Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
ARTICLE

Relationship between Fos Production and Classical Fear Conditioning: Effects of Novelty, Latent Inhibition, and Unconditioned Stimulus Preexposure

Jelena Radulovic, Jens Kammermeier and Joachim Spiess
Journal of Neuroscience 15 September 1998, 18 (18) 7452-7461; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07452.1998
Jelena Radulovic
1Max Planck Institute for Experimental Medicine, Department for Molecular Neuroendocrinology, 37075 Goettingen, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jens Kammermeier
1Max Planck Institute for Experimental Medicine, Department for Molecular Neuroendocrinology, 37075 Goettingen, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joachim Spiess
1Max Planck Institute for Experimental Medicine, Department for Molecular Neuroendocrinology, 37075 Goettingen, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The relationship between FOS production in the sensory cortex and limbic system and the ability of C57BL/6N mice to acquire context- and tone-dependent freezing were investigated after fear conditioning, which was achieved by exposure of mice to context only or context and tone (10 kHz, 75 dB) as conditioned stimuli (Cs) paired with an electric footshock (0.7 mA, constant) as unconditioned stimulus (Us). The effect of preexposure to Cs or Cs paired with Us on FOS production and learning was also tested. It was demonstrated that high simultaneous FOS production in the parietal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala paralleled the ability of mice to acquire strong freezing responses to novel Cs. After contextual preexposure (latent inhibition), FOS production could be elicited in the central amygdala only by shock and in the basolateral amygdala only by tone. Under these conditions, the ability of mice to acquire contextual freezing was almost abolished, whereas tone-dependent freezing was reduced. Lacking FOS production in the central amygdala after preexposure to context followed by shock (Us preexposure effect) paralleled the inability of mice to acquire tone-dependent freezing, although the tone elicited FOS production in the basolateral amygdala. On the basis of these findings it was concluded that synchronous Cs- and Us-induced FOS production in several defined forebrain areas was accompanied with associative learning of novel stimuli, and that a subsequent low level of FOS production might have been responsible or indicative for delayed conditioning to those stimuli.

  • fear conditioning
  • latent inhibition
  • Us preexposure
  • FOS
  • amygdala
  • hippocampus
  • parietal cortex
  • mice
  • C57BL/6N

Context- and tone-dependent fear conditioning may be rapidly induced in rodents after exposure to novel conditioned (Cs) and unconditioned (Us) stimuli (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1969; Bolles and Collier, 1976). However, fear conditioning may be markedly delayed by nonreinforced presentation of phasic and contextual stimuli when the same stimuli are subsequently presented with a Us, a phenomenon described as latent inhibition. Similarly, preexposure to a Us in a defined context markedly reduces its reinforcing strength when the same Us is later presented with a Cs (Baker and Mackintosh, 1979).

We have reported previously that C57BL/6N mice acquire a strong freezing response after context- and tone-dependent fear conditioning performed in a single trial (Stiedl and Spiess, 1997; Radulovic et al., 1998). This paradigm was demonstrated to be advantageous in studying the molecular changes in the brain during learning because molecular events can be monitored during a defined time course of memory formation, repeated exposures to Cs and Us are avoided, and the effects of Cs and Us can be dissociated. By using FOS protein as a marker of neuronal activity (Sagar et al., 1988), we have demonstrated that FOS production in several forebrain areas is linked to acquisition of foreground contextual fear conditioning (Milanovic et al., 1998). Recent investigations of synaptogenesis in FOS-producing neurons (Kleim et al., 1996) and the use of FOS antisense oligonucleotides (Mileusnic et al., 1996; Swank et al., 1996) provided evidence that induction of c-fos may represent a necessary step toward the formation of long-term memory. However, in our experiments FOS production did not correlate with the fear response after the memory test because of a marked reduction of FOS production that was observed in conditioned and control animals. In agreement with this observation, Campeau et al. (1997) demonstrated that c-fos expression in the hippocampus and amygdala of fear-conditioned and control rats did not differ. These results were in agreement with the finding that c-fos was downregulated after repeated exposure to the same stimuli (Papa et al., 1993; Chen and Herbert, 1995; Hess et al., 1995), but contrasted with observations that FOS production is increased after reexposure to a Cs (Campeau et al., 1991; Pezzone et al., 1992; Smith et al., 1992; Beck and Fibiger, 1995).

