Abstract
The prominent role of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in working memory (WM) is widely acknowledged both in nonhuman primates and in humans. However, less agreement exists on the issue of functional segregation within different subregions of the PFC with regard to the domains of spatial and nonspatial processing or involvement in simpler versus more complex aspects of WM, e.g., maintenance versus processing function. To address these issues, six monkeys were trained to perform four WM tasks that differed with respect to domain (spatial vs nonspatial) and level of WM demand (recall of one vs three items). The delayed response format was used to assess simple one-item memory, whereas self-ordering tasks were used to require the monkey to maintain and organize three items of information within WM. After training, the monkeys received bilateral PFC lesions in one of two different areas, Walker’s areas 9 and 8B (dorsomedial convexity; n = 3) or areas 46 and 8A (dorsolateral cortex, n = 3) and then tested postoperatively on all tasks.
Monkeys with lesions of the dorsomedial convexity were not impaired either on spatial or nonspatial WM tasks, whether the task required simple storage or sequential processing. By contrast, lesions of the dorsolateral cortex produced a significant and persistent impairment in both simple and complex spatial WM but no impairment in the two nonspatial WM tasks. These results support a functional segregation within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for WM: the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (area 46/8A) is selectively involved in spatial WM, whereas the dorsomedial convexity (area 9/8B) is not critically engaged in either spatial or nonspatial working memory. Furthermore, the specific involvement of area 46/8A in spatial sequencing as well as in single-item storage WM tasks supports, in the nonhuman primate, an areal dissociation based on domain rather than on processing demand.
It is widely accepted that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) plays a major role in the most sophisticated aspects of human thought, including reasoning and planning. These functions of metacognition may greatly depend on more fundamental cognitive operations, namely the working memory (WM) processes defined by the ability to maintain, manipulate, and use mental representations for goal-directed behavior (Baddeley, 1996; Goldman-Rakic, 1987). Experimental studies in nonhuman primates and more recent functional imaging studies in humans have underlined the critical role played by the prefrontal cortex in WM (for review, see Goldman-Rakic, 1987, 1995;Owen, 1997; Ungerleider et al., 1998). However, unresolved issues with particular relevance to human cognition include whether the PFC’s role in WM functions is that of a scratch-pad for temporary storage of information or a nondenominational central processor and whether these key aspects of WM are dissociable and how they might be distributed within the PFC. Two models of functional segregation of the PFC have been proposed. One model, developed by Goldman-Rakic and colleagues (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Funahashi et al., 1993; Wilson et al., 1993; O’Scalaidhe et al., 1997), postulates a modular organization of WM based on the domain of information processing (the “domain specificity” model). In this view, the cortex surrounding the principal sulcus (Walker’s area 46) is specialized for “on-line” processing of information concerning the location of objects, whereas cortices below area 46—the inferior convexity (Walker’s areas 12 and 45)—are involved in processing the features and identity of objects within WM. In this theoretical framework, “domain” embraces any sensory modality that registers information relevant to that domain, e.g., visual and auditory signals can provide input to a spatial domain. The second model, proposed by Petrides and Owen (Petrides et al., 1993; Petrides, 1995; Owen et al., 1996a,b, 1998; Owen, 1997), postulates a segregation within the PFC based on the level of processing within WM (“operation–segregation” model). In this model, the PFC is divided into two regions that are referred to, respectively, as the mid-frontal cortex (in monkeys, middle portions of Walker’s area 9 and the rim of area 46) and the inferior convexity (the lower portion of area 46 and Walker’s areas 12/45). According toOwen (1997), the mid-lateral or dorsomedial convexity (our terminology) is viewed as an executive processor that allows active manipulation and monitoring of information within WM. By contrast, the inferior convexity is involved in lower demand processing such as maintenance of information in WM. According to the operation–segregation view, different levels of processing are segregated across prefrontal areas in a hierarchical scheme, whereas in the domain-specific model, different levels of processing within a given domain may be performed within the same region, possibly engaging different numbers of cellular processing units (columns) within that region to accommodate different processing demands.
