Abstract
The following experiments determined that the somatosensory whisker system is functional and capable of experience-dependent behavioral plasticity in the neonate before functional maturation of the somatosensory whisker cortex. First, unilateral whisker stimulation caused increased behavioral activity in both postnatal day (P) 3–4 and P8 pups, whereas stimulation-evoked cortical activity (14C 2-deoxyglucose autoradiography) was detectable only in P8 pups. Second, neonatal rat pups are capable of forming associations between whisker stimulation and a reinforcer. A classical conditioning paradigm (P3–P4) showed that the learning groups (paired whisker stimulation–shock or paired whisker stimulation–warm air stream) exhibited significantly higher behavioral responsiveness to whisker stimulation than controls. Finally, stimulus-evoked somatosensory cortical activity during testing [P8; using 14C 2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) autoradiography] was assessed after somatosensory conditioning from P1–P8. No learning-associated differences in stimulus-evoked cortical activity were detected between learning and nonlearning control groups. Together, these experiments demonstrate that the whisker system is functional in neonates and capable of experience-dependent behavioral plasticity. Furthermore, in contrast to adult somatosensory classical conditioning, these data suggest that the cortex is not required for associative somatosensory learning in neonates.
- vibrissae
- whiskers
- development
- learning
- neural plasticity
- barrels
- somatosensory cortex
- behavioral plasticity
The rat mystacial vibrissae somatosensory system processes environmental tactile cues from the facial whiskers. Mystacial vibrissae are active tactile organs used to scan object surfaces (Welker, 1964; Woolsey and Van der Loos, 1970;Carvell et al., 1991) and extract detailed and complex information (Simons, 1995, 1997; Carvell and Simons, 1996; Nicolelis et al., 1996). Both behavioral plasticity (Steiner and Huston, 1992; Steiner and Gerfin, 1994; Bermejo et al., 1996; Siucinska and Kossut, 1996) and experience-induced cortical plasticity (Delacour et al., 1987; Kossut, 1992; Welker et al., 1992; Diamond et al., 1993; Armstrong-James et al., 1994; Armstrong-James, 1995; Nicolelis et al., 1995; Joublin et al., 1996; Siucinska and Kossut, 1996; Warren and Dykes, 1996; Simons, 1997; Kossut and Siucinska, 1998) have been demonstrated as a result of adult whisker manipulations.
However, far more dramatic plasticity can be seen in the adult somatosensory system as a result of neonatal manipulations (Van der Loos and Woolsey, 1973; Simons and Land, 1987; Carvell and Simons, 1996; Maier et al., 1996). For example, neonatal (younger than P4) whisker removal or whisker deafferentation results in the loss of corresponding barrel representations (Van der Loos and Woolsey, 1973;Killackey et al., 1978; Nicolelis et al., 1996) with a corresponding disruption of adult whisker dependent behaviors, including a loss in the ability to orient the snout properly (Symons and Tees, 1990) and discriminate surface textures (Carvell and Simons, 1996).
Rats are born with whiskers; fine whiskers in follicles appear early [embryonic day (E) 12] (Yamakato and Yohro, 1979), and specialization of follicle sensory mechanoreceptors occurs at E20 (English et al., 1980) well before the maturation of trigeminal ganglia and nuclei (by P1; Taber, 1963; Forbes and Welt, 1981). After thalamic barreloid maturation at P2, the cortical barrel field first appears at P3–P5 (Rice et al., 1985). The system is vulnerable to manipulations during early somatotopic pattern development (Weller and Johnson, 1975; Harris and Woolsey, 1979; Henderson et al., 1992, 1994; Fox, 1995; Rhoades et al., 1996). Thalamocortical afferents modify barrel pattern before P5 (Schlaggar and O’Leary, 1994; Rice, 1995). However, it is unclear whether the whisker system is behaviorally functional in the neonate before whisking movements develop at approximately P12 (Welker, 1964).
Tactile stimulation in the perioral area is critical for neonate survival. Depriving the neonate of perioral sensation, including the whiskers and oral region, disrupts essential behaviors such as nipple attachment, and survival rate is greatly reduced (Hofer et al., 1976;Hofer, 1981; Larson and Stein, 1984; Distel and Hudson, 1985;Morrow-Tesch and McGlone, 1990). However, the specific role of whiskers in these behavioral effects and whether the whisker system is behaviorally functional in the neonate are still unclear.