To obtain additional information on the significance of FOS production for acquisition of conditioned fear, we have investigated the FOS production and behavior of mice after context- and tone-dependent fear conditioning induced by a single training trial. In addition, the relationship between FOS levels and acquisition of conditioned fear was studied in mice preexposed to context or to context and shock before conditioning. This habituation to context or context paired with shock was performed by monitoring FOS production to determine the brain structures activated by individual Cs and Us as well as the degree of convergence of Cs and Us within a particular forebrain area.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals. Eight-week-old male inbred C57BL/6N mice, obtained from Charles River (Sultzfeld, Germany), were used in the experiments. The mice were individually housed in macrolon cages according to the recommendations of the Society for Laboratory Animal Science (Germany). Standard pelleted diet and water were offeredad libitum. Mice were kept in mouse “hotels” 5 d before the beginning of the experiments. The mouse hotel and the entire fear-conditioning equipment were located in the same room to prevent exposure to novelty and transport of mice after the training or before the testing trials. The mouse hotel was located within a soundproof, nontransparent wooden enclosure equipped with its own air exchange system (air exchange, 60 m3/hr). In the mouse hotels, the temperature was maintained at 22 ± 1°C, the humidity was kept at 55 ± 10%, and the 12 hr light/dark cycle was 7 A.M. to 7 P.M.

Apparatus. As described previously ( Radulovic et al., 1998;Stiedl and Spiess, 1997), the experiments were performed by a computerized fear-conditioning system purchased from TSE (Bad Homburg, Germany). The computer was connected to a control unit containing a shock and a tone generator. Training took place in an apparatus consisting of a box [58 cm (length) × 30 cm (width) × 27 cm (height)] with a gray interior and a 12 V light at the ceiling.

The conditioning and preexposure context consisted of a plexiglass chamber [35 cm (length) × 20 cm (width) × 20 cm (height)] placed on a removable shock grid made of stainless steel rods (diameter 4 mm; bars spaced 0.9 cm apart). The shock grid was connected to a shocker–scrambler unit delivering shocks of defined duration and intensity. The shock grid, the floor below the shock grid, and the chamber were cleaned with 70% aqueous ethanol before each individual mouse entered the chamber.

The pulsed auditory signal (five counts per second, 10 kHz, 75 dB sound pressure level) was presented for 30 sec by a high-frequency loudspeaker (Conrad, KT-25-DT).

The shock (0.7 mA, 2 sec, constant) was delivered through a stainless steel floor grid.

The novel context consisted of a Plexiglas chamber [35 cm (length) × 20 cm (width) × 20 cm (height)] that was exposed to 20 V light, did not have a rod floor, and was divided in half by inserting a clear plastic panel connecting the diagonal corners. The chamber was washed with 1% acetic acid before each individual mouse entered the chamber.

Fear conditioning. Nine experimental groups of mice were used in the experiments (Fig. 1). Three groups were set up for the preexposure procedures. Nonpreexposed mice (N) were habituated to handling and to the environmental stimuli of the experimental room for 3 min/d for 6 consecutive d. Two other groups of mice were preexposed either 3 min to the context (C) or 3 min to the context and a 2 sec shock (Cs) and then habituated as described above for 5 consecutive d. The habituation procedure was introduced to completely familiarize the mice with the stimuli of the experimental room and thus prevent any interference of uncontrolled novel stimuli during the experiments. In addition, this procedure markedly reduced generalization of fear response to stimuli other than the Cs in mice of the Cs preexposure group.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Experimental design.

On the training day, the N, C, and Cs preexposed mice were subjected to three additional training conditions. Mice of each group were exposed for 3 min to the context (N/C, C/C, and Cs/C groups), or 3 min context followed by a 2 sec shock (N/Cs, C/Cs, and Cs/Cs groups), or 3 min context, 30 sec tone, and 2 sec shock (N/CTS, C/CTS, and Cs/CTS groups). All preexposure and training procedures took place in the same context.

The contextual memory test was performed 24 hr after the training so that 12 mice of each experimental group were placed in the preexposure/conditioning context for 3 min. The tone-dependent memory test was performed 2 hr later, by exposure of the same mice to a novel context (3 min) followed by the tone used for conditioning (3 min). Freezing, defined as the lack of movement except for respiration and heart beat, was assessed every 10 sec over 18 intervals by a trained observer who was unaware of the experimental design. The data were converted to the percentage of samples scored as freezing.

