Abstract
Avian vocalizations are generally understood to play a pivotal role in reproductive functions. The role of the hypothalamus in gonadotropin release in higher vertebrates including birds is well established. To date, however, a direct linkage between the neuronal processing of vocal input and the contingent luteinizing hormone (LH) response has not been demonstrated. In this study, using female ring doves, we recorded neuronal activity from hypothalamic nuclei that, as we have shown previously, receive acoustic inputs from the auditory thalamic relay. Concurrently with recording single-unit responses to stimulation with species-specific coo vocalizations, we sampled LH levels in blood from the pituitary veins. LH concentration in the plasma was significantly elevated in birds hearing species-typical coos but not in birds exposed to experimentally altered coos or white noise or in birds that received no vocal stimulation. We found two types of neurons in the preoptic and anterior hypothalamus that selectively responded to the female nest coo: excitatory units and inhibitory units. Among the excitatory neurons are units characterized by two bursts separated by a period of slow spiking or complete silence, in a pattern approximately corresponding temporally to the two-note coo. We designate them as female-nest-coo-specific units. Most neurons in the posterior hypothalamus were nonselective in their response. Female nest coo and male nest coo stimulation evoked an equal magnitude of discharge changes from responsive units in the preoptic-anterior hypothalamic area. We found, however, that the LH increment was three times greater for birds hearing female nest coos than for birds hearing male nest coos. These observations suggest that feature-detecting neurons such as the female-nest-coo-specific units are involved in gonadotropin-releasing hormone output. The present findings are consistent with the well established behavioral evidence that female nest coos mediate ovarian growth.
- vocalization
- sound stimulation of endocrine response
- hypothalamus
- auditory processing
- GnRH
- call-specific neuronal discharge
Neuroendocrine control of reproduction is remarkably conserved throughout the vertebrate subphylum (Muske, 1993). Of the various neuropeptides and neurotransmitters synthesized in the hypothalamus that are involved in reproduction, the gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) controls the release of luteinizing hormone (LH) from the anterior pituitary and triggers the cascade of endocrine responses that culminate in ovulation (Silverman et al., 1994). This exquisite machinery is adapted to the particular environmental niche and social organization of each species (Marshall, 1942; Lehrman, 1961; Bronson, 1989; Ball, 1993; Wingfield et al., 1994). Successful breeding thus relies on selective processing and encoding of relevant sensory information within the hypothalamus. The importance of communication signals in reproduction has been shown in a wide variety of vertebrate classes (Wingfield et al., 1994). Vocal communication is one of the forms most intensively studied, especially in frogs and birds (Capranica and Moffatt, 1983; Cheng, 1992, 1993;Wilczynski et al., 1994). In many bird species, courtship calls play a crucial role in breeding success (Lehrman, 1961; Kroodsma, 1976; Wingfield et al., 1994). In the ring dove (Streptopelia risoria), the male typically initiates courtship with bow cooing and nest cooing, to which the female responds with her own nest cooing. In 5–7 d after the female begins nest cooing, she lays a clutch of one or two eggs. Muting the female blocks her own ovarian growth, whereas playing back the female’s nest coo reinstates such growth (Cheng, 1992), and a deafened female’s movements associated with the nest-coo display can stimulate moderate ovarian growth (Cheng et al., 1988). These findings suggest that both auditory cues and the motor act of displaying are involved in ovarian development.
To determine the neuronal networks that subserve these endocrine effects of nest coos, we applied anterograde and retrograde tracing methods. We found massive hypothalamic afferent projections from the shell region of the auditory thalamic nucleus (Durand et al., 1992) and minor projections from the midbrain vocal control nucleus (Cheng and Zuo, 1994). That the hypothalamic recipients of these projections respond to sound was subsequently confirmed in an antidromic electrophysiological study (Cheng and Peng, 1997). These findings provide the impetus for the present test of the hypothesis that coo-sensitive neuronal units in the hypothalamus are involved in LH release. We demonstrate biologically significant sound stimulation of LH release by combining playbacks of the species-specific courtship call, recording of hypothalamic neuronal activity, and sampling of anterior pituitary secretory output in the same experimental preparation.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects. Ring doves (Streptopelia risoria) were bred at the Institute of Animal Behavior facility at Rutgers University under a 14/10 hr light/dark photoperiod. At the time of testing, the birds were 135–150 d of age and weighed 150–180 gm. A different set of birds was used in each of two experiments: 23 females and 1 male were used in experiment I, and 11 females and 2 males were used in experiment II. All birds in the experiments were anesthetized with urethane (20% w/v solution, 0.4 ml/kg, i.m.; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) and fixed in a Kopf small stereotaxic device with hollow ear bars.
In experiment I, blood samples were drawn at the conclusion of each sound stimulation and recording session. In experiment II, blood samplings were performed concurrently with sound stimulation and recording. Birds were tracheotomized before being placed in a Kopf device with the ventral surface of the brain exposed, allowing both recordings and blood samplings from the ventral surface.
