Abstract
Differential-phase-sensitive neurons in the electrosensory lateral line lobe (ELL) of the African electric fish, Gymnarchus niloticus, are sensitive to time disparities on the order of microseconds between afferent action potentials. These action potentials fire in a phase-locked manner in response to the animal’s own wave-type electric organ discharges (EODs) (Kawasaki and Guo, 1996). The time disparity is one of the essential cues for an electrical behavior, the jamming avoidance response (JAR). To gain an insight into the accurate temporal processing in the ELL, firing time accuracy and dynamic response properties of action potentials of the phase-locked neurons (PLNs) in the ELL were examined. The temporal accuracy of the entire neuronal circuit for the JAR was also measured using behavioral responses.
Standard deviation of firing times of PLNs’ action potentials was ∼6 μsec. The PLNs represent zerocrossing times of each stimulus cycle with this accuracy even when stimulus phase was modulated at high frequencies (∼50 Hz). Distinct JAR occurred when time disparity was diminished below 1 μsec, and a marginal JAR could still be detected with a time disparity of 100 nsec. Standard deviation of the firing times of EODs was approximately several hundred nanoseconds. This stability of the EOD, however, was demonstrated to be unnecessary for the JAR. JARs occurred even when a large artificial jitter (∼60 μsec) was introduced to a stimulus that mimicked fish’s own EOD and the time disparity for JAR was diminished to 1 μsec. This immunity of JAR to the EOD jitter is explained by the insensitivity of the differential-phase-sensitive neurons in the ELL to a common phase modulation.
The JAR of the South American electric fish, Eigenmannia, also occurs in response to stimuli that generate comparably small phase differences (Rose and Heiligenberg, 1985b; Carr et al., 1986a). The present study revealed that the independently evolvedEigenmannia and Gymnarchus exhibit a comparative level of remarkable temporal accuracy.
- phase
- phase-locked neurons
- electric fish
- jamming avoidance response
- phase comparison
- binaural comparison
- convergent evolution
- adaptation
- vector strength
Behavioral studies of the auditory system indicate that temporal disparities on the order of microseconds can be resolved by the CNS. Human subjects can localize a sound source with an accuracy of ∼1° using small differences in sound arrival times between two ears (Mills, 1972). Several species of vertebrate animals also use small interaural time differences to localize sound sources (Rabizza and Masterton, 1972; Brown et al., 1978; Knudsen et al., 1979; Simmons et al., 1983). Although such behavioral experiments clearly demonstrate the ability of the CNS to deal with temporal codes of microseconds or even submicroseconds, the physiological mechanisms underlying this capability are not fully understood.
Kawasaki and Guo (1996) recently discovered that neurons in an early stage of central processing of electrosensory information in an African electric fish, Gymnarchus niloticus, exhibit a sensitivity to time disparities between electrosensory inputs on the order of microseconds.
A “wave”-type electric fish, Gymnarchus, constantly emits cyclic electric organ discharges (EODs) at individually fixed frequencies (300–400 Hz). An electric field is thus established around the body, and its distortion by objects is detected by electroreceptors located over the body surfaces (Lissmann and Machin, 1958). When two fish with similar discharge frequencies meet, they shift their discharge frequencies away from each other to avoid mutual jamming of their electrolocation systems (Bullock et al., 1975). This jamming avoidance response (JAR) requires the detection of time disparities between sensory signals at different electroreceptors (Kawasaki, 1993a). The signal phases, or zerocrossing times of the electrosensory signal received by electroreceptors, are conveyed in a phase-locked manner to the first brain station, the electrosensory lateral line lobe (ELL), by S-type afferent fibers. While directly projecting to the inner cell layer (ICL) of the ELL, collateral axons of the S-type afferents synapse onto the giant cells of the ELL, which in turn project to the ICL. Because the S-type afferent fibers and giant cells, collectively referred to as phase-locked neurons (PLNs) in this study, fire one action potential for each zerocrossing of the electrosensory signal with a certain latency, times of zerocrossing of signals at different electroreceptors are represented at the ICL as the times of the action potentials of PLNs. Kawasaki and Guo (1996) describe physiological and morphological characteristics of these PLNs that give input to differential-phase-sensitive neurons that are sensitive to time disparities on the order of single-digit microseconds (Fig.1).
