looking at the opercular portions of the mosaics, occasional islands that occur are more apt to belong to the ipsilateral eye, echoing the tendency in the periphery. Third, in a recent study of column mosaics in newborn macaques, labeled using [3H]proline rather than CO, a slight predominance of the contralateral eye in opercular cortex was also found (Horton and Hocking, 1996).
In local areas of the column mosaics, one eye sometimes predominates strongly over the other, even on the operculum (see bracket, Figs. 2A, 4A). We do not know why these local irregularities occur, but they could be mistaken easily for a deprivation effect if isolated from the context of the whole mosaic. This caveat is relevant to optical imaging studies, which typically sample only a small area of cortex.
DISCUSSION
In 1980, we began flat-mounting striate cortex by cutting off the operculum, splitting the calcarine fissure, and freezing the pieces against a glass slide to create large, smooth surfaces for sectioning. A practical motive, paramount to a graduate student, was to abbreviate the onerous chore of microtoming. Flat-mounting means fewer sections, with huge savings in time, slides, boxes, solutions, etc. For example, LeVay’s original reconstruction required cutting more than 1000 serial horizontal sections through the intact occipital lobe. In the present study, less than 40 sections were cut from each flat-mount. Originally, another motive to prepare flat-mounts was to examine the two-dimensional array formed by CO patches in peripheral regions of striate cortex hidden from direct view, like the blind spot or monocular crescent (Horton, 1984). Dismembering the occipital lobe, however, had the drawback that the fragments did not fit back together and tissue was lost along the margins of the cuts. This problem was solved by Olavarria and Van Sluyters (1985) and Tootell and Silverman (1985), who showed that one could convert convoluted cortex to large tangential sheets by unfolding sulci and gyri. This approach has made it possible to flat-mount striate cortex in a single piece (Anderson et al., 1988; Rosa et al., 1988, 1992; Florence and Kaas, 1992). It reverses the sequence of steps taken by Daniel and Whitteridge (1961), who ingeniously derived a clay surface model of striate cortex by measuring cortical magnification factor, folded it up, cut serial parasagittal sections, and showed its close resemblance to the intact occipital lobe of the monkey.
Combining the flat-mounting technique with computer photomontaging has allowed us to reconstruct 96–99% of the column pattern in each animal with comparative ease. The principal finding to emerge is that a surprising degree of variation exists in the periodicity of ocular dominance columns in a sample of normal, adult macaques with similar V1 area. Along the V1/V2 border, the number of column pairs ranged between 101 and 154 and the average column width ranged from 670 to 395 μm, a factor of 1.7:1. These figures underestimate the true range, because they are based on examination of just six animals.
Flat-mounting striate cortex in both hemispheres of six animals permitted us to verify directly the accuracy of the original computer reconstruction done by LeVay et al. (1985) from serial sections of intact occipital lobe. Their rendition was faithful to the real, unfolded appearance of the ocular dominance columns and remains a breathtaking accomplishment. It was representative of an average macaque, with values for V1 area, perimeter, column widths, and number of column pairs that are close to the means of our reconstructions (Table 2). We can add only a few points to their description of the column pattern. First, the columns are slightly wider along the vertical meridian than the horizontal meridian (except for the contralateral eye’s columns in the far periphery, which become enlarged by fragmentation of the ipsilateral eye’s columns). Second, the fovea contains unusually large columns. This effect was not appreciated by LeVay et al. (1985), perhaps because the foveal region in their reconstruction was disrupted by relieving cuts, which gave it an excessively rounded appearance. Another possibility is that their animal had only modest enlargement of the foveal columns, because this phenomenon is quite variable. Third, the predominance they reported of the contralateral eye in peripheral cortex actually extends into the operculum, although it is a small effect (∼52%/48%), and dissipates the closer the columns get to the foveal representation. Fourth, their reconstruction was too elongated, with a major/minor axis of 2.4:1. This ratio was close to 2.0:1 in all of our specimens.