The present experiments were designed to determine whether neonatal rats are functionally responsive to stimulation of the whiskers and to assess potential behavioral and cortical neural plasticity in the whisker system. Results show that the neonatal whisker system is functional and capable of associative behavioral plasticity (Landers and Sullivan, 1999), although this plasticity may be mediated by subcortical structures.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects. The subjects were male and female rat pups born of Long–Evans rats (Harlan Sprague Dawley, Indianapolis, IN) in the vivarium at the University of Oklahoma. No more than one male and one female from a litter were used in an experimental condition. Dams were housed in rectangular polypropylene cages (34 × 29 × 17 cm) lined with wood chips in a temperature-controlled (20°C) and light-controlled room (12 hr light/dark cycle). Ad libitumfood and water were available at all times. Births were checked twice daily. The day of birth was considered to be P0. Pups were tested at either P3–P4 (as barrels begin to appear; Rice et al., 1985) or at P8 (as stimulus-evoked cortical 2-DG uptake approaches adult levels;Kossut and Hand, 1984; Wu and Gonzalez, 1997).
Behavioral assessments. To evaluate whether neonatal rat pups can respond to whisker stimulation, we assessed pup behavior during unilateral whisker stimulation at P3–P4 and P8. Pups were removed from the mother and placed in plastic Petri dishes (100 mm diameter, 15 mm height) and left unrestrained. A 10 min acclimation period preceded the experimental session to allow for recuperation from experimental handling. Unilateral whisker stimulation consisted of manually stimulating the whiskers for 30 sec (∼50 sweeps back and forth across the entire whisker field), using a wooden rod 1 mm in diameter. Stimulation included repeated flexion of every mystacial vibrissae, without stimulating the intravibrissal hair or skin on the snout. The side of the snout that was stimulated was alternated between litters to control for potential laterality bias of individual pups (Tobet et al., 1993). Behavior was recorded 10 sec before stimulation and during whisker stimulation (30 sec), using a motor activity scale based on the number of elements moved, and was sustained for 2 sec (Hall, 1979; Sullivan et al., 1991). The activity scale range was from 0–5 [0, no movement; 1, movement of one element (i.e., head or a limb); 2, movements of two elements (i.e., treading); 3, movements of three elements (i.e., pivot); 4, movements of four elements (i.e., locomotion); 5, movements of five elements (i.e., roll over)]. This rating scale frequently has been used to assess neonatal rat behavior and measures changes in general motor activity, which is the characteristic response of the motorically immature neonatal rat pup to presentation of a stimulus (Hall, 1979). Pups were returned immediately to the mother after completion of the session.
Associative conditioning. To assess potential behavioral plasticity in the whisker system, we used both an appetitive and an aversive classical conditioning paradigm. The conditioned stimulus (CS) was always 30 sec of unilateral whisker stimulation (described above) and the unconditioned stimulus (US) was a gentle stream of warm air (US, 10 sec stream of warm air) for appetitive conditioning and a moderate shock (US, 0.5 sec, 0.5 mA shock to the hind trunk) for aversive conditioning. Paired experimental subjects were given eight pairings of the CS and US with a 3 min intertrial interval (ITI). Conditioning control groups included CS-only (eight 30 sec unilateral whiskers stimulation trials, 3 min ITI), US-only (eight US’s, 3 min ITI), and/or random CS–US (eight presentations each of the CS and US, with the CS presented at an ITI of 3 min and the US presented randomly between the CS presentations with the constraint of nonoverlapping CS–US presentations). Equal numbers of males and females were assigned to each of the conditioning groups. The side of the snout receiving vibrissae stimulation was alternated between each litter.
For pups used in the behavioral plasticity experiments, the pups received only one training session at P3–P4. For the neural assessment experiment the pups received daily training from P1 through P8 to maximize learning (totaling eight sessions). Training was videotaped occasionally to verify that pups in different conditions were treated similarly.
A behavioral rating scale (described above; Hall, 1979) was used to monitor the acquisition of conditioned responses (CR) to whisker stimulation during training. Behavior was observed for 10 sec before each whisker stimulation for a “pre” score and during each whisker stimulation before US onset for the “during” score.