Possible physiological requirements for such neuronal sensitivity to small time disparities include accurate representation of temporal information by PLNs that provide synaptic input to the differential-phase-sensitive neurons. In this study, temporal accuracy and dynamics of individual PLNs in the ELL were examined. Standard deviation of firing times, or jitter, of PLNs was as small as 5.6 μsec on average.
Expression of this neuronal accuracy for temporal processing in behavioral output was also examined by measuring thresholds for time disparities for the JAR. Gymnarchus was found to exhibit JARs in response to stimulus patterns capable of generating time disparities among electroreceptor afferents of only a few hundred nanoseconds.
These results were compared with those obtained previously in an independently evolved gymnotiform electric fish,Eigenmannia, which performs the same type of JARs (Rose and Heiligenberg, 1985b; Carr et al., 1986a,b).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals and physiological recording. Approximately 50Gymnarchus niloticus (13–20 cm) were used. Environmental conditions in the holding tanks were identical to those described inKawasaki (1994). For both physiological and behavioral experiments, intramuscular injection of Flaxedil (gallamine triethiodide, 0.03%, 3–8 μl) immobilized fish and greatly attenuated EOD amplitude. Oxygen-saturated water was provided through a mouthpiece to maintain the immobilized preparation.
After local application of Xylocane (2%), a small hole was drilled in the skull above the corpus cerebelli. The caudal part of the ELL was exposed by removing the caudal edge of the corpus cerebelli with fine suction tubing (inner diameter 500 μm). Fish were gently held with a sponge-lined clamp and submerged in water (tank size ∼40 × 40 × 10 cm) except for a small area around the skull opening. Intracellular recordings from PLNs were made with sharp glass-capillary electrodes (30–50 MΩ, 3 m KCl) in the deep fiber layer of the ELL (Bass and Hopkins, 1982). This layer was localized by recording field potentials in the ELL that show systematic change of waveform along the depth (see Fig. 2 in Kawasaki and Guo, 1996) with a glass-capillary electrode (5–10 μm tip diameter, 3 mNaCl). No anatomical confirmation was made to distinguish recordings from the terminals of the S-type afferent fibers and giant neurons.
Attenuated EODs were recorded in the immobilized preparation to register JAR by suction electrode attached to the tail. EODs were also measured in intact fish that were freely swimming but voluntarily hiding in a plastic tubing (diameter 5 cm; length 15 cm, slightly shorter than that of the fish). Recording electrodes for EODs were attached at both ends of the tubing.
Stimulus presentation. A sinusoidal signal, S1, which simulated the fish’s own EOD, was applied between a silver wire electrode in the mouth and an electrode near the tail. This electrode pair simulated the amplitude and geometry of the electric field generated by the fish’s own EODs before being silenced with Flaxedil. S1 was generated by a phase synthesizer (Wavetek, 650) with a phase accuracy of 35 nsec. The frequency of S1 was set constant to be near that of fish’s EOD before immobilization. For measuring dynamic phase responses, S1 was sinusoidally modulated in phase by applying a sinusoidal signal to the phase-modulation input of the phase synthesizer. To add artificial jitter in S1, noise was generated with a white-noise generator (Hewlett Packard, HP-8057A), filtered (Krohn Hite, 3550R, passband 20–200 Hz), and fed into the phase modulation port of the phase synthesizer. The phase modulation of S1 by the noise was measured and expressed by the same method as for measuring neuronal jitter (see below).