Acoustic stimuli. Acoustic stimuli used in this study were as follows: (1) female nest coo, (2) male nest coo, (3) reversed female nest coo, (4) reversed male nest coo, and (5) male bow coo (an agnostic coo emitted only by males, used in a few trials). To obtain the stimuli, multiple coo vocalizations were recorded from 16 different individuals, using a Marantz PMD 430 stereo cassette tape recorder. Recordings were made with a Sennheiser microphone placed 4 inches above the nest bowl and connected to the tape recorder outside the acoustic isolation chamber. The behavioral context of each coo recorded was observed and noted. Manipulation and analysis of vocalizations using a computerized analysis and resynthesis system have been described (Margoliash, 1983; Yu and Margoliash, 1996). Analog recordings of the nest coos were digitized at a sampling rate of 22.6 kHz at 16 bit resolution on a Gateway (North Sioux City, SD) 2000 486/33 computer equipped with a DT 2821 analog-to-digital board (Data Translation, Marlboro, MA) and displayed spectrographically on the computer screen using SIGNAL sound analysis software, version 2.23 (Engineering Design, Belmont, MA). Reversed coos were created using the “reverse time buffer” command. Computer-generated 500 msec white noise bursts [two/sec at 75 dB sound pressure level (SPL)] with a bandwidth of 0–11 kHz were used. All vocalizations were standardized to an intercall interval of 2 sec and to a maximum amplitude of 75 ± 2 dB SPL. The tape of a female’s nest coos was edited to contain a high number of coos without noise or other artifacts. Each session lasted 280 sec, with a between-sessions pause of 60 sec. Tapes of a male nest coo, a reversed female nest coo, a reversed male nest coo, and white noise (100 sec) were similarly edited. In addition, a composite tape was created consisting of three parts in order: white noise (100 sec), female nest coo (280 sec), and reversed female coo (100 sec), with a 60 sec interval between parts. When recording from the ventromedial nucleus (VMN), bow coo stimulation was added. The edited but undigitized tape of coos was used when tracking responsive units. Once a responsive unit was located, we switched to the computer resynthesized recordings for further study of the response properties of the unit. Each coo was presented 10–12 times, with 4–6 sec between trials. When the response properties of the same unit to different coos were studied, different sounds were presented in random sequences and repeated 10–20 times. For the 22 birds from which we also collected blood samples, a single stimulus was presented.
Sounds were presented via either a Yamaha NS 10M loudspeaker placed in front of the birds (n = 6) at a distance of ∼80 cm or a Sony earphone (diameter, 3.5 cm) within a cone-shaped box connected to the hollow ear bars. The loudspeaker was calibrated in decibels SPL using a calibrated condenser microphone (Bruel-Kjaer 4135) placed to duplicate the position of the bird’s head in the stereotaxic instrument. The output of the earphones was calibrated with a ½ inch condenser microphone (Bruel-Kjaer 4135) attached to a funneled probe that was inserted into the opening of each hollow ear bar through a small canal at its center (Biederman-Thorson, 1970a,b). The peak amplitude of the sound stimuli was set to between 70 and 75 dB sound pressure level with a manual attenuator. The outputs of different acoustic stimuli were monitored by the storage oscilloscope.
Recordings. Extracellular recordings were made from single cells located within the preoptic area (POA), the anterior medial hypothalamus (AMH), or the VMN. Single cells were recorded with glass micropipettes (tip, 1–3 μm; 6–15 MΩ impedance by BL-1000B microelectrode tester) filled with 2% potamine sky blue (PSB; ICN Biomedicals, Cleveland, OH) in 3 m NaCl. In experiment I, recording microelectrodes were inserted into the POA, AMH, or VMN from the dorsal plane with a Burleigh Piezoelectric Microstepper. Coordinates for placement of recording electrodes were as follows: POA, anterior, 4.8–5.2 mm; lateral, 0.6–0.8 mm; and ventral, 7.5–8.0 mm; AMH, anterior, 3.5–4.0 mm; lateral, 0.5–0.8 mm; and ventral, 7.9–8.3 mm; and VMN, anterior, 2.6–3.0 mm; lateral, 0.4–0.7 mm; and ventral, 8.0–8.4 mm. In experiment II, the recording microelectrodes were lowered at a 5–8° angle from the vertical line at an entry point 1–1.5 mm lateral to the midline of the optic chiasm to POA or AMH under the visual guidance of a Zeiss operation microscope. Micropipettes were connected to a preamplifier by a 0.12 mm wire of platinum lead. Single-unit signals were amplified by a Neurodata IR-283 preamplifier, monitored with a Marantz 420 audio monitor, and displayed together with acoustic stimuli on a Tektronix 5110 storage oscilloscope. A Nicolet 4094C digital oscilloscope was linked to a Macintosh IIx computer via a National Instrument NB-GPIB board for collection on-line in the form of continuous rate meter and perievent histogram records.
Records were stored on magnetic tape for off-line analysis by the software program SCOPE (courtesy of J. Tepper, Rutgers University), using a Macintosh IIx computer and a National Instrument NB-MI0 l6L multifunction board. The neurons responding to acoustic stimulus were studied and classified by the poststimulus and peristimulus time histogram. The number of spikes evoked by each stimulus was recorded for the duration of the stimulus, and the series was repeated at 75 ± 2 dB SPL for each unit. The spikes were counted digitally. A response was considered excitatory when the firing rate during an acoustic stimulus was increased at least 15% above the control levels of background firing; a response was considered inhibitory when spontaneous discharge was depressed at least 15% by an acoustic stimulus. The spontaneous spike rate was measured 5–10 sec before the onset of an acoustic stimulus. The stimulus spike rate was defined as the number of spikes produced during the stimulus divided by the duration of the stimulus.