Löwel (1994) has reported that cats raised with strabismus have coarser ocular dominance columns, a finding questioned by Jones et al. (1996). Anderson et al. (1988) reconstructed the complete pattern of ocular dominance columns in four normal cats, but they did not examine intrinsic variation in column periodicity. Our findings suggest caution before ascribing changes in column width to various manipulations, because so much natural variation exists, at least among macaques. Of course, it is possible that our monkeys with the widest columns had microstrabismus. We examined our monkeys carefully for strabismus and found none. However, one cannot exclude a small-angle deviation unless behavioral testing is done. Probably the most telling point is that ocular dominance columns form before birth in macaques, before strabismus can possibly have any influence on their pattern. Animals vary in ocular dominance column spacing and number right at birth (compare Figs. 2 and 3 in Horton and Hocking, 1996), indicating that this variability is inherent.
Tootell et al. (1988) compared the pattern of ocular dominance columns in opercular flat-mounts from two specimens of M.artoides and one specimen of M.fascicularis. They reported that “dominance strips are significantly wider in M. artoides than in M. fascicularis and this appears to be correlated with a larger brain size and a larger striate area.” Given the variability we have found, it is premature to conclude that certain species of macaques have wider columns or that column width correlates with striate area in macaques. In striate cortex of M. mulatta, just as inM. fascicularis, there is a wide range in the number of sets of columns and in their spacing from animal to animal (Horton and Hocking, in press). We have no explanation for this variability, because we do not understand the factors controlling column dimensions. It would be interesting to examine the patterns of ocular dominance columns in identical twin macaques.
The mammalian cortex is subdivided into a multitude of functional areas, each containing its own columnar architecture. Little attention has been paid to the issue of variation from animal to animal in the periodicity and dimensions of columns in any given cortical area. Ocular dominance columns are among the easiest columns to label in the entire brain and, therefore, provide an excellent means for assessing natural variation in column size. Our data indicate that the width of ocular dominance columns can vary nearly twofold from one animal to the next. This finding has implications for other systems of columns in V1.Hubel and Wiesel (1977) have proposed that striate cortex is diced into modules, comprised of overlapping cycles (called hypercolumns) for each type of column. For these columns to remain in register, i.e., to express a complete set of values within each module, they must be sized proportionately. For example, one would predict that a macaque with especially wide ocular dominance columns would also have wider orientation columns. Unfortunately, we have no data pertaining to orientation columns in our animals, but we have counted the density of CO patches, which fit in register with the ocular dominance columns (Horton and Hubel, 1981). As expected, animals with thin ocular dominance columns have smaller, more numerous patches, whereas the opposite is true in animals with wide columns. For example, the density of CO patches on the operculum of Monkey 3 was well over twice the density of patches on the operculum of Monkey 1 (not illustrated). This finding is consistent with the wide range in total blobs per hemisphere found by Purves and LaMantia (1993) in their survey of normal rhesus monkeys.
Variation in ocular dominance column periodicity should also be reflected in the clumping patterns of geniculocortical afferents, the size of dendritic arbors of cortical cells, the periodicity of the lattice-like arrays of intrinsic horizontal connections (Rockland and Lund, 1983; Lund et al., 1993), and the patchiness of connections from V1 to other cortical areas. It would be of particular interest to learn whether animals with more pairs of ocular dominance columns tend to have more cycles of CO slabs (thick, pale, thin, pale) in V2. This might indicate whether relative column periodicity varies independently by cortical area, or whether it is a correlated, transcendent property of the entire cortex for any given brain.
Footnotes
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jonathan C. Horton, Beckman Vision Center, 10 Kirkham Street, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94143-0730.
This work was supported by grants from the National Eye Institute, That Man May See, Inc., and Research to Prevent Blindness. We thank Dr. Dale Purves and Dr. Geoffrey J. Goodhill for their critical review of this manuscript.