To induce JAR, a sinusoidal signal, S2, which simulated the EOD of a neighboring fish, was applied through an independent electrode pair straddling the fish. The frequency of S2 was switched every 30 sec between 2 Hz higher and 2 Hz lower than that of S1. A 4 min session consisted of eight alternating 30 sec periods of frequency differences, +2 and −2 Hz. The magnitude of JAR was measured as an average of responses during these eight consecutive periods. For each period, timings of every 100 EODs were registered with a divide-by-100 counter (see below). Thus, EOD frequency was averaged over ∼250 msec and sampled ∼4 times/sec. The change in frequency was integrated over each of the 30 sec periods and converted to the unit of Hz/min. The sign of the frequency shift in each period was defined as positive when JAR occurred in the expected direction and as negative when frequency shift occurred in the opposite direction. Percentage of correct responses was calculated as the number of periods in which a positive frequency shift occurred by dividing by 8. The sign test shows that 100% (8/8) and 87.5% (7/8) correct responses correspond to p = 0.0039 and p = 0.0351, respectively. Cases of <75% (6/8, p = 0.14) are all statistically insignificant. The threshold for the JAR was determined by attenuating S2 with a custom-made signal attenuator the S/N ratio of which was >100 dB.
Measurement and expression of phase. Intracellular potentials of PLNs were recorded with sharp glass-capillary electrodes filled with 3 m KCl that were connected to an electrometer (WPI, S-7071A). Recording noise was <0.5 mV. When two PLNs were recorded simultaneously, they were from one side of the ELL. The capacitive compensation circuit in the electrometer was turned off because it introduced a high-frequency (>10 kHz) noise that disrupted accurate measure of zerocrossing times. Intracellular potentials were amplified by 60 dB with an AC-coupled operational amplifier (TL074) specially installed within the electrometer. This amplified signal was AC-coupled further, clipped at ± 0.7 V by diodes, and sent to a custom-made Schmitt trigger circuit that compared input signals against a stable reference of 0 V and generated TTL pulses associated with zerocrossings. The 60 dB amplification greatly eliminated artifact jitter associated with the Schmitt triggering of high-frequency noise components introduced in the recording and reference signal. This notion was supported by the fact that further amplification (80 dB) did not improve jitter measure. EODs recorded from intact fish were also amplified by 60 dB before being sent to the Schmitt trigger circuit.
Successive positive zerocrossing times, ti , of action potentials of PLNs were registered by a Schmitt trigger circuit of a time-stamp recorder (Tucker-Davis Technology, ET-1) with a resolution of 100 nsec. Two time-stamp recorders were used to measure phase differences between a stimulus and action potentials of a PLN, or between two PLNs. These recorders were commonly driven by a single crystal oscillator to ensure an accurate measure of phase differences.
A successive series of phases, pi , is expressed in μsec as: Equation 1where ti are zerocrossing times of action potentials and ri (reference times) are the predicted zerocrossing times of an ideally constant signal with no jitter. ri values were obtained either by hardware synchronization pulses from the stimulus generator or by computing with sufficient accuracy as: Equation 2where n is the number of time stamps, which was typically ∼3500 for the standard 10 sec sampling period. These two methods yielded indistinguishable ri because the hardware synchronization pulses from the signal generator and the computation were equally accurate (<35 nsec).
Standard deviation, amplitude component of Fourier spectrum, time series, and its probability distribution function ofpi were computed and plotted by a 486 PC (Gateway 2000, DX486/33) using custom software.
Jitter of EOD was measured by a similar method. As for PLNs, EOD was amplified by 60 dB and sent to the same time-stamp recorder, which produced a series of time stamps, ti . A series of periods, qi , was computed as: Equation 3Gymnarchus often showed a very slow drift in EOD frequency that yielded a continuous change ofqi . When this happened, measurement was halted until the fish stopped such drift.
Standard deviation (ς) of pi andqi is hereafter referred to as jitter.