Blood sampling procedure. Birds were anesthetized with urethane (0.4 ml/kg, i.m.). The procedure used in withdrawing blood from the pituitary veins has been described (Huang et al., 1995; Peng et al., 1996). In brief, the pituitary gland and the ventral diencephalon were exposed parapharyngeally under the visual guidance of a Zeiss dissecting microscope. Using a glass cannula with a polyethylene tube, blood was drained from the veinal region of the pituitary gland and collected into a vessel at 10–20 min intervals for a duration of 120 min. Blood samples were immediately centrifuged at 10,000 × g for 1 min. Serum samples were stored at −20° C until radioimmunoassay (RIA) for LH. In experiment I, blood samplings at 10–20 min intervals for 120 min commenced after the sound stimulation and recording session, which lasted 50–70 min. The pituitary gland was surgically exposed immediately after freeing the bird from the recording position. This procedure usually took 15–25 min. In experiment II, blood samples were collected at 10–20 min intervals for 120 min while the recording and sound stimulation were in progress; recording and sound stimulation were allowed to run for 40–50 min before the first sampling was performed.
Localization of recording electrodes. The position of each recording electrode was marked by iontophoresis of PSB by passing 5–10 μm negative current for 10–20 min through the recording electrode or by application of PSB dyes using a pneumatic pressure ejection system (Cheng and Peng, 1997). The anesthetized birds were intracardially perfused with 0.9% NaCl solution followed by 10% formalin. All recording sites were localized by examination of 40–60 μm frozen sections (Fig. 1). After perfusion, the birds were laparotomized to determine the size of the largest follicles. Only birds with follicles of F5 and F4 were used for the study. F4 and F5 represent the lower end of preovulatory follicles within the hierarchy, with F1 nearest to the time of LH surge (Bahr et al., 1983). By selecting birds with small follicles and therefore low LH output, we allowed a greater latitude for the system to exhibit any incremental changes of LH output.
Position of recording sites in the hypothalamus.Top panel, Distribution of recording sites (shaded areas) in the preoptic area (POA), the anterior medial hypothalamus (AM), and the ventromedial nucleus (VMN, also known as the posterior medial hypothalamus).Bottom panel, Photomicrograph of the histological location of one recording electrode (arrow) in the AM area.
LH radioimmunoassay. Plasma LH was assayed by means of an RIA specific for chicken LH (PRC-AE1–1; Scanes and Follett, 1972) obtained from Dr. Peter Sharp (Agriculture and Food Research Council Poultry Research Center, Roslin, Midlothian, UK). The assay was performed in the laboratory of P. Johnson (Cornell University). The antiserum, also obtained from Dr. Sharp, was designated anti-chicken LH 3/3 (Sharp et al., 1987). Before assay of the experimental samples, ring dove plasma was tested at various dilutions to verify parallelism in the assay. The mean interassay CVs for high and low pools included in our assays were 10.3 and 12.9%, respectively.
RESULTS
Of the 24 birds in experiment I, l4 females were used exclusively for recording and sound stimulation. Blood samplings, 10 for each bird, were attempted for the remaining 10 birds. Samplings for eight birds were successful; the other two were aborted. Blood samplings were completed for all 12 birds in experiment II. In total, RIA was performed on the 22 birds (20 females and 2 males) for which 10 complete samplings were taken. One bird with four samplings was not included in the statistical treatment.
Table 1 provides a summary of the neuronal responses recorded from units in the POA, AMH, and VMN (also known as the posterior medial hypothalamus) in response to different acoustic stimuli, including white noise. Extracellular recordings were made from 334 spontaneously firing neurons in POA, 346 in AMH, and 225 in VMN. Figure 1 shows the distribution of recording sites in these nuclei. In POA, as well as in AMH and VMN, some neurons exhibited a fast, regular pattern of discharge averaging 6.2 ± 1.2 impulses/sec; others showed a slow and irregular pattern of discharge averaging 3.7 ± 0.6 impulse/sec; and still others showed a phasic discharge pattern.
Distribution, number, and percentages of sound-sensitive units in three regions of the hypothalamus
Effects of female nest coos on neuronal activity
The following results were based on analyses of 199 units in the POA, 203 in the AMH, and 124 in the VMN.
Excitatory responses
Within the POA, 20% (n = 32) of the units recorded exhibited excitatory responses to the female nest coo. The average firing rate increased significantly from the baseline rate of 5.6 ± 0.8 to 12.4 ± 1.1 (mean ± SD) spikes/sec (p < 0.01). The excitatory response exhibits one interesting feature unique to units of this group: the increased firing rate was not a continuous train of discharge; it was interrupted by a period (300 msec) of a near-baseline firing rate during a long response bout lasting 1200–1600 msec. This bursting pattern is most striking in a few units we designated as female-nest-coo-specific units (see below). Of 32 excitatory-responsive neurons, 29 neurons selectively responded to the female nest coo. Figure2 illustrates one such neuron. Figure2A shows a significant increase in discharge in response to the female nest coo (p < 0.01); note the baseline firing in the middle of response. Figure2B shows no response to the reversed female nest coo. Figure 2C shows insignificant changes in response to white noise, and Figure 2D shows insignificant changes in response to the male nest coo. The other three neurons showed weak excitatory discharge in response to each of the four stimuli (not significantly different from baseline rate; p > 0.05).