Behavior test. In addition to acquisition curves, pup learning also was assessed by assessing pups’ responsiveness to five CS-only presentations. At 4 hr after training the pups again were removed from the mother and placed in the clean Petri dishes, given 10 min to acclimate, and then given five 30 sec unilateral whisker stimulations (3 min ITI). All aspects of testing were consistent with those used during training and are described above. An observer blind to the experimental conditions of pups was used for testing when available.
14C 2-deoxyglucose autoradiography. To determine whether the somatosensory cortex responds to stimulation of the whiskers and to assess potential cortical neural plasticity in the whisker system, we assessed neural activity of the cortex via14C 2-deoxyglucose autoradiography (2-DG; 20 μCi/100 gm;Sullivan et al., 1991). Pups were handled similarly to those used for behavioral testing, except that the 2-DG was injected 5 min before the start of whisker CS-only presentations and a greater number of whisker CS-only presentations were used to accommodate the longer period of testing required for 2-DG (45 min). Then the pups were decapitated and their brains quickly removed. For tangential sectioning of the barrel field, brains first were bisected midsagittally, and the cortical hemispheres were removed. The cortex was mounted on a pedestal with Tissue Tek, flattened with a glass coverslip, and frozen with dry ice, followed by immersion of the brain in 2-methylbutane at −30 to −40°C and stored in a −70°C freezer. For sectioning, frozen brains were equilibrated to −20°C in a cryostat; sections were cut at 40 μm, mounted on subbed glass coverslips, and dried on a slide warmer for 5–10 min. Some sections were mounted on subbed slides and put aside for succinic dehydrogenase staining (SDH; Killackey and Belford, 1979; Jablonska et al., 1995) to verify the location of the cortical barrel field.
Then coverslips containing the 2-DG brain sections were exposed to Kodak BioMax MB x-ray film for 4 d in an exposure cassette. A set of 14C-labeled methylmethacrylate standards previously calibrated to the same concentration of 14C uptake in brain sections was exposed with each sheet of film. Autoradiographs were developed with standard film development techniques and GBX solutions.
The autoradiographs were analyzed by an MCID computer-based digital image processor from Imaging Research (St. Catherine’s, Ontario, Canada); the person analyzing the films was unaware of the experimental condition of the autoradiograph. Measurements were taken from the cortex contralateral to stimulation. For each brain section of each pup the optical density was averaged over five samples in the whisker somatosensory cortex and over an additional five samples in an adjacent region of the somatotopic map corresponding to the visual cortex (Chapin and Lin, 1990; Rice, 1995). For each brain section the means of each of the two brain areas were calculated, and the ratio of 2-DG uptake in the somatosensory cortex and visual cortex was calculated. For each pup there was an average of seven brain sections (range, 5–9). Via this method an optical density ratio represented the density found in stimulated areas relative to nonstimulated areas of the same brain section.
RESULTS
Neonatal rat pups respond to whisker stimulation
Both P3–P4 (n = 12) and P8 (n = 12) pups responded to whisker stimulation [main effect of treatment, ANOVA; F(1,20) = 49.7; p < 0.0001; no significant effect of age and of treatment × age interaction]. A Fisher post hoc test revealed that both P3–P4 and P8 pups exhibited an increase in activity during the whisker stimulation relative to prestimulation (p < 0.05). Pup responsiveness to stimulation was characterized by an increase in behavioral activity, accompanied by occasional head-up and head turns toward and away from the stimulation. Occasional mouthing movements were observed also.
Only P8 pups exhibited 2-DG uptake in the whisker somatosensory cortex
Autoradiography indicated that P8, but not P3–P4, pups express a significant increase in 2-DG activity in the primary somatosensory cortex contralateral to whisker stimulation (Fig.1; main effect of treatment × age, ANOVA; F(1,9) = 16.6; p < 0.01). Figure 2 depicts a 2-DG autoradiograph for the stimulated and nonstimulated P4 and P8 barrel cortex and a neighboring section stained with SDH (P4). These data, using assessments within the barrel field, support previous studies byWu and Gonzalez (1997), which suggest that layer 4 (coronal sections) of the somatosensory cortex does not express stimulation-evoked 2-DG activity until P7, after the barrel field pattern is defined and layer 4 upper tier thalamocortical afferents are mature (Melzer et al., 1994;Wu and Gonzalez, 1997). Therefore, it appears as if neonatal rat pups are capable of behaviorally responding to whisker stimulation without the involvement of the somatosensory cortex.