Estimation of phase difference between PLNs during the JAR.Magnitude of phase modulation of the signal mixture of S1and S2 is a function of the amplitude ratio of S1 and S2, ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ (Heiligenberg and Bastian, 1980a; Kawasaki, 1993b). The magnitude is computed as: Equation 4where T is the period of S1. φ, however, differs across the body areas because of different ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ ratios at different areas. ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ ratios are different because electric field vectors by fish’s own and neighbor’s EOD form different angles at different body areas. These differences in φ create phase differences between body areas, which are one of the essential cues for the JAR (see Fig. 3 in Kawasaki, 1993b). Therefore, an ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ ratio measured at a particular point on the body surface does not give an estimate of the magnitude of phase modulation for individual PLNs that represent different areas of the body surface. Distribution of the magnitude of phase modulation of PLNs at a given stimulus condition was examined by recording intracellularly from 220 PLNs in the ELL while ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ was set to 0.2 as measured at a standard location, the gill cover. There ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ was measured by a pair of fork electrodes (1 cm separation), one pole of which was placed close to the gill cover. The two poles of the pair were oriented at the angle perpendicular to the gill cover. The frequency difference between S1 and S2 was set to 2 Hz. Magnitudes of phase modulation in PLN action potentials, which occurred at 2 Hz in this condition, were computed from the 2 Hz component of the amplitude spectrum of pi . Mean, standard deviation, and the minimum and maximum of the magnitude of phase modulation of 220 PLNs were 16, 10, 0.3, and 48%, respectively, of the estimated magnitude from the ratio ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ at the standard location by Equation 4. “Mean phase modulation” in this study is defined as estimated modulation depths by Equation 4multiplied by 16%. The high estimate of ‖S2‖/‖S1‖ measured at the standard location is probably because S1 current density rapidly falls with the distance from the gill cover behind which one of the current sources was located, and the pair of recording electrodes with 1 cm separation did not effectively measure the local current density at the surface of the gill cover. Because the right and left sides experience phase modulations that are 180° out of synchronization (see Fig. 10 in Heiligenberg and Rose, 1985), mean phasedifferences between the two sides available to the differential-phase-sensitive neurons in the ELL are twice as large as “mean phase modulation.”
RESULTS
Accuracy of phase representation by PLNs
We sampled pi of intracellularly recorded action potentials from 134 PLNs in the ELL (Fig.2). Figure 2A showspi of action potentials from a representative PLN whose jitter was 7.8 μsec. Jitters of 134 PLNs were tightly distributed around the mean of 6.5 μsec. A jitter of <10 μsec was shown in 95% of recorded PLNs, and the most accurate PLN showed a jitter of 1.3 μsec (Fig. 2B). The frequency contents of pi shown by these neurons were examined by Fourier analysis. As shown in Figure 2C, the amplitude spectrum of pi was flat except for a prominent peak at 60 Hz. The 60 Hz peak was seen in all cases studied and is believed to be the power line frequency contamination through the recording system. Thus, a neuronal noise with a wide range of frequencies contributed to the jitter. To assess the contribution of the 60 Hz peak to jitter magnitude, pi were digitally filtered against 60 Hz and pi was reconstructed by reverse FFT. This procedure reduced the mean of standard deviations by 14%, yielding 5.6 μsec (Fig.2B).
Jitter correlation between PLNs
Jitter in individual PLNs could be caused by the following: (1) noise commonly affecting all PLNs, such as inherent noise in S1 and noise in electroreceptors that may be coupled to each other (Szabo, 1962); and (2) noise arising independently in individual PLNs. Differential-phase-sensitive neurons in the ELL, which detect phase differences between inputs from PLNs, must be affected by the latter type of noise but may be immune to the noise common in all PLNs. To investigate differential jitter between PLNs, we made simultaneous intracellular recordings from 40 pairs of PLNs. Jitter in individual neurons, jitter of phase differences between the two PLNs, and correlation coefficients between pi of the two neurons are presented in Table 1. Correlation coefficients were small, indicating that a major source of jitter can be attributed to a noise arising independently in different PLNs.