To obtain further information on these issues, the aim of the present study was to determine the critical level of anatomical–functional segregation for WM within the PFC. Toward this end, monkeys were trained to perform WM tasks varying with respect to sensory domain (spatial vs nonspatial) and level of processing (simple vs complex WM processing). The complex WM tasks used in this study were “self-ordered tasks” (Petrides and Milner, 1982; Petrides et al., 1993; Petrides, 1995). These tasks differ from the classic delayed response tasks in that the memory load is higher (three items vs one item), and in addition the subject must extract and arrange the relevant mental representations into a coherent temporal sequence. Once the monkeys reached criterion performance levels on these tasks, they received bilateral resections of the dorsomedial (DM) convexity (areas 9 and 8B) or the middle dorsolateral (DL) region (areas 46 and 8A). If segregation in the PFC is based on sensory domain, we expect lesions of areas 46/8A to produce a selective spatial WM deficit. Alternatively, if the DM or DL cortex contains the executive processor, lesions of one or both areas should produce a supra-domain deficit, particularly in both self-ordering tasks, which tap the more complex processing functions of WM.
A preliminary report of these data has been published previously in abstract form (Levy and Goldman-Rakic, 1997).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
General procedure
All animals included in this study were first trained to perform four different WM tasks (two spatial and two nonspatial) at a stable and high level of performance. They were then assigned to receive a bilateral aspiration lesion of either the DM or DL region of the PFC. The monkeys were tested again on all tasks after surgery. Postoperative performance of each group was compared with its preoperative performance (“within-group” comparison) and with each other (“between-group” comparison).
Animals
Six adult rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), four males and two females (one in each of the two groups), were able to complete the study. These animals were housed in separate cages in animal rooms under standard conditions of temperature, relative humidity, air exchange, and day/night cycles. They were fed a diet of monkey chow and fruit adjusted to maintain a stable level of performance. Water was available ad libitum. This study was performed in accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals adopted and promulgated by the National Institutes of Health. All procedures were approved by the Yale Animal Care and Use Committee.
Preoperative training
Behavioral tasks. The four tasks were given in the following order: (1) spatial delayed response (SDR) task, (2) three-position self-ordered (POS-SO) task, (3) delayed object nonmatching-to-sample (DNMTS) task, and (4) three-object self-ordered (OBJ-SO) task. Monkeys were first habituated to a Wisconsin General Testing Apparatus (WGTA), in which they moved freely. The testing sessions in the WGTA were given in a darkened and sound-shielded room with a background of 80 dB white noise. An opaque sliding screen separated the monkey’s compartment from a test tray during the delay periods of each task and between intertrial intervals. Correct choices were reinforced with preferred rewards (halved peanuts, raisins, or small slices of apple). Incorrect choices were not reinforced. Monkeys were trained to perform 20 trials per session for the SDR and DNMTS tasks and 10 trials per session for the POS-SO and OBJ-SO tasks. In all tasks, monkeys received one session per day, 5 d per week.
Spatial delayed response task. The training on the SDR task was performed in several steps (Goldman, 1971). Initially, a brief delay (1 sec) was interposed between the baiting and the response. Once the monkey achieved a criterion of 18 or more correct out of the 20 trials (90% correct responses in one session), the length of the delay period was gradually increased to 3 and 5 sec. At 5 sec, monkeys had to reach a performance criterion of 90 correct responses in 100 trials (i.e., a mean of 90% correct responses over five consecutive sessions). The delay period was then increased to 10 sec, and performance criterion was set at 90% or more correct responses over 5 consecutive days. Monkeys failing to reach this level of performance after 500 trials (25 sessions) were rejected from the study.
Delayed object nonmatching-to-sample task. Monkeys were trained on an object-unique version of the DNMTS task (Bachevalier and Mishkin, 1986). The delay period was 10 sec. Twenty trials were given each day. New objects were selected from a set of 1000 objects. All animals were trained to the criterion of 90 or more correct responses in 100 trials (i.e., a mean of 90% correct responses over five consecutive sessions). Monkeys failing to reach this level of performance after 500 trials (25 sessions) were rejected from the study.