Whisker stimulation produced a stimulus-evoked increase in relative focal 2-DG uptake in the contralateral whisker cortical barrel field of P8 rat pups, but not in P3–P4 rat pups. Theasterisk represents a significant difference (p < 0.05) between stimulated and nonstimulated pups.
Representative autoradiographs of tangential sections through the cortical barrel field in P4 (left) and P8 (right) rat pups. The figure in the center is a neighboring section from the P4 rat stained for succinic dehydrogenase. This SDH-stained section includes four rows of the barrel field representing the facial vibrissae, which are marked by arrows. Stimulation of the contralateral whiskers evoked an increase in 2-DG uptake in the cortical barrel field of P8 pups relative to nonstimulated P8 pups (arrow). In P4 pups, however, no whisker stimulation-evoked increase in cortical barrel field 2-DG uptake was detected.
Neonatal rat pups (P3) can learn an association by using whiskers
Association of a whisker stimulation CS with an appetitive warm airstream US produced rapid acquisition of a conditioned behavioral activation. As shown in Figure3A2, during training the pups in the paired training group (n = 10) acquired a CR of increased activity to the whisker CS (during the 20 sec before onset of the reward) as compared with pups receiving whisker stimulation CS-alone (n = 10; repeated measures ANOVA, group × trial interaction; F(3,54) = 21.79;p < 0.01). Fisher post hoc tests revealed that paired pups showed significantly more stimulus-evoked activation than control pups beginning at trials 3–4 (p < 0.01). This CR was specific to the CS because pre-CS behavior (i.e., just before pups received each whisker stimulation) did not differ between groups (Fig. 3A1;F(1,18) = 3.857, NS).
Associative pairing of a whisker (conditioned stimulus, CS) and heat from a warm air stream (unconditioned stimulus, US) produced a conditioned behavioral activation response (generalized increase in behavioral activity) to the whisker CS alone. Behavioral activity before the onset of each CS remained relatively stable over the course of conditioning (A1), whereas CS-evoked activity increased over repeated trials (A2) in pups in the paired group as compared with the control pups. B, Subsequent CS-only tests (4 hr after conditioning) revealed a significant CS-evoked behavioral response in paired pups relative to controls. Asterisksrepresent a significant difference between paired and control groups;p < 0.05.
The CR to the whisker stimulation CS was maintained for at least 4 hr. As shown in Figure 3B, pups trained with the whisker CS paired with a warm air US showed an enhanced behavioral CR to CS-only trials 4 hr after testing as compared with pups originally trained in the control group [t(18) = 4.46;p < 0.001]. During testing the CR had similar characteristics to the unconditioned response (UR) to the warm air stream; that is, presentation of the US produced behavioral activation similar to the UR, and the CS elicited increased behavioral activation after conditioning similar to the CR.
Similar results were obtained by using a moderate shock (0.5 mA) as the US (Fig. 4). As shown in Figure4A2, the whisker-shock paired group (n = 8) expressed a significant increase in behavioral activity in response to whisker stimulation CS (during the 29 sec before onset of the reward) as compared with random (n= 8) and CS-only (n = 8) groups during training (repeated measures ANOVA; group × trial interaction;F(3,63) = 19.38; p < 0.01). Because US-only pups (n = 8) did not receive the CS during training, their response to this stimulation could not be observed. Post hoc Fisher tests revealed that paired pups showed significantly more activation than control pups by trials 3–4 (p < 0.05). As shown in Figure4A1, the pups in groups receiving shock also showed a mild, nonspecific increase in behavioral activity over the course of training that was statistically significant, although this cannot account for the CS-specific response (pre-CS activity; repeated measures ANOVA; group × trial interaction;F(3,63) = 4.81; p < 0.01).
Associative pairing of a whisker CS and an electric shock US produced a conditioned behavioral activation response (generalized increase in behavioral activity) to the whisker CS alone. Behavioral activity before the onset of the CS remained relatively stable over the course of conditioning (A1), although pups in shocked groups showed a slight increase in activity. CS-evoked activity increased over repeated trials (A2) in pups in the paired group as compared with pups in all control groups.B, Subsequent CS-only tests (4 hr after conditioning) revealed a significant CS-evoked behavioral response in paired pups relative to controls. Asterisks represent a significant difference between paired and control groups; p < 0.05.