Dynamic responses of PLNs
We examined dynamics of the PLNs by modulating the phase of S1 at different frequencies and measured the dynamic responses of the resulting phase modulation in PLNs (Fig.3). All neurons responded at near unity gain from 0.4 to 15 Hz. Gain was reduced to 0.5 ± 0.12 at 100 Hz (Fig.3C). Even with a large modulation (50 μsec), the accuracy with which the PLN action potentials encoded stimulus phase was preserved, as shown in the jitter of the top traces in Figure 3,A and B. Although gain was constant up to 15 Hz, different neurons may have shown different phase delay, which may have introduced phase differences among PLNs. To examine this possibility, we recorded simultaneously from two PLNs and measured phase differences during sinusoidal modulations of S1 phase at different frequencies. As shown in Figure 3, D and E, two neurons sampled simultaneously were phase-modulated in synchrony, leaving only small phase differences even at high modulation frequencies (∼100 Hz). These data indicate that amplitude and time course of phase modulation in S1 are accurately represented by PLNs from low frequencies up to a few tens of Hertz.
Behavioral threshold for the JAR
The limit of Gymnarchus’ ability to detect small phase differences was tested by measuring JAR under conditions in which phase difference was progressively reduced. The phase-chamber stimulus delivery method (Rose and Heiligenberg, 1985b; Kawasaki, 1993a,b), in which body surface was electrically divided into two sections, provides phase differences with a known minimum value. Although this method yielded robust JARs when phase differences exceeded tens of microseconds, it failed to evoke any detectable JAR when phase differences fell below 10 μsec in our preliminary experiments. The free-field stimulus delivery method (Kawasaki, 1993a), in which S1 and S2 were mixed in the experimental tank, however, was much more effective in evoking JAR when phase differences were smaller. Therefore, the free-field condition was used to measure the behavioral threshold for phase differences. Phase differences were estimated as described in Materials and Methods. Figure4A shows JAR measured under progressively smaller phase modulations. Figure 4Bshows percent correct responses with various mean phase modulations from eight individuals. JAR always occurred in the correct direction with mean phase modulation of >1 μsec. When mean phase modulation was <1 μsec, 16 of 36 behavioral sessions yielded 100% correct responses. Even when the mean phase modulation was <200 nsec, 10 of 16 sessions showed ≥87.5% correct responses. JAR requires not only phase modulation but also concurrent amplitude modulation (Kawasaki, 1993a). Because the magnitude of local amplitude modulation is a strict function of ‖S2‖/‖S1‖, one can infer corresponding magnitude of amplitude modulation. The corresponding amplitude modulation at the behavioral threshold was 0.02%.
Jitter of EODs
Jitter of EODs was measured from 11 individuals. As shown in Figure 5, the jitter of EOD was generally <1 μsec (0.65 ± 0.19 μsec, n = 11). For two particularly stable individuals, jitter was 0.38 ± 0.05 μsec.
JAR with artificial jitter in S1
Is this high degree of stability of EOD necessary for the detection of small phase differences for the JAR? We artificially introduced jitter in S1 and measured JAR in curarized fish using the same experimental procedure as in Figure 4, except that S1 was artificially jittered. Under this condition, all PLNs should experience the introduced jitter commonly; thus, no phasedifferences should be created between PLNs by jitter. The magnitude of JAR and percentage of correct responses were compared in three conditions, i.e., ςS1 = 0 (no jitter), ςS1 = 20 μsec, and ςS1 = 60 μsec, with progressively smaller amplitudes of S2 and, thus, of differential phase modulation. As shown in Figure 6, JARs occurred with comparable magnitudes and accuracy even when differential phase modulation was in the nanosecond range and S1 contained a large jitter that was 100-fold larger than the phase differences.