FOS immunohistochemistry. One hour after the training, six mice of each group and four naive mice were anesthetized with ketamine (130 mg/gm body weight) and xylasine (13 mg/gm body weight) in saline (0.01 ml/gm, i.p.) and then transcardially perfused with ice-cold phosphate buffer (0.1 m), pH 7.4, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer, pH 7.4 (150 ml/mouse). Brains were post-fixed for 48 hr in the same fixative and then immersed for 24 hr each in 10, 20, and 30% sucrose in PBS. After the tissue was frozen by liquid nitrogen, 50-μm-thick coronal sections were cut on the cryostat. Every fifth section was collected, starting from the olfactory bulb to the end of the hippocampus. Twelve-well plates (Corning, Corning, NY) were used for performing immunocytochemical staining of free-floating sections as described previously (Milanovic et al., 1998). After elimination of endogenous peroxidase activity by 1% H2O2 in methanol for 15 min, the sections were saturated with 5% goat serum and 0.3% Triton X-100 in 0.01m PBS, and then incubated with rabbit anti-FOS antibody (Oncogene Science), 1:20,000 dilution for 48 hr at +4°C. Subsequently, the sections were washed and incubated at room temperature with biotinylated goat anti-rabbit antibody followed by the ABC complex (Vector ABC kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). For visualization, DAB was used as chromogen (Sigma fast tablet set). The sections were mounted, dehydrated, and coverslipped with Eukitt. The specificity of immunostaining was confirmed on sections that were incubated with FOS antibody preabsorbed overnight at +4°C with appropriate synthetic antigenic peptide in tenfold excess over the amount of antibody (Oncogene Science).

Quantification and data analysis. All sections from the olfactory bulb to the end of the hippocampus were analyzed qualitatively. FOS-positive cells were counted in selected forebrain areas with a Macintosh-based image analysis system (NIH Image), as described previously (Pomonis et al., 1997). Nuclei were counted individually and expressed as number of FOS-positive nuclei per 0.1 mm2. The anteroposterior (AP) coordinates of sections (Franklin and Paxinos, 1997) included for detailed analysis were as follows: AP −1.22, medial nucleus of the amygdala; AP −1.34 central, cortical, and basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, CA1 region of the hippocampus and parietal (somatosensory) cortex. The counting was performed in an area of the same shape and size for each brain region. Statistical analysis of behavioral and immunohistochemical data were performed by ANOVA with preexposure and training as a group factor followed by the Bonferonni–Dunn test for post hoccomparisons. The results are presented as mean ± SE.

RESULTS

FOS production after training and behavior during the memory test of nonpreexposed mice

After the training, FOS production of all nonpreexposed mice (N/C, N/Cs, and N/CTS groups) was observed throughout numerous cortical, limbic, diencephalic, and mesencephalic areas. Exposure to context only (N/C group) significantly increased FOS production in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (t(1,8) = 10.55,p < 0.001), hippocampus (t(1,8)= 6.84, p < 0.001), and parietal cortex (t(1,8) = 10.64, p < 0.001), in comparison with naive mice. Statistically significant group differences were determined within the central and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala (Figs. 2A,3). In the central nucleus of the amygdala, mice of the N/Cs and N/CTS groups produced significantly more FOS (F(8,45) = 19.824, p < 0.001) than mice of the N/C group. In mice of the N/CTS group, the number of FOS-positive nuclei in the basolateral amygdala was significantly higher (F(8,45) = 49.210,p < 0.001) when compared with the mice of the N/C and N/Cs groups (Figs. 2A, 3). In the other forebrain areas examined for FOS production, no group differences were detected. It should be mentioned, however, that FOS production was also detected in numerous auditory areas, such as the inferior colliculus, medial geniculate nucleus, and auditory cortex of the N/CTS group. In additional experiments with mice exposed to context and tone without shock (data not presented), similar results were observed, except for the central amygdala, which showed FOS production on a low or undetectable level.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

A, Fos production in the forebrain of N/C, N/Cs, and N/CTS groups 1 hr after training. Data represent mean ± SE of six mice/group. B, Freezing behavior of the N/C, N/Cs, and N/CTS groups during the context- and tone-dependent memory test 24 hr after the training. Data represent mean ± SE of 12 mice/group. Statistically significant differences: *p < 0.01.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

FOS production in the central amygdala (A), basolateral amygdala (B), hippocampus (C), and parietal cortex (D) of N/C, N/Cs, and N/CTS groups. Scale bar, 50 μm.

During the memory test, mice of the N/C group did not exhibit freezing behavior. Mice of the N/Cs group had high freezing levels during the context-dependent but not tone-dependent memory test, whereas mice of the N/CTS group responded with freezing during the context-dependent as well as the tone-dependent memory test (Fig. 2B).