Three-position self-ordered task. In the final phase of the POS-SO task, the monkey confronted a test tray containing nine food wells, spaced 4 cm from each other, arranged in three arrays of three wells (Fig. 1). Three identical blue plaques (5.5 cm square) were placed on three of the nine food wells, each plaque covering a reward. The monkey was then allowed to displace any one of the three plaques and to retrieve the reward underneath. The opaque screen was then lowered for a 10 sec delay period, during which time the displaced plaque was replaced but the food well was not rebaited. After this first delay, the monkey was allowed to make a second choice by displacing any of the three plaques. However, to find a reward, the animal had to choose from one of the two food wells that had not been chosen previously. After the second choice, the opaque screen was lowered again for a second 10 sec delay period. To complete the trial, after the delay, the monkey had to correctly locate the only remaining well that still contained a reward. The trial was completed only when the monkey had found all rewards. Thus, a trial could be completed in three steps, and the choice of a nonrewarded plaque prolonged the trial until all rewards were retrieved (all choices being separated from each other by a 10 sec delay). Monkeys received 10 trials per day. The plaques were positioned pseudorandomly to prevent automatic spatial strategies of retrieval. Eighty-three different combinations were possible. Performance was evaluated on six parameters: (1) the number of correct second choices per session, (2) the number of correct third choices per session, (3) the number of correct second and third choices per session, (4) the number of correct sequences per session (i.e., the number of trials when the first, second, and third choices were consecutively correct), (5) the total number of choices per session, and (6) the number of perseverative errors per session (i.e., the number of errors made by consecutively choosing the same plaque). The probability of being correct by chance on the first trial is 100%. It decreases to 66.6% by the second choice and to 33.3% by the third choice. The probability of being correct by chance for a whole sequence is the probability of being correct by chance on the third choice, having been correct by chance on the second choice (i.e., one-third × two-thirds), which is 44.4%. Criterion was 90% or more for combined second and third correct choices (i.e., 18 or more correct out of 20 choices per session) on 5 consecutive days. This criterion had to be reached within 25 consecutive sessions (250 trials) at 10 sec delay. If a monkey failed to reach criterion, 10 more sessions were given and the criterion was lowered to 85%. Monkeys failing to reach this criterion were eliminated from the study.
Monkeys learned this task in several phases, starting with a simple version of the task. In the first version, the monkey performed the task with two plaques without the screen being lowered and raised between choices. The delay period was then introduced, starting with swift lowering and raising of the opaque screen and progressively increasing the delay period to 10 sec. Once this task was acquired, the POS-SO task was introduced, again with a progressive increase of the delay period. For both tasks, criterion was 90% correct responses for 5 consecutive days at 5 and 10 sec delay periods. For other intermediate delay periods, the criterion was 90% correct responses in one session. Monkeys failing to reach criterion within 25 sessions at any of these steps were eliminated from the study.
Three-object self-ordered task. The OBJ-SO task is based on that introduced by Petrides (1995). In the final stage of training (Fig. 2), the general format of the task is identical to the POS-SO task described above. The main difference between the OBJ-SO and POS-SO tasks was that in the former, the relevant choices were based on the physical attributes of objects (they differed by shape, color, and size) and not on their position. Thus, to prevent the monkeys from adopting a strategy of retrieval based on position, the location of objects was changed on each trial after a pseudorandom order. Monkeys received 10 trials per day. Three different objects from the 1000 member set were used on each trial. Because the combination of objects was randomized at the beginning of each session, the probability of encountering the same combination twice was virtually zero. Objects were first presented to the monkeys before testing began, and those provoking a fear reaction were not used in the task. Performance was assessed on the same six parameters used in the POS-SO task (see above). The probabilities of being correct by chance are identical to those described for the POS-SO task. All animals were trained to the criterion of 90% correct on cumulative second and third choice scores over five consecutive sessions (i.e., 90 or more correct out of 100 choices) within 25 sessions (250 trials). If a monkey failed to reach this criterion, 10 more sessions were given and the criterion was lowered at 85%. Monkeys failing to reach this criterion were eliminated from the study.