DISCUSSION
Accurate representation of temporal information by PLNs
A major result of this study is that phase, i.e., zerocrossing times of electrosensory signals at electroreceptors, is represented by firing times of individual PLNs in the ELL with an accuracy of a few microseconds (Fig. 2). Corresponding vector strength, a measure of the degree of phase locking as defined by Goldberg and Brown (1969), for the mean jitter (5.6 μsec) in this study is 0.9998. This value is far greater than that typically found in the VIIIth nerve of the mammalian auditory system (Anderson et al., 1970). The highest degree of neural phase locking in the vertebrate auditory system is found in the barn owl, which uses small interaural time differences for sound localization (Knudsen et al., 1979). Neurons in the nucleus of magnocellularis of the barn owl show vector strength from 0.5 to 0.7 at low frequencies, which corresponds to a jitter of several tens of microseconds (Sullivan and Konishi, 1984; Carr and Konishi, 1990). Jitter measurement in phase-locked neurons of an electric fish,Eigenmannia, (Rose and Heiligenberg, 1985b; Carr et al., 1986a) shows somewhat larger values than those found here inGymnarchus. The temporal accuracy of the PLNs ofGymnarchus roughly corresponds to the sensitivity of differential-phase-sensitive neurons in the ELL, which are presumed to receive direct inputs from PLNs (Kawasaki and Guo, 1996).
Temporal sensitivity expressed in the JAR
Gymnarchus always showed 100% correct JARs when mean phase modulation was >1 μsec (Fig. 4). Distinctive JARs were often recorded with mean phase modulation of a few hundred nanoseconds. The behavioral sensitivity to microsecond time disparities is not unique toGymnarchus; similar or even better temporal sensitivity has been shown in the electrosensory system of a gymnotiform electric fish as well as other vertebrate auditory systems (Rabizza and Masterton, 1972; Brown et al., 1978; Knudsen et al., 1979; Simmons et al., 1983). Despite independent evolution of the electrosensory and electrogeneric systems (Lauder and Liem, 1983), gymnotiform electric fishEigenmannia performs JARs that are very similar to those ofGymnarchus (Bullock et al., 1975) using identical computational algorithms, consisting of multiple steps of amplitude and phase processing (Bullock et al., 1975; Heiligenberg et al., 1978;Heiligenberg and Bastian, 1980b; Heiligenberg, 1991; Kawasaki, 1993a,1996). Eigenmannia exhibits comparable phase sensitivity to that found in this study (Rose and Heiligenberg, 1985b; Carr et al., 1986a). The brain structures for phase comparison, however, differ in these two species of electric fishes. The phase comparison circuit exists in a midbrain structure, the torus semicircularis, inEigenmannia (Rose and Heiligenberg, 1985a, 1986a,b; Carr et al., 1986b) but in a hindbrain structure, the ELL, inGymnarchus (Kawasaki and Guo, 1996).
A possible role of the differential-phase system in electrolocation
In both Gymnarchus and Eigenmannia, a larger difference in frequency (more than tens of Hertz) between a fish’s own and a neighbor’s EOD does not induce JAR and, correspondingly, has little effect on electrolocation performance (Heiligenberg, 1973,1975)—frequency differences of a few Hertz have the strongest effects on the JAR and interference effects on the electrolocation performance. From those observations, one can speculate that the functional significance of the JAR is to increase the beat frequency resulting from EODs of two fish by mutually creating a larger frequency difference (Bullock et al., 1972; Heiligenberg, 1977, 1991). Although both Gymnarchus and Eigenmannia shift the discharge frequency correctly in their JAR when phase differences are small, the magnitude of the JARs in such a condition is too small to create a large frequency difference. Thus, the small and slow JARs near the threshold conditions may not serve to improve electrolocation performances.