Response of unit 960108 in the preoptic area of a female ring dove to different acoustic stimuli. A, The female nest coo as the stimulus; the activity of the unit increases significantly. Top, Dot raster plot showing 10 sweeps of the unit’s response. Each dot represents one spike. Bin size is 20 msec. Middle, Histogram of the unit’s response. Bottom, Computer amplitude display and spectrogram of the female nest coo: amplitude over time. B, Reversed female nest coo as the stimulus; no change in neuronal activity can be detected in the number of spikes over time or in the histogram representation. C, White noise as the stimulus: no change in neuronal activity. D, Male nest coo as the stimulus: no change in neuronal activity. All histograms in this paper represent the sum of 10 repetitions of the stimulus. All units are well isolated. In all figures, a closed triangle denotes the onset of the stimulus.
Within the AMH, 24% (n = 41) of the recorded units exhibited excitatory responses to the female nest coo. The average firing rate increased significantly from the baseline rate of 4.4 ± 0.7 to 9.5 ± 1.2 spikes/sec (p< 0.01). With the exception of five neurons that also exhibited a significant increase of discharge in response to male nest coos, these excitatory neurons (n = 36) showed a selective response to female nest coos. In comparison with female-nest-coo-sensitive excitatory neurons in the POA, the responses of the AMH neurons were 200–250 msec longer in duration. Most AMH neurons displayed a continuous discharge without an episode of the silent or near-baseline discharge characteristic of POA neurons; eight AMH neurons exhibited the POA type of discharge.
As in the POA and AMH, there were units in the VMN in which female nest coo stimulation evoked an excitatory response, but they were fewer in number (six units, or 6.8% of 88 neurons). The average firing rate of such units increased significantly from the baseline rate of 5.6 ± 0.6 to 12.4 ± 1.2 spikes/sec (p < 0.01), with a duration comparable to that of a female nest coo bout. Four of these six units also exhibited significant excitatory responses to male nest coo and male bow coo stimulation (p< 0.05). Figure 3 shows discharge patterns of unit 971216 to three different coos. The discharge evoked by female nest coo stimulation was the strongest (Fig. 3A), followed by that evoked by the male bow coo (Fig. 3B) and then the male nest coo (Fig. 3C), all of which showed a significant change from the baseline rate.
Response of excitatory unit 971216 to different coo stimuli in the ventromedial nucleus. A, The female nest coo. Top, dot raster plot of the unit’s response;middle, histogram of the unit’s response;bottom, computer amplitude display of the female nest coo. B, Male bow coo. C, Male nest coo; note strong firing response to the female nest coo, relatively weaker response to the male nest coo, and intermediate response to the male bow coo.
Female-nest-coo-specific units
Four excitatory units in the POA and five in the AMH exhibited a discharge pattern distinct from the female-nest-coo-sensitive excitatory units described in the preceding sections. These units typically were silent (n = 6) or displayed slow and irregular discharges (n = 3), with a discharge train of 7–15 spikes and a response latency of 400–550 msec to female nest coo stimulation. Figure 4 shows the evoked response of one such neuron (960107) in the POA to female nest coo stimulation.
Response of female-nest-coo-specific unit 960107 in the preoptic area of a female ring dove. Top, Dot raster plot and histogram showing response of the unit to the female nest coo presented at 70 ± 5 dB SPL. Bottom,Computer amplitude display and spectrogram of the female nest coo.
The spike train of neurons that responded only to the female nest coo consisted of two bursts, of four to seven spikes each, separated by a silent period of 200–600 msec. The pattern approximately corresponds temporally to the two-note coo. This discharge pattern was evoked exclusively by the female nest coo; neither the male nest coo nor the reversed female nest coo induced a two-part spike train. In the present experiments, these neurons were normally silent or near silent when not responding to the female nest coo. Because of these unique features, we designated them female-nest-coo-specific neurons. Table2 shows the number of female-nest-coo-specific units recorded in the three regions of the hypothalamus. It should be noted that such neurons recorded from AMH did not always exhibit the distinct two-burst pattern; the response latency (350–550 msec) was comparable to that of POA units (Fig.5). One unique silent neuron in the AMH exhibited a single spike response to female nest coo stimulation (Fig.6). We have not found any female-nest-coo-specific units in the posterior medial hypothalamus.
Breakdown of excitatory responses to female nest coos in three regions of the hypothalamus
Response of female-nest-coo-specific unit 960127 in the anterior medial hypothalamus (Ant. Med. Hypoth.). Histogram shows the unit’s response to the female nest coo stimulus presented at 70 ± 5dB SPL.
Unique response of anterior medial hypothalamus (Ant. Med. Hypoth.) unit 960124 to the female nest coo.Top, Dot raster plot showing the unit’s single spike response. Middle, Histogram of the unit’s response.Bottom, Computer amplitude display of the female nest coo.
Inhibitory response
Some of the female-nest-coo-sensitive neurons exhibited an inhibitory response. A total of 17 units in the POA showed a decline of firing rate on average from 6.7 ± 0.9 to 3.1 ± 0.6 spikes/sec (p < 0.01). Of these, eight units showed a high spontaneous firing rate (11.2 ± 1.11 spikes/sec), seven units showed a phasic discharge, and two units showed a slow and irregular discharge. Fourteen inhibitory units were selectively responsive to the female nest coo. Figure7 shows one such neuron: an inhibitory response to the female nest coo (Fig. 7A) but virtually no change in firing rate in response to the reversed female nest coo or white noise or male nest coo (Fig. 7B–D). Three neurons showed an inhibitory response to the female nest coo and also showed a weak (statistically insignificant, p > 0.05) inhibitory response to male nest coos and reversed female nest coos.