As shown in Figure 4B, pups trained with the whisker CS paired with a shock US showed an enhanced behavioral CR to CS-only trials 4 hr after testing as compared with control pups (ANOVA; main effect of group; F(3,28) = 36.22;p < 0.001). Post hoc Fisher tests revealed that pups receiving paired whisker stimulation and shock US had higher CS-evoked activity levels at testing relative to control groups (p < 0.01) and that the random, CS-only, and US-only groups were not significantly different from one another. During testing the CR had similar characteristics to the UR elicited by shock; that is, presentation of the US produced behavioral activation, and the CS elicited increased behavioral activation after conditioning. However, the shock US also elicited vocalization and behavior that could be interpreted as escape behavior, and these behaviors were not elicited by the CS after learning. This is consistent with aversive conditioning in the olfactory system of neonatal rats (Camp and Rudy, 1988; Miller et al., 1990; Wilson and Sullivan, 1994).
Neonatal somatosensory learning in the whisker system does not appear to involve cortical barrel changes
Although stimulus-evoked increases in relative 2-DG uptake were detected in the P8 whisker somatosensory cortex, no differences were found in stimulus-evoked relative 2-DG uptake between different training conditions after 8 d of conditioning (Fig.5; F(3,19) = 2.33, NS).
Mean relative whisker CS-evoked 2-DG uptake in the cortical barrel field in P8 pups after 8 d of conditioning. No learning-associated significant differences were detected among groups.
DISCUSSION
These experiments provide the first demonstration of behavioral responsiveness to whisker stimulation before the onset of whisking (P12; Welker, 1964) and indicate that this system is behaviorally functional in the first week of life during the period of initial cortical development. Stimulation of mystacial whiskers caused increased behavioral activity as early as P3, before the onset of stimulus-evoked barrel cortical activity as measured by 2-DG autoradiography. Furthermore, these experiments demonstrate that the neonatal rat pup whisker system is capable of the experience-dependent behavioral plasticity demonstrated by classical conditioning. The inability to detect stimulus-evoked somatosensory cortical 2-DG uptake in P3–P4 pups and a failure to detect learning-correlated differences in 2-DG uptake in the cortical whisker somatosensory system of P8 pups suggest that neonatal associative learning in this system may rely on earlier-developing subcortical structures. Experience-dependent structural or functional modification of subcortical circuits, however, may lead to subsequent changes in the development of the cortex that were not examined here specifically (Woolsey, 1990; Florence and Kaas, 1995; Ralston et al., 1996; Sengelaub et al., 1997; Kilgard and Merzenich, 1998). These data are in sharp contrast to both 2-DG and other neurophysiological studies with whisker somatosensory classical conditioning in adults in which cortical changes are associated with classical conditioning (Siucinska and Kossut, 1996; Butt et al., 1997;Kossut and Siucinska, 1998) and other temporally correlated presentations of stimuli (Clark et al., 1988; Wang et al., 1995;Weinberger, 1995; Zhou and Fuster, 1997; Maalouf et al., 1998).
The present data indicate that the whiskers of the rat pup are functional and responsive to stimulation in P3–P4 pups, before the onset of sensory-evoked activation of the somatosensory cortex (Kossut and Hand, 1984; Wu and Gonzalez, 1997; present study). The sensory-evoked behavior examined here was a nonspecific behavioral activation, similar to that observed in rat pups caused by olfactory stimulation (Sullivan et al., 1991; Sullivan and Wilson, 1993). In neonates, behavioral activation evoked by odors has been hypothesized to be mediated by subcortical structures, whereas more specific responses—e.g., odor approach/avoidance responses—may be mediated by “higher,” potentially cortical structures (Kucharski and Hall, 1988; Sullivan and Wilson, 1993; Rudy, 1994). For example, bilateral amygdala lesions in P5 rat pups disrupt the acquisition of conditioned odor approach responses but do not affect conditioned behavioral activation to odors (Sullivan and Wilson, 1993). Thus, it is suggested that the neonatal whisker system, although behaviorally functional, probably lacks the ability to perform detailed sensory discriminations and the stimulus analyses shown by the mature somatosensory system. However, given the present demonstration of behavioral responsiveness to whisker stimulation early in the ontogeny of cortical function, behaviorally relevant early sensory input could play a major role in cortical development via experience-induced changes in trigeminal and thalamic nuclei.