This highly sensitive differential-phase system, however, may have a functional significance for electrolocation itself while no jamming signals exist. As reported by Kawasaki and Guo (1996, see Fig. 12G), PLNs in the ELL give rise to action potentials with different latencies for different stimulus amplitudes. Modulation of stimulus amplitude by 10% can result in a latency shift of PLNs’ spiking by several tens of microseconds. The accuracy of PLN spiking found in this study implies that an electrolocation target may differentially shift the firing times of PLNs by local amplitude modulation, and differential-phase information associated with the target may be available to the differential-phase-sensitive neurons in the ELL. Amplitude-dependent latency shift in phase-coding afferent fibers, the T-afferents, of Eigenmannia has been examined but has not been measured with sufficient accuracy (Bullock and Chichibu, 1965; Heiligenberg and Partridge, 1981) to test a possible role of the phase system in electrolocation. Thus, the differential-phase system of Gymnarchus andEigenmannia may be of multifunctional serving for both electrolocation and JAR in both genera. The role of the differential-phase system in electrolocation needs to be examined by recording from differential-phase-sensitive neurons while presenting electrolocation targets in both genera. The same experiments are needed in Sternopygus, which does not perform JAR (Matsubara, 1982;Rose et al., 1987), and in pulse gymnotiform species that do not use phase information for their JARs (Heiligenberg, 1980). Such comparative analysis should provide insight into the evolutionary origin of the differential-phase system.
Electrolocation objects with capacitance should also evoke local phase shifts. Involvement of phase-sensitive systems for detection of capacitive objects has been implicated in other electric fish species (Meyer, 1982; von der Emde and Ringer, 1992; von der Emde and Bell, 1994).
Dynamic responses of PLNs
As shown in Figure 3A–C, PLNs follow the phase modulation of a stimulus up to several tens of Hertz and thus could encode the rapid changes in electrosensory signals. If each PLN action potential were a response strictly to a past cycle of stimulus with a fixed latency, however, the dynamic phase responses of PLNs would be flat over higher frequencies up to the signal’s carrier frequency. The sharp decline of gain spectrum at higher frequencies (Fig. 3C) suggests that PLNs and their associated electroreceptors may have a resonance property by which a firing time of a particular action potential is affected by the past several cycles. In such conditions, response phases to a rapid dynamic phase modulation may differ from neuron to neuron, and this may introduce phase differences between their responses. To test this notion, simultaneous recordings from pairs of PLNs were performed (Fig.3D,E). They revealed that PLN phases are commonly modulated by phase modulations of stimuli even with a high-frequency phase modulation. Thus, little differential modulation between these neurons is introduced as long as the neurons are commonly driven by a single signal source.
Stability of EODs
EOD jitter measured in this study confirms the measurement of EOD jitter by Bullock et al. (1975). The frequency “singing” described in Bullock et al. (1975), however, was not observed, and all individuals showed stable EOD in a given measurement period (10 sec).
JAR under S1 with artificial jitter
The rapid dynamics of phase responses in the PLNs (Fig. 3) implied that differential-phase information would be preserved even when a carrier signal (EOD or S1) contained jitter. This hypothesis was tested by adding artificial jitter in S1(Fig. 6). The experiment demonstrated that Gymnarchusperforms normal JARs even when unnaturally large jitter (ς = 20–60 μsec) is introduced in S1 and a minimal phase difference that is only a small fraction of the jitter was available to the fish. Thus, phase stability in the EOD is not required to detect small phase differences.
Emergence of the behavioral accuracy
There are 10- to 100-fold differences in temporal accuracy expressed in the behavior (JAR) and in the differential-phase-sensitive neurons in the ELL of Gymnarchus. A possible explanation of the difference is a spatial averaging of temporal information by a large number of differential-phase-sensitive neurons. The significance of the spatial averaging of electrosensory signal for the JAR ofEigenmannia is demonstrated behaviorally (Rose and Heiligenberg, 1985b), and a neuronal correlate of the behavioral accuracy is found in a premotor nucleus, the prepacemaker nucleus ofEigenmannia (Kawasaki et al., 1988a,b). InGymnarchus, neuronal circuits for JAR beyond the ELL (Kawasaki and Guo, 1996) that possibly perform such spatial averaging have not yet been studied.
Footnotes
This study was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health Grant R29 MH48115-01A1 and by a Research Scientist Development Award K-02 MH01256-01 from ADAMHA to M.K. We thank Yasuko Kawasaki for figure preparation, two anonymous referees for critical comments, and Cameron McLaughlin for editing English.
Correspondence should be addressed to Masashi Kawasaki, Department of Biology, Gilmer Hall, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22903.