Monkeys learned this task in several phases starting with a simple version of the task. On the first version, they had to perform a two-object self-ordered task. In this task, using a two-well board, the monkeys had to self-order their choices between two different objects. The delay period between the first and second choice was progressively increased to 10 sec. Once this task was acquired, the OBJ-SO task was introduced, again with a progressive increase of the delay period. Criterion was 90% correct responses for 5 consecutive days at 5 and 10 sec delay periods. For other intermediate delay periods, the criterion was 90% correct responses in one session. Monkeys failing to reach criterion within 25 sessions at any of these steps were eliminated from the study.
Surgery
Animals were restrained using ketamine (10 mg/kg) and atropine sulfate (0.2 mg/kg). After intravenous catheterization, a flash injection (50 mg/ml) of sodium pentobarbitol was followed by continual perfusion throughout the procedure. One-stage bilateral aspiration lesions were made under aseptic conditions. The monkeys were divided into two groups. Group DL (n = 3; two males and one female) received lesions of area 46 and anterior 8A, intended to include the lips, banks, and depths of the principal sulcus throughout its entire extent and the cortex anterior to the arcuate sulcus as well as the adjacent cortices up to 5 mm below and above the principal sulcus. The second group (n = 3; two males and one female) received DM lesions of areas 9 and 8B, starting ∼5 mm above the principal sulcus and including the entire Walker’s area 9, sparing the frontopolar cortex rostrally and Brodmann’s area 6 caudally.
Postoperative phase
After surgery, monkeys were given 2 weeks of recovery followed by a postoperative training phase in which they were tested on the same four tasks as before lesions. Testing used the 10 sec delay version of each of the four tasks. The order of tasks was identical to that used in the preoperative phase. Monkeys were trained to criteria (i.e., five consecutive sessions at 90% correct responses) or 25 sessions on each task, whichever came first.
Histology
At the end of the experiments, the animals received a lethal dose of sodium pentobarbitol, administered intravenously. They were then perfused transcardially with 4% paraformaldehyde. The brain was removed from the skull and fixed in formalin, before being blocked and processed. Coronal sections and surface reconstruction of the lesions for each case are illustrated in Figures3 and4.
Analysis of results
The results were analyzed in two ways. The first compared performance before and after the lesion in each group separately (within group comparison). The second analysis compared postoperative performance between the two groups of lesioned monkeys (between groups comparison). In both analyses, group performance was the mean of the five best consecutive sessions.
Statistical analyses for the between-groups comparison were performed using one-factor ANOVA (Statview 4.5, Abacus Concept) with lesion group (DM and DL) as the factor. Within-group comparison was performed using two-tailed paired t tests (Statview 4.5, Abacus Concept). This analysis was used to compare the performance before and after lesions in each group. All data are presented as the mean ± SEM. The null hypothesis was rejected at an α risk of 5%.
RESULTS
Preoperative performances
Animals were assigned to lesion groups in advance of preoperative training. Only the monkeys who reached criterion performance on all tasks were included in the lesion groups. There were no differences in preoperative performance scores between groups on any of the four WM tasks (Tables 1,2). All monkeys were first trained on the simple storage WM task, i.e., the SDR task. Because the tasks that were presumed to be more difficult (self-ordered tasks) were learned after the SDR task, their acquisition might have been facilitated by the learning of the simple WM tasks that preceded them. It is thus not possible to use the number of days to criterion for each task as an index of difficulty. However, it should be noted that three monkeys who reached criterion on the SDR task had to be eliminated from the study because they were unable to reach criterion on the POS-SO task, supporting the assumption that the latter task was more difficult because of its higher storage/processing demands.