FOS production after training and behavior during the memory test of mice preexposed to context

FOS production in the C/C group of mice (Figs.4A,5) was significantly decreased in all tested areas (p < 0.001) when compared with the N/C group (Fig. 2A, Table1). The observed values were similar to those found in naive mice (data not shown).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

A, Fos production in the forebrain of of C/C, C/Cs, and C/CTS groups 1 hr after training. Data represent mean ± SE of six mice/group. B, Freezing behavior of C/C, C/Cs, and C/CTS groups during the context- and tone-dependent memory test 24 hr after the training. Data represent mean ± SE of 12 mice/group. Statistically significant differences: *p < 0.01.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

FOS production in the central amygdala (A), basolateral amygdala (B), hippocampus (C), and parietal cortex (D) of C/C, C/Cs, and C/CTS groups. Scale bar, 50 μm.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

Significant differences in FOS production of groups subjected to the same training but different preexposure conditions

In mice of the C/Cs group, FOS production was induced only in the central nucleus of the amygdala (Figs. 4A, 5) when compared with the C/C group (p < 0.001). However, the number of FOS-positive nuclei in this area was significantly lower than in the N/Cs (p < 0.05) or N/CTS group (p < 0.05) (Table 1).

In the C/CTS group, in comparison with the C/C and C/Cs groups, the number of FOS-positive nuclei was significantly higher in the basolateral (p < 0.001) and central (p < 0.01) nuclei of the amygdala (Figs.4A, 5). In those areas, FOS production did not differ from that of the N/CTS group. However, in all other brain areas, such as the hippocampus (p < 0.001), parietal cortex (p < 0.001), medial amygdala (p < 0.001), and cortical amygdala (p < 0.001), the FOS levels were significantly lower than the FOS production of the N/CTS group.

Mice preexposed to context differed significantly from nonpreexposed mice in their ability to acquire context- and tone-dependent conditioned fear (F(8,99) = 419.629,p < 0.001). Mice of the C/Cs and C/CTS groups exhibited low freezing levels during the contextual memory test (Fig.4B). The mice of the C/CTS group showed increased freezing during the tone-dependent memory test (Fig.4B), but the freezing scores were significantly lower than those of the N/CTS mice subjected to the same training (p < 0.001).

FOS production after the training and behavior during the memory test of mice preexposed to context followed by shock

Cs preexposure of mice (Figs.6A,7) resulted in a significantly lower FOS production in all brain structures of the Cs/C and Cs/Cs groups (p < 0.001) when compared with the N/C and N/Cs groups (Table 1). In addition, mice of the Cs/Cs group had significantly lower (p < 0.01) FOS levels in the central nucleus of the amygdala than mice of the C/Cs group (Figs.4A, 5, 6A, 7). In mice of the Cs/CTS group, significantly higher FOS production (p < 0.001) was detected only in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala when compared with the Cs/C and Cs/Cs groups (Figs. 6A, 7). In this anatomical structure, FOS production of the Cs/CTS group did not differ (p = 0.403) from the N/CTS group (Figs.2A, 3, 4A, 5; Table 1), but in all other brain areas it was significantly lower (p< 0.001). Similar results were obtained in additional experiments with mice preexposed to context and reexposed to context and tone (data not shown).

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

A, Fos production in the forebrain of of Cs/C, Cs/Cs, and Cs/CTS groups 1 hr after training. Data represent mean ± SE of six mice/group. B, Freezing behavior of Cs/C, Cs/Cs, and Cs/CTS groups during the context- and tone-dependent memory test 24 hr after the training. Data represent mean ± SE of 12 mice/group. Statistically significant differences: *p < 0.01.

Fig. 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 7.

FOS production in the central amygdala (A), basolateral amygdala (B), hippocampus (C), and parietal cortex (D) of Cs/C, Cs/Cs, and Cs/CTS groups. Scale bar, 50 μm.

Mice of the Cs/Cs and Cs/CTS groups acquired a strong context-dependent fear response that did not differ from the response of the N/Cs and N/CTS groups. However, tone-dependent conditioned fear was significantly reduced (F(8,99) = 419.629,p < 0.001) in the Cs/CTS group (Fig.6B) when compared with the N/CTS and C/CTS groups.

DISCUsSION

Widespread FOS production of nonpreexposed mice was observed in numerous brain areas, including the nuclei of the amygdala, hippocampus, and parietal cortex, in agreement with previous mapping studies (Handa et al., 1993; Papa et al., 1993). These data were interpreted as the response of animals to novel environmental stimuli. The observed differences between the N/C, N/Cs, and N/CTS groups confirmed and extended our previous results (Milanovic et al., 1998) indicating that the shock used as a Us in the N/Cs and N/CTS groups elicited marked FOS production in the central nucleus of the amygdala, whereas exposure to a tone as a novel Cs in the N/CTS group resulted in high FOS levels in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala.