Response of inhibitory unit 960126 in the preoptic area to different acoustic stimuli in a female ring dove.A, Histogram shows the unit’s response. With the female nest coo as stimulus, the unit’s response declines significantly.B–D, Unit’s response, respectively, to reversed female nest coo, white noise, and the male nest coo. None of these stimuli evoked changes in neuronal activity.
In the AMH, 22 units exhibited inhibitory responses to female nest coo stimulation. On average, the firing rate declined significantly, from 5.3 ± 0.8 to 2.4 ± 0.5 spikes/sec. Five of these units also showed inhibitory responses (p < 0.02) to the male nest coo and reversed female nest coo. In the VMN, eight units showed inhibitory responses to female nest coo stimulation. On average, the firing rate declined from 5.8 ± 0.8 to 3.1 ± 0.6. Of these units, three showed phasic discharge, one showed a fast discharge, and one showed a slow and irregular discharge. Only three units showed selective inhibitory responses to female nest coo stimulation.
Most recorded units showed no response to female nest coo stimulation (Table 1). In total, 114 neurons in the POA, 116 in the AMH, and 74 in the VMN did not respond to female nest coo stimulation.
Effects of reversed female nest coos on neuronal activity
The response to reversed female nest coos contrasts starkly with the response to normal female nest coos. We recorded only one unit in the POA–AMH region that exhibited a weak excitatory response to reversed female nest coos, with the firing rate increasing from 4.2 ± 0.3 to 5.5 ± 0.5 spikes/sec (p> 0.05). However, six units in the VMN showed significant responses to the reversed female nest coo; two were excitatory and four were inhibitory.
Effects of male nest coo on neuronal activity
Responses to male nest coo stimulation were recorded from a total of 102 units in the POA, 113 in the AMH, and 86 in the VMN. Nine units in the POA showed excitatory responses to the male nest coo with an average firing rate increase of 4.9 ± 0.5 to 8.7 ± 0.8 spikes/sec (Fig. 8A). Seven of these units did not respond when the male nest coo was reversed, and two showed a response to the female nest coo as well (5.4 ± 0.3 to 6.5 ± 0.7 spikes/sec). Responses of 11 units were inhibitory, with the average firing rate declining from 5.8 ± 0.6 to 3.2 ± 0.4 spikes/sec (Fig. 8B). Eight of these units showed no change in discharge when reverse male nest coos were presented, and the remaining three units showed a decline in discharge (p < 0.05).
Response of units in the preoptic area to the male nest coo. A, Excitatory response. Top, Dot raster plot of the unit’s response; middle, histogram of the response; bottom, computer amplitude display of the male nest coo. B, Inhibitory response.Top, Dot raster plot of the unit’s response;middle, histogram of the response;bottom, computer amplitude display of the male nest coo.
Sixteen units in the AMH showed excitatory responses, with an average increased firing of 5.1 ± 0.8 to 11.4 ± 1.2 spikes/sec (Fig. 9A). Five of these also showed excitatory responses to other acoustic stimuli (p < 0.05). Responses of 21 units were inhibitory, with an average decline from 6.5 ± 0.7 to 2.7 ± 0.6 spikes/sec (Fig. 9B). Seven of these units also showed various degrees of inhibitory responses to female nest coos, reversed male nest coos, and reversed female nest coos.
Response of units in the anterior medial hypothalamus (Ant. Med. Hypoth.) to the male nest coo in a female ring dove. A, Histogram showing response of excitatory unit 960107. B, Histogram showing response of inhibitory unit 960915.
In the VMN, 4 units showed excitatory responses and 12 units showed inhibitory responses. Most of these units were not selectively responsive to the male nest coo. We did not find units whose response features met the criteria to be designated as female-nest-coo-specific neurons.
Effects of nest coo stimulation on pituitary LH level
LH levels in 220 blood samples from 19 experimental birds and 3 control birds provided the basis for analysis. Ten samples were taken from each bird, the first five at 10 min intervals and the second five at 20 min intervals. The control birds underwent identical surgical and recording procedures but without sound stimulation.
Five different coos (female nest coo, reversed female nest coo, male nest coo, reversed male nest coo, and male bow coo) and white noise were used as acoustic stimuli. The average LH concentration in the pituitary was significantly higher in response to the female nest coo (24.62 ± 3.12 ng/ml) than to any other acoustic stimulus (male nest coo, 15.25 ± 2.31 ng/ml; white noise, 13.65 ± 2.41 ng/ml; control, 11.49 ± 2.57 ng/ml; p < 0.01). All three nuclei received the same set of stimuli, but a high LH level was observed only when POA or AMH units were activated in response to female nest coo stimulation. Figure 10illustrates the neuronal activity of one POA neuron in response to the female nest coo, reversed female nest coo, male nest coo, or reversed male nest coo and also the corresponding LH levels. The peak LH concentrations sampled during 120 min of female nest coo stimulation were three times higher (45.8 ± 7.15 ng/ml) than those sampled during stimulation with white noise (15.51 ± 4.18 ng/ml) and those of control birds (14.1 ± 3.7 ng/ml), which did not receive acoustic stimulation (p < 0.01; Fig.10A1). A comparison of discharge rates in response to female nest coos versus white noise stimulation during a 0–40 min time frame showed a significant difference (p < 0.05; Fig. 10A2). With reversed female nest coo stimulation, the peak LH level was indistinguishable from that of the control (Fig. 10A1, open circles). The male nest coo stimulation also resulted in an LH concentration (peak, 23.92 ± 5.6 ng/ml) higher than that of the control (15.54 ± 4.92 ng/ml) (Fig. 10B1). The female nest coo induced significantly higher LH levels than the male nest coo (Fig.10A1,B1).