The hypothesized role of subcortical mediation of whisker-evoked behavior and conditioning is similar to that observed in the neonatal olfactory system. Neonatal olfactory conditioning involves structural and functional modification early in the olfactory pathway (olfactory bulb; Wilson et al., 1987; Sullivan et al., 1989; Woo and Leon, 1991). Several subcortical loci in the vibrissae somatosensory pathway could be involved in neonatal behavioral responses to whisker stimuli. Trigeminal and thalamic somatosensory nuclei appear functional early in development, and normal sensory-evoked activity in those structures is required for proper functional organization of higher centers in the pathway (for review, see Armstrong-James, 1995; Diamond, 1995;Henderson and Jacquin, 1995; Keller, 1995). Here again, neonatal somatosensory behavior and conditioning may be similar to those reported for the olfactory system. Additionally, subcortical learning has been documented in a variety of adult sensory systems, clearly indicating that the cortex is not necessary for at least some forms of simple learning (Tsukahara et al., 1981; Wall et al., 1985; Iwata et al., 1986; Lennartz and Weinberger, 1992; Albrecht and Davidowa, 1993; Edeline, 1998).
An additional similarity between neonatal somatosensory and olfactory conditioning is their dependence on norepinephrine (NE). Both the behavioral and neural plasticities induced by neonatal olfactory conditioning involve and require NE from the locus coeruleus (for review, see Wilson and Sullivan, 1994). Similarly, the behavioral whisker conditioning in neonates described here involves and requires NE (Landers and Sullivan, 1999). It is hypothesized that the association of whisker stimulation and NE release induced by the unconditioned stimulus induces synaptic modifications that result in enhanced behavioral responsiveness to subsequent whisker stimulation alone. As in neonatal olfactory conditioning, this association may occur early in the somatosensory pathway. The locus coeruleus innervates the somatosensory system at a number of different levels along the ascending somatosensory pathway (Simpson et al., 1997), and NE has been implicated in development, function, and plasticity within this system (Osterheld-Haas et al., 1994; Warren and Dykes, 1996; Levin et al., 1998; Waterhouse et al., 1998). Studies currently are underway to identify the subcortical nuclei critical for whisker-evoked behaviors in neonates. The critical role of NE in neonatal learning is in sharp contrast to that of the adult in which NE has long been known to have a modulatory effect on learning, memory, and its expression (Mason, 1984; Harley, 1987; Fillenz, 1990; Liang et al., 1990; Sara et al., 1994).
Recent studies from our laboratory suggest that the stimulation of whiskers in neonatal rat pups is behaviorally relevant even before the onset of whisking. For example, neonatal rat pups use their whiskers in nipple attachment and during social interactions with siblings. Specifically, dewhiskered pups (P3–P4) with an otherwise normal perioral area take a longer time to attach to their mother’s nipples and appear less active when interacting with a sibling (Young and Sullivan, 1997). Conditioned behavioral activation has been demonstrated to enhance ongoing behaviors such as huddling and milk ingestion in neonates (Sullivan et al., 1986). Under natural conditions the association of maternal stimulation (such as tactile stimulation, warmth, and milk) with whisker stimulation could result in a learned change in behavioral state that would facilitate infant responsiveness to the dam and siblings, as has been demonstrated for learned odor cues (Sullivan et al., 1986). The relatively nonspecific sensory information required to produce this critical behavioral response could be controlled entirely by subcortical pathways and, thus, may not dependent on the late-developing neocortex. Experience-induced plasticity in these subcortical circuits also could have important consequences for subsequent cortical development.
Footnotes
This work was funded by National Institute of Mental Health Grant HD33402 and National Science Foundation Grant IBN-9814837 (to R.M.S.). This work was done in partial completion of the requirements for the Master of Science degree (to M.S.L.). We thank Drs. Don Wilson, Thomas Woolsey, and Mark Jacquin for advice on initial aspects of this work. We also thank Victoria Perez and Brian Yeaman for their assistance in data collection.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Regina M. Sullivan, Department of Zoology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK 73019.