Repeated exposure of mice to the same Cs (C/C group) or Cs/Us (Cs/Cs group) resulted in a significant decrease of FOS levels and thus demonstrated that reduction of FOS production occurred after nonassociative as well as associative learning and that it was not affected by the emotional state of the mice as suggested previously (Sandner et al., 1993). These results contrasted with the observation that conditioned fear elicits c-fos mRNA and FOS production in the limbic system on reexposure of mice to a Cs (Campeau et al., 1991;Pezzone et al., 1992; Smith et al., 1992; Beck and Fibiger, 1995). It is difficult to explain the observed discrepancy. On the basis of our own experience, novel stimuli accidentally used before the training may have to be considered. We were only able to prevent this type of interference by extensive habituation of the mice to the training environment, the experimenter, and the lack of any transport before handling. Our results also contrasted with the observation that specific brain areas express c-fos mRNA after learning in instrumental paradigms (Castro Alamancos et al., 1992; Bertaina and Destrade, 1995;Hess et al., 1995; Kleim et al., 1996). This discrepancy may be attributable to the differences between classical and operant conditioning and primarily attributable to the fact that animals do not need to perform a particular task during classical conditioning.

The data obtained from the preexposure experiments provided further evidence that novelty represented the major factor triggering FOS production. Therefore, it was possible to analyze the brain areas activated by individual novel Cs and Us. The results demonstrated that the strongest FOS production was observed after exposure of mice to novel contextual stimuli (N/C group), resulting in high FOS levels in the parietal cortex, the hippocampus, and all nuclei of the amygdala except for the central nucleus. When shock was introduced as a novel stimulus (C/Cs group), FOS production was induced only in the central nucleus of the amygdala, whereas exposure to a tone as a novel stimulus (Cs/CTS group) resulted in FOS production in the basolateral nucleus. These results indicated that the shock does not converge with the contextual and auditory stimuli within the cortical, hippocampal, and amygdaloid brain areas. In contrast, Cs/Us convergence at the hippocampal level was demonstrated during trace conditioning of the eyeblink response with the finding that both Us and Cs altered the neural activity of the CA1 hippocampal area (Mcechron and Disterhoft, 1997). The observed discrepancy may be attributable to different neural pathways mediating conditioned fear and conditioned eyeblink responses.

Although FOS production in the central nucleus of the amygdala was induced only by shock (C/Cs group) and not by context or tone, the FOS level within this nucleus was significantly enhanced when shock used as a novel Us was paired with novel contextual (N/Cs group) or auditory (C/CTS group) stimuli. It appears therefore that brain areas activated by a novel Cs have the capacity to facilitate the responsiveness of the central amygdala to a Us paired with the Cs. This interaction seems to occur only when both the Cs and Us are novel, because in the Cs/CTS group of mice, which were twice exposed to context and shock, the novel tone could not enhance FOS production in the central amygdala.

The contextual and auditory Cs used in the present experiments mainly elicited FOS production in different forebrain areas. An exception was the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, where both Cs induced FOS. Although the highest FOS level in this area was found when novel contextual and auditory stimuli were applied simultaneously (N/CTS group), each Cs applied individually (N/C and Cs/CTS groups) also elicited FOS production, thereby suggesting that contextual and phasic Cs converge within the basolateral amygdala. This result is consistent with the finding that the basolateral amygdala (Kim and Fanselow, 1992) is required for both context- and tone-dependent fear conditioning.

The series of behavioral studies performed after different preexposure conditions strongly suggested a relationship between FOS production after the training and the learning efficiency monitored during the memory test. High FOS levels detected throughout the forebrain of nonpreexposed mice paralleled their ability to acquire strong contextual (N/Cs and N/CTS groups) and tone-dependent freezing (N/CTS group). These findings are in agreement with results pointing to significant roles of the amygdala (Helmstetter, 1992; Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Fanselow et al., 1994), hippocampus (Kim and Fanselow, 1992;Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Chen et al., 1996), and parietal cortex (Thinus et al., 1996) in acquisition of context- and tone-dependent freezing as well as of other autonomic responses reflecting fear (Roozendaal et al., 1991). Accordingly, C and Cs preexposure resulted in reduction of FOS production in certain forebrain areas as well as in impaired acquisition of conditioned fear.