Postoperative performance
SDR task (Fig. 5)
The monkeys with DL lesions were significantly and markedly impaired both compared with preoperative performance (percentage of correct responses ± SEM, before lesions: 91.36 ± 0.90; after lesions: 66 ± 7.18, p < 0.05) and relative to the DM group (F(1,4)= 46,7, p < 0.005). Furthermore, the DL group showed no evidence of improvement over the course of testing (one-factor ANOVA with repeated measures with trials as the factor and the number of correct sequences as the dependent measure:F(2,24) = 1.29, p = 0.22) (also see Fig. 6). By contrast, DM monkeys performed above the 90% level postoperatively (93.41 ± 2.09), and this performance did not differ significantly from preoperative performance (p = 0.88).
DNMTS task (Fig. 5)
The DL lesion group was neither impaired nor improved as compared with the preoperative phase (92.00 ± 1.73 vs 94.67 ± 2.33,p = 0.18) and did not differ from the DM group (F(1,4) = 0.23, p = 0.66). The DM group performed significantly better postoperatively than preoperatively (96.00 ± 1.53 vs 91.67 ± 1.53,p = 0.02). There was no difference between lesion groups in the number of sessions necessary to reach criterion in the postoperative phase.
POS-SO task (Table 1, Fig. 7)
DL lesions produced a sharp drop in performance compared with preoperative performance (p < 0.05) for all measures except for the percentage of correct second choices (p = 0.13). Furthermore, DL monkeys performed significantly more poorly than the DM group, on all of the six performance measures (F(1,4) > 7.8,p < 0.05, on all measures). To determine whether the DL monkeys improved with postoperative experience, we performed an analysis on the postoperative performance scores throughout the 25 postoperative sessions using a one-factor ANOVA with daily performance as the factor. The result indicated that the performance of the DL group improved in the course of postoperative testing (F(2,24) = 2,2, p = 0.01). However, it is important to note that none of the monkeys in this group were able to reach criterion on any of the six measures. By contrast, monkeys with lesions of the DM region performed as well postoperatively as preoperatively on this task (p > 0.05 for all measures) (Table 1, Fig. 7). However, two of the three monkeys with DM lesions (DM1 and DM2) showed a slight decrement in performance after surgery on one of the six measures—the percentage of correct sequences (DM1: before lesions, 86 ± 2.45,after lesions, 78 ± 3.74; DM2: before lesions, 82 ± 5.83, after lesions, 70 ± 7.07).
OBJ-SO task (Table 2, Fig. 7)
No significant differences were found between preoperative and postoperative performance in the DM group (in all measures,p > 0.05) or the DL group (in all measures,p > 0.05). DL and DM groups did not differ from one another, although monkeys generally performed somewhat more poorly on all measures postoperatively (in all measures,F(1,4) < 2, p > 0.05).
DISCUSSION
The present study demonstrates a functional dissociation between the dorsomedial and dorsolateral PFC with respect to WM processes. Only lesions of dorsolateral areas 46/8A impaired WM processes, whereas lesions of the dorsomedial areas 9/8B were without lasting consequences. Second, the deficit after the dorsolateral lesion was confined to the spatial domain and did not encompass object WM. Finally, the findings revealed that dorsolateral cortical areas are no less critical for tasks with low WM demand than for more complex sequential processing with higher WM load. These results support an anatomical–functional segregation of the PFC for WM based on the type of information being processed rather than on the nature of the operations performed.
Processes and brain regions engaged in the SDR and DNMTS tasks
DL lesions produced a severe and stable impairment on the SDR task, whereas monkeys with lesions of the DM convexity exhibited either a transient impairment or performed as well as they did preoperatively. Both findings are in accord with numerous studies demonstrating that lesions of dorsolateral PFC, and more specifically those restricted to the principal sulcus and the middle third of its sulcus (Butters and Pandya, 1969; Butters et al., 1971, 1972, Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Fuster, 1989), are sufficient to produce a deficit as severe as larger PFC lesions. In contrast, neither spatial delayed response nor delayed alternation deficits are observed after lesions of either the adjacent dorsomedial cortex (Goldman, 1971) or the inferior convexity cortex (Mishkin and Manning, 1978; Passingham, 1985). Collectively, these findings confirm the critical role of the principal sulcal region in the most elementary WM operation within the spatial domain, i.e., the on-line maintenance and use of mental representations for simple actions based on binary choice.