Effects of nest coo stimulation on neuronal response of preoptic area units and pituitary LH levels in female ring doves. There is a significant effect of different acoustic stimuli on LH levels (F(5,54) = 9.86;p < 0.01) and on percentage discharge rate (F(5,42) = 4.57; p < 0.01). A, Effects of the female nest coo.A1, LH levels were significantly higher for the female birds (n = 3) hearing female nest coo playbacks (closed circles) than for those (n = 2) receiving no playback (closed triangles) at time points 0, 10, and 20 min in which 0 = 40 min after the onset of female nest coo stimulation (t = 3.68; p < 0.01). Hearing the reversed female nest coo (open circles;n = 2), on the other hand, produced LH levels indistinguishable from those of the control (t = 0.44; p > 0.05). A2, Percentage changes of firing rate of units recorded during each sound stimulation.Closed circles. Responses of six units responsive to the female nest coo; closed triangles, responses of four units (n = 2) when there was no playback;open circles, responses of four units to reversed female nest coo stimulation; closed rhombuses, responses of three units to white noise stimulation. B, Effects of the male nest coo. B1, LH levels after hearing the male nest coo (closed circles) were greater than in the control (closed triangles) (t = 2.31; p < 0.05). Reversing the male nest coo (open circles) produced no changes in LH response (t = 1.53; p > 0.05). A comparison of LH levels by female nest coo and male nest coo stimulation showed a significant difference (t = 2.31; p < 0.05). B2, Percentage changes of firing rate of units recorded during each sound stimulation.Closed circle, male nest coo responses of four units;open circles, reversed male nest coo responses of three units. Note that the percentage changes of neuronal response to the female nest coo (Fig. 10A2) and male nest coo (Fig. 10B2) are similar (t = 0.65; p > 0.05).
We also asked whether there was a correlation between the magnitude of neuronal discharge and the level of LH output. Using the change of firing rate of the control as 100%, we calculated the percentage change of firing rate of POA neurons after 40 min stimulation of female nest coo versus male nest coo. We found 198 and 185% changes, respectively. The plots in Figure 10 are based on 114 POA units, 42 in response to female nest coos and 28 to reversed female nest coos, 16 to white noise, and 28 in the control group. Each point in the plot represents the sum total of discharges recorded in that period. The 42 units recorded in response to female nest coos during 60 min of stimulation can be broken down into 9 excitatory, 5 inhibitory, and 28 insignificant response units. The corresponding breakdown for the male nest coo plot in Figure11B2 is 5 excitatory, 4 inhibitory, and 32 unresponsive. The discharge rates for female and male nest coos were not statistically different (Fig.10A2,B2).
Effects of nest coo stimulation on neuronal response of anterior medial hypothalamus units and pituitary LH levels in female ring doves. There is a significant effect of different acoustic stimuli on LH levels (F(3,36) = 6.91; p <.01) and on discharge rate (F(3,31) = 4.67; p < 0.01). A, LH levels were significantly higher for the female birds (n = 3) hearing female nest coo play backs (closed circles) than for those receiving no playback (closed triangles) at time points 0, 10, and 20 min, in which 0 = 40 min after the onset of female nest coo stimulation (t = 3.08; p < 0.01). LH levels after hearing the male nest coo (closed squares) were also greater than LH levels in the control (closed triangle) (t = 2.81;p < 0.05). Hearing the reversed nest coo (open circles: reversed female nest coo and reversed male nest coo combined), on the other hand, produced LH levels indistinguishable from those of the control (t = 0.09; p > 0.05). B, Percentage changes of firing rate of units recorded during each sound stimulation.Closed circles, Female nest coo, responses of nine units; closed squares, male nest coo, responses of seven units; closed triangles, no playback, responses of five units; open circles, reversed male nest coo and reversed female nest coo combined, responses of seven units. Note that as in the preoptic area units shown in Figure 10, the percentage changes of neuronal response to the female nest coo and to the male nest coo are strikingly similar (t = 1.39; p> 0.05).
In the AMH, female nest coo stimulation evoked a 192% change in firing over the control; the corresponding LH levels (peak) were 38.92 ± 7.4 ng/ml, significantly different from the control (Fig.11A). The male nest coo stimulation evoked an 184% firing rate change over the control (Fig. 11B); corresponding LH levels (26.82 ± 5.3 ng/ml) were not as high as with female nest coo stimulation but were significantly different from those of the control (Fig. 11A). As in the POA, the female nest coo induced a significantly greater LH level than the male nest coo, although there was no statistical difference in discharge rate (Fig. 11B). Recording from female doves showed that when reversed female nest coos and reversed male nest coos were delivered, no changes in discharge rate or LH level were observed (Fig.11A,B, open circles).