Significant decrease of contextual freezing of the C/Cs and C/CTS groups paralleled the low FOS level in the hippocampus, parietal cortex, and cortical and medial nuclei of the amygdala. Thus it appeared that activation of the central amygdala elicited by the Us after contextual preexposure was not sufficient for acquisition of context-dependent freezing. The tone-dependent freezing response of the C/CTS group was also significantly reduced, but to a lesser extent, implying that simultaneous activation of the basolateral and central amygdala may be sufficient for acquisition of tone-dependent freezing. This finding is in agreement with the observation that the involvement of hippocampus is not required for acquisition of tone-dependent fear (Selden et al., 1991; Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992). The significance of simultaneous FOS production in the basolateral and central amygdala for tone-dependent fear conditioning was further supported by the finding that the Cs/CTS group, in which FOS production was detected only in the basolateral but not central amygdala, acquired very weak tone-dependent freezing. On the basis of these findings it was concluded that synchronous FOS production, followed most probably by activation of specific molecular cascades (Morgan and Curran, 1991), in defined brain areas is accompanied by associative learning involving novel stimuli. Interestingly, the novel neutral stimuli used as Cs exhibited a much stronger capacity to induce the production of the FOS protein than the novel aversive stimulus used as Us.

Activation of certain brain areas during latent inhibition has been reported previously using a conditioned emotional response procedure and using FOS as a marker of neural activity. Latent inhibition was induced by preexposure of rats to both contextual and auditory Cs and resulted in reduction of the FOS production in most of the brain areas, except for the nucleus accumbens, dentate gyrus, and subiculum, where an increase was detected (Sotty et al., 1996). In those and other areas, however, we could not find any increase of the FOS protein after reexposure to the same Cs and Us (data not shown). We assume that this discrepancy may be attributable to numerous differences in the experimental conditions, such as the type of the learning procedure, sequence and number of stimuli presentations during the preexposure and training, number of training trials, interference of novel stimuli, and the time point of FOS immunodetection.

So far, the phenomenon of latent inhibition has been explained mainly by decreased attentional processes during encounter of stimuli. A major role has been attributed to various neurotransmitter systems (Dunn et al., 1993; Baxter et al., 1997). The present findings extend these observations, suggesting that reduction of FOS may reflect the decrease of Cs processing underlying latent inhibition. The same molecular changes seem to be involved in the Us preexposure effect, thus providing a relatively simple molecular basis for a phenomenon that has been described by numerous adaptational (Taylor, 1956; Kamin, 1961; Mis and Moore, 1973), associative (Tomie, 1976), and cognitive (Baker and Mackintosh, 1979) psychological models. Taking into account that a causal relationship between FOS production and learning has been demonstrated recently, as mentioned above (Mileusnic et al., 1996;Swank et al., 1996), the results obtained in this study suggested that learning impairments induced by Cs and Cs/Us preexposure might have been caused by downregulation of FOS production in specific brain areas. This explanation would be consistent with the hypothesis that reduced processing of Cs and Us, which occurs when they are learned as consistent predictors of certain events, prevents subsequent association of the same Cs and Us with other events.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Max Planck Society, Germany. We thank Dr. Ragna Lohman and Dr. Oliver Stiedl for helpful discussions. We also thank Karin Birkenfeld for technical assistance with the immunohistochemistry. Almuth Burgdorf and Svea Dettmer are acknowledged for their help in the preparation of this manuscript.