Figure 12 summarizes the results of all pituitary LH levels in response to the different acoustic stimuli. Although the male and female nest coos evoked a similar percentage change in the discharge rates of POA and AMH neurons (in the range of 170–200%), the resulting LH concentrations in the pituitary were strikingly different. The female nest coo stimulation induced a magnitude of LH output three times (peak) greater than that of the male nest coo stimulation.
Histogram shows mean pituitary LH output radioimmunoassayed in birds exposed to different acoustic stimuli. The female nest coo group showed a significantly greater LH output than any of the other groups (F(4,20) = 23.74;p < 0.01). FNC, Female nest coo;MNC, male nest coo; RNC, reversed male nest coo and reversed female nest coo combined; Control, received no sound stimulation.
DISCUSSION
In discussing our findings, we need to consider the experimental conditions under which we obtained our data. First, the females we used for this study were 6 months old and had not been paired with males. At the time of the experiments, they had follicles of stage F5 and F4. Follicles of these stages measure ∼2–2.5 mm; a preovulatory follicle measures ∼16–17 mm in this species. In other words, the LH output we observed was not part of the preovulatory LH surge. Second, follicles at these stages contain very low levels of estrogen, if any. Our LH data, therefore, reflect the GnRH response to acoustic stimulation with little or no estrogen priming. Third, blood samples were taken between 10 A.M. and 6 P.M., a period during which male and female pairs normally engage in vocal courtship exchange. Our LH data therefore mirrors what may actually transpire during the normal courtship cycle. Finally, assays of LH concentrations in the pituitary were independently performed in a laboratory that was not involved in the stimulation-recording experiments and therefore had no knowledge of the treatment groups.
We made the following predictions based on the evidence that led up to this study: (1) given the importance of the female nest coo in follicular growth of female doves (Cheng 1992, 1993), we predicted that the female nest coo would evoke the greatest LH response; and (2) given that the POA, the anterior medial hypothalamus, and the ventromedial nucleus receive axonal projections from the nucleus ovoidalis (the auditory thalamic relay) and its shell region (Durand et al., 1992;Cheng and Peng, 1997) and from the midbrain vocal control nucleus (Cheng and Zuo, 1994), we predicted that units in these areas might respond differentially to the female and male nest coos.
The present analysis of LH data from females exposed to different coos and to white noise supports prediction 1 in that the female nest coo induced an LH level three times greater than that induced by the male nest coo. A response of LH release of this magnitude, presumably attributable to GnRH neuronal action, did not occur with any other acoustic stimulus. With regard to prediction 2, we found excitatory units in the POA, the AMH, and the VMN that responded to the female nest coo but with a higher percentage of neurons in the POA and AMH responding selectively to the female nest coo. Indeed, the majority of VMN units were nonselective (Table 1). The discovery of a special kind of excitatory unit, i.e., the female-nest-coo-specific neuron, was entirely unexpected. These neurons were typically silent, responding only to the female nest coos and having a unique discharge pattern, i.e., two bursts of four to seven spikes separated by a silent period of 200–600 msec and a discharge duration of 1000–2000 msec (Fig. 5), approximately the same time frame as the two-note nest coo (female nest coo, ∼1100 msec; male nest coo, ∼1200 msec). The specificity of these neurons was further verified by their lack of response when the female nest coo was reversed. Interestingly, such neurons were very few in number. They constituted ∼2.6% (n = 9) of the total units (n = 342) recorded in the POA and AMH combined.
Two forms of GnRH are widespread among avian species, chicken GnRH-I (cGnRH-I) and cGnRH-II. The general consensus is that the active form of GnRH for regulating anterior pituitary function is cGnRH-I (Sharp et al., 1990). The distribution pattern of neurons selectively sensitive to the female nest coo matches well with the overwhelming concentration of cGnRH-I neurons located in the POA (Kuenzel and Blahser, 1991;Silver et al., 1993; Cheng and Zuo, 1994). The distribution of cGnRH-I neurons extends to the AMH in turkeys (Millam et al., 1993) but is only sparsely present in ring doves (Cheng and Zuo, 1994). The close proximity of female-nest-coo-sensitive neurons to cGnRH-I neurons suggests that female-nest-coo-sensitive neurons may be cGnRH-I neurons or may synapse with cGnRH-I neurons.
The VMN of the ring dove, in which we found only inhibitory responses to the female nest coo, is completely void of any cGnRH-I neurons but dense with immunoreactive fibers (Silver et al., 1993; Cheng and Zuo, 1994). In the turkey, for which the distribution of both cGnRH-I and cGnRH-II are available, a high concentration of cGnRH-II in the VMN underscores the possibility that some of the inhibitory units we recorded in this region may be from or near cGnRH-II neurons and suggests that cGnRH-II neurons may play some role in reproduction.
Among the excitatory and inhibitory units responsive to female nest coo or male nest coo stimulation, some were clearly selective responses. Reversing the female nest coo or the male nest coo, for example, did not elicit bursting in previously responsive units. This selective responsiveness strongly suggests that units sensitive to the female nest coo, in particular the unique female-nest-coo-specific units in the POA-AMH areas, can detect the temporal pattern of acoustic signals. The female-nest-coo-specific neurons exhibit a characteristic bursting response preceded and followed by silent or low discharge intervals. Similar patterned, selective responses were recently observed in the song-specific neurons of zebra finches (Lewicki, 1996) and in the electrosensory cells of electric fishes, which encode the temporal features of the stimulus waveform (Gabbiani et al., 1996).
The discharge pattern of female-nest-coo-specific neurons provides unmistakable cues that distinguish it from responses to the other acoustic stimuli (compare Figs. 4-6 with 2B–D). This may be a significant functional feature of the system in light of the fact that percentage discharge changes in the POA–AMH region generated by the female and male nest coo stimulation were indistinguishable. The sum totals of excitatory and inhibitory discharges arriving at GnRH networks were similar whether the nest coos were female or male. However, the female nest coo stimulated a much higher level of LH release. We submit that one of the functions of female-nest-coo-specific neurons, with their unique response pattern, may be to send a “go” signal to GnRH or GnRH-related neurons. The concept of state space (Williams and Zipser, 1989) may be useful here to articulate how the female-nest-coo-responsive GnRH network might operate. The GnRH network may recognize the female-nest-coo-specific discharge as a prescribed instruction to move from a state of low response to a state of optimal LH release. In this model, female-nest-coo-specific discharges would have to appear only intermittently among a series of discharges from female-nest-coo-sensitive excitatory units. This may explain why there are so few female-nest-coo-specific neurons.
Recent studies on the synaptic inputs to GnRH neurons have yielded a consistent picture across different species of vertebrates. Catecholaminergic innervation of GnRH neurons and inhibitory inputs from opioid and GABA neurons have been reported in mammals as well as in birds (Muske, 1993; Silverman et al., 1994). We have identified the hypothalamic projection from the shell region of the nucleus ovoidalis as enkephalinergic (Durand et al., 1994). The projection originating from the vocal control nucleus is also enkephalinergic (Cheng and Zuo, 1994). These projections may exert inhibitory control of reproduction, because endogenous opioids on LH release are inhibitory (Cicero et al., 1979; Van Vugt et al., 1989).
Most neuroendocrine control of GnRH neurons appears to be inhibitory in nature. In addition to inhibitory effects of GABAergic and enkephalinergic systems on GnRH activity, transitory reduction of inhibitory action of 5-hydroxytryptamine and dopamine is involved in ovulation (Sharp et al., 1984). Inhibitory mechanisms also mediate ultrastructure changes in LHRH neurons in seasonal breeders (Parry and Goldsmith, 1993). Theoretical frameworks concerning the role of environmental and social factors in reproduction have not dealt with these neuroendocrine findings. Although the inhibitory nature of the effects of environmental stimuli on LH has been observed before (Yokoyama and Farner, 1976), the external cues have generally been thought to stimulate the hypothalamus–pituitary–ovarian (HPO) system (Ball, 1993; Wingfield et al., 1994). We cast the role of courtship in a different light. We propose that at least in birds, species-specific courtship behavior, such as the female nest coo, serves not so much to stimulate as to release inhibitory control of GnRH response (Cheng, 1993)—in other words, that the HPO system is normally kept in check until specialized courtship and/or environmental cues lift the inhibition. Selectively responsive inhibitory neurons may also facilitate GnRH output by suppressing their inhibitory control in response to the female nest coo. These inhibitory neurons may represent the network of inhibitory control of GnRH mentioned in the beginning of this paragraph. The inhibitory control of GnRH neurons is consistent with the observation in ring doves that ovarian follicles exhibit exponential growth once set in motion. Potential beneficiaries of the control of ovarian growth by disinhibition can be found in species of opportunistic breeders in which rapid onset of reproduction is best served by disinhibition. A beautiful example was recently documented in zebra finches, a species in which breeding is cued by water availability, in a study in which vasotocin administered by osmotic minipump inhibited breeding behavior within 2 d (Harding and Rowe, 1997).
The present finding on LH release is in general agreement with that ofEverett et al. (1976, 1981) on LH release in rats on medial POA electrochemical stimulation. In the present study, the peak concentration of LH increase was observed after ∼40–60 min of female nest coo stimulation (Fig. 11), a much faster rise than when electrochemical stimulation was used in rats, in which a latency of 90–120 min was recorded for peak LH (Colombo et al., 1974). Note also that the present study is not complicated by tissue loss, as was the case in experiments using electrochemical stimulation. With tissue loss, mechanisms of LH release through disinhibition were thought feasible (Dyball et al., 1976; Dyer et al., 1976). Based on the present findings, we suggest that both excitatory input and inhibitory input are likely to be involved.
Concluding remarks
We have identified female-nest-coo-sensitive excitatory and inhibitory neurons in the POA, AMH, and VMN. The female-nest-coo-specific neurons were found exclusively in the POA–AMH areas and are characterized by a two-burst pattern with approximately the same temporal contours as the coo. After <1 hr of female nest coo stimulation, the pituitary LH concentration increased dramatically over the baseline concentration. The male nest coo also evoked an LH increment but with a much weaker response. Discharge changes recorded from units in the POA–AMH further suggest that these units are involved in the elevation of LH output. These observations led us to conclude that GnRH networks in ring doves respond preferentially to the species-specific female nest coo.
Footnotes
This work was supported by NIMH Grant MH-47010 to M.F.C. We thank Carrie Brooks for excellent technical assistance. We are grateful to Dr. Peter Sharp (Agriculture and Food Research Council Poultry Research Centre) for the reagents for the chicken LH RIA. We also thank Dr. Marylou Glasier for critical reading of an earlier version of this manuscript and Dr. Michael Casey for helpful discussion of experimental results.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Mei-Fang Cheng, Rutgers University, Department of Psychology, 101 Warren Street, Newark, NJ 07102.