    Correspondence should be addressed to Jelena Radulovic, Max Planck Institute for Experimental Medicine, Department for Molecular Neuroendocrinology, Hermann-Rein-Strasse 3, 37075 Goettingen, Germany.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Baxter MG,
    2. Holland PC,
    3. Gallagher M
    (1997) Disruption of decrements in conditioned stimulus processing by selective removal of hippocampal cholinergic input. J Neurosci 17:5230–5236.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Baker AG,
    2. Mackintosh NJ
    (1979) Pre-exposure to the Cs alone, Us alone, or Cs and Us uncorrelated: latent inhibition, blocking by context, or learned irrelevance? Learn Motiv 10:278–294.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    1. Beck CHM,
    2. Fibiger HC
    (1995) Conditioned fear-induced changes in behavior and in the expression of the immediate early gene c-fos: with and without diazepam pretreatment. J Neurosci 15:709–720.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Bertaina V,
    2. Destrade C
    (1995) Differential time courses of c-fos mRNA expression in hippocampal subfields following acquisition and recall testing in mice. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2:269–275.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Blanchard RJ,
    2. Blanchard DC
    (1969) Crouching as an index of fear. J Comp Physiol Psychol 67:370–375.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bolles RC,
    2. Collier AC
    (1976) The effect of predictive cues on freezing in rats. Anim Learn Behav 4:6–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Campeau P,
    2. Hayward MD,
    3. Hope BT,
    4. Rosen JB,
    5. Nestler EJ,
    6. Davis M
    (1991) Induction of the c-fos proto-oncogene in the rat amygdala during unconditioned and conditioned fear. Brain Res 565:349–352.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Campeau S,
    2. Falls WA,
    3. Cullinan WE,
    4. Helmreich DL,
    5. Davis M,
    6. Watson SJ
    (1997) Elicitation and reduction of fear: behavioral and neuroendocrine indices and brain induction of the immediate-early gene c-fos. Neuroscience 78:1087–1104.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Castro Alamancos MA,
    2. Borrell J,
    3. Garcia Segura M
    (1992) Performance in an escape task induces fos-like immunoreactivity in a specific area of the motor cortex of the rat. Neuroscience 49:157–162.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Chen C,
    2. Kim J,
    3. Thompson RF,
    4. Tonegawa S
    (1996) Hippocampal lesions impair contextual fear conditioning in two strains of mice. Behav Neurosci 110:1177–1180.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Chen X,
    2. Herbert J
    (1995) Regional changes in c-fos expression in the basal forebrain and brainstem during adaptation to repeated stress: correlations with cardiovascular, hypothermic and endocrine responses. Neuroscience 64:675–685.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Dunn LA,
    2. Gail ES,
    3. Kilts CD
    (1993) Effects of antipsychotic drugs on latent inhibition: sensitivity and specificity of an animal behavioral model of clinical drug action. Psychopharmacology 112:315–323.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Fanselow MS,
    2. Kim JJ,
    3. Yipp J,
    4. De Oca B
    (1994) Differential effects of the N-methyl-d-aspartate antagonist dl-2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate on acquisition of fear of auditory and contextual cues. Behav Neurosci 108:235–240.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Franklin KBJ,
    2. Paxinos G
    (1997) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. (Academic, San Diego).
  15. ↵
    1. Handa RJ,
    2. Nunley KM,
    3. Bollnow MR
    (1993) Induction of c-fos mRNA in the brain and anterior pituitary gland by a novel environment. NeuroReport 4:1079–1082.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Helmstetter FJ
    (1992) Contribution of the amygdala to learning and performance of conditional fear. Physiol Behav 51:1271–1276.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Hess Us,
    2. Lynch G,
    3. Gall CM
    (1995) Regional patterns of c-fos mRNA expression in rat hippocampus following exploration of a novel environment versus performance of a well-learned discrimination. J Neurosci 15:7796–7809.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Kamin LJ
    (1961) Apparent adaptation effects in the acquisition of a conditioned emotional response. Can J Psychol 15:176–188.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Kim JJ,
    2. Fanselow MS
    (1992) Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science 256:675–677.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Kleim JA,
    2. Lussnig E,
    3. Schwarz ER,
    4. Comery TA,
    5. Greenough WT
    (1996) Synaptogenesis and Fos expression in the motor cortex of the adult rat after motor skill learning. J Neurosci 16:4529–4535.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Mcechron MD,
    2. Disterhoft JF
    (1997) Sequence of single neuron changes in CA1 hippocampus of rabbits during acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioned responses. J Neurophysiol 78:1030–1044.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Milanovic S,
    2. Radulovic J,
    3. Laban O,
    4. Stiedl O,
    5. Henn F,
    6. Spiess J
    (1998) Production of the FOS protein after contextual fear conditioning of C57BL/6N mice. Brain Res 784:37–47.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Mileusnic R,
    2. Anokhin K,
    3. Rose SP
    (1996) Antisense oligodeoxynucleotides to c-fos are amnestic for passive avoidance in the chick. NeuroReport 7:1269–1272.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Mis RW,
    2. Moore JW
    (1973) Effect of preacquisition UCs exposure on classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. Learn Motiv 4:108–114.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    1. Morgan JI,
    2. Curran T
    (1991) Stimulus-transcription coupling in the nervous system: involvement of the inducible proto-oncogenes fos and jun. Annu Rev Neurosci 14:421–451.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Papa M,
    2. Pellicano MP,
    3. Welzl H,
    4. Sadile AG
    (1993) Distributed changes in c-fos and c-jun immunoreactivity in the rat brain associated with arousal and habituation to novelty. Brain Res Bull 32:509–515.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Pezzone MA,
    2. Lee WS,
    3. Hoffman GE,
    4. Rabin BS
    (1992) Induction of c-fos immunoreactivity in the rat forebrain by conditioned and unconditioned aversive stimuli. Brain Res 597:41–50.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Phillips RG,
    2. LeDoux JE
    (1992) Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 106:274–285.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Pomonis JD,
    2. Levine AS,
    3. Billington CJ
    (1997) Interaction of hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus and central nucleus of the amygdala in naloxone blockade of neuropeptide Y-induced feeding revealed by c-fos expression. J Neurosci 17:5175–5182.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    Radulovic J, Kammermeier J, Spiess J (1998) Generalization of fear responses in C57BL/6N mice subjected to one-trial foreground contextual fear conditioning. Behav Brain Res, in press.
  31. ↵
    1. Roozendaal B,
    2. Koolhaas JM,
    3. Bohus B
    (1991) Central amygdala lesions affect behavioral and autonomic balance during stress in rats. Physiol Behav 50:777–781.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Sagar SM,
    2. Sharp FR,
    3. Curran T
    (1988) Expression of c-fos protein in the brain: metabolic mapping at the cellular level. Science 240:1328–1331.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Sandner G,
    2. Oberling P,
    3. Silveira MC,
    4. Di Scala G,
    5. Rocha B,
    6. Bagri A,
    7. Depoortere R
    (1993) What brain structures are active during emotions? Effect of brain stimulation elicited aversion on c-fos immunoreactivity and behavior. Behav Brain Res 58:9–18.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Selden NRW,
    2. Everitt BJ,
    3. Jarrard LE,
    4. Robbins TW
    (1991) Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 42:335–350.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Smith MA,
    2. Banerjee S,
    3. Gold PW,
    4. Glowa J
    (1992) Induction of c-fos mRNA in rat brain by conditioned and unconditioned stressors. Brain Res 578:135–141.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Sotty F,
    2. Sandner G,
    3. Gosselin O
    (1996) Latent inhibition in conditioned emotional response: c-fos immunolabelling evidence for brain areas involved in the rat. Brain Res 737:243–254.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Stiedl O,
    2. Spiess J
    (1997) Effect of tone-dependent fear conditioning on heart rate and behavior of C57BL/6N mice. Behav Neurosci 111:703–711.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Swank MW,
    2. Ellis AE,
    3. Cochran BN
    (1996) C-Fos antisense blocks acquisition and extinction of conditioned taste aversion in mice. NeuroReport 7:1866–1870.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Taylor JA
    (1956) Level of conditioning and intensity of the adaptation stimulus. J Exp Psychol 51:127–130.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Thinus BC,
    2. Save E,
    3. Rossi AC,
    4. Tozzik A,
    5. Ammassari TM
    (1996) The differences shown by C57BL/6 and DBA/2 inbred mice in detecting spatial novelty are subserved by a different hippocampal and parietal cortex interplay. Behav Brain Res 80:33–40.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Tomie A
    (1976) Interference with autoshaping by prior context conditioning. J Exp Psychol 2:324–334.
    OpenUrl
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 18 (18)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 18, Issue 18
15 Sep 1998
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Relationship between Fos Production and Classical Fear Conditioning: Effects of Novelty, Latent Inhibition, and Unconditioned Stimulus Preexposure
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Relationship between Fos Production and Classical Fear Conditioning: Effects of Novelty, Latent Inhibition, and Unconditioned Stimulus Preexposure
Jelena Radulovic, Jens Kammermeier, Joachim Spiess
Journal of Neuroscience 15 September 1998, 18 (18) 7452-7461; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07452.1998

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Relationship between Fos Production and Classical Fear Conditioning: Effects of Novelty, Latent Inhibition, and Unconditioned Stimulus Preexposure
Jelena Radulovic, Jens Kammermeier, Joachim Spiess
Journal of Neuroscience 15 September 1998, 18 (18) 7452-7461; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07452.1998
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUsSION
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • fear conditioning
  • latent inhibition
  • Us preexposure
  • fos
  • amygdala
  • hippocampus
  • parietal cortex
  • mice
  • C57BL/6N

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Scaffolding of Fyn Kinase to the NMDA Receptor Determines Brain Region Sensitivity to Ethanol
  • Cytoskeletal and Morphological Alterations Underlying Axonal Sprouting after Localized Transection of Cortical Neuron AxonsIn Vitro
  • Aberrant Chloride Transport Contributes to Anoxic/Ischemic White Matter Injury
Show more ARTICLE
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.