The DNMTS task is used to measure recognition memory. Although recognition memory refers to the ability to form lasting traces and requires medial temporal cortical regions (Mishkin and Murray, 1994), this task can also be viewed as a low-demand WM task. Indeed, in the DNMTS task, the monkey has to keep “in mind” the previous nonrelevant stimulus. In the present study, neither monkeys with DM nor those with DL lesions were impaired on a trial-unique version of DNMTS. Similar results have been reported previously for matching or nonmatching-to-sample tasks (Stamm and Rosen, 1973;Passingham, 1975; Mishkin and Manning, 1978; Bachevalier and Mishkin, 1986). Thus, there is little evidence to indicate a role of either the DM or DL prefrontal sectors in object recognition or in basic short-term memory for object features. In contrast, impairments in performance of DNMTS tasks have been reported after lesions of the inferior convexity (Mishkin and Manning, 1978; Kowalska et al., 1991) or ventromedial prefrontal lesions (Bachevalier and Mishkin; 1986,Meunier et al., 1997). Although the nature of the underlying impairment after such lesions remains elusive, a WM deficit specific to features of objects could account for the behavioral impairments observed after inferior prefrontal lesions. Recent electrophysiological studies have revealed that neurons in these regions are highly responsive to visual stimulation and to the recall of objects or faces and are unresponsive in SDR tasks (Wilson et al., 1993; O’Scalaidhe et al., 1997,1999).
Processes and brain regions engaged in the SO tasks
The SO tasks, used originally in frontal lobe patients by Petrides and Milner (1982), require the subject to monitor the order of items in short-term memory. In the present study, we used a nonspatial SO task (OBJ-SO) based on Petrides (1995) and designed a new spatial version (POS-SO) expressly for this study. The SO tasks require the same basic WM processes engaged by the SDR task (i.e., maintenance and use of internal representations). However, they impose a larger memory load (three items vs one) than do the DR tasks, and the monkey also has to compare his current choice with previous choices, i.e., it has to serially self-order internal representations. This additional monitoring process is conceptually an important upgrade in the level of processing over the classic DR tasks, requiring a supplementary executive operation.
DL lesions produced a profound and persistent impairment in the POS-SO task. Because the POS-SO task requires the same basic WM processing as the SDR task, it is not possible to determine, from our study, the contribution to this deficit of the additional components that differentiate the POS-SO task from the SDR task. However, all monkeys with DL lesions were impaired even at the second choice, a condition that is less demanding than the SDR format because the probability of being correct by chance is higher than in the SDR task. These results suggest that difficulties in the POS-SO task could be attributed, in large part, to the short-term memory deficit responsible for the SDR task. This would favor a role of the DL area both in elementary WM operations (maintenance) and in more executive operations (self ordering), and indeed, the maintenance of single items in short-term memory is a constituent operation of sequential processing. An alternative interpretation of these data is that the dorsolateral region is mainly involved in storing spatial information, whereas another area of the PFC may be selectively involved in the executive aspects of WM. If this were the case, it would be necessary to demonstrate that there is a cortical area, damage to which causes deficits on the POS-SO task but not in the SDR task. No such executive area has yet been discovered, and the requisite double dissociation remains to be demonstrated. At present, a parsimonious interpretation of our data is that the storage and processing components of working memory are inextricable within informational domains.
Our failure to demonstrate impairment in the POS-SO task after DM lesions (areas 9 and 8B) indicates that these regions are no more essential for self-ordering spatial information than they are for the simple maintenance of spatial information. These results are in accord with a study by Passingham (1985), in which monkeys were taught to retrieve rewards hidden behind 25 small doors without returning to the same location twice, thus engaging self-monitoring and serial organization of mental representations. Yet, lesions of the principal sulcus produced a marked impairment in this task, whereas dorsomedial frontal convexity lesions [similar to our DM lesions and to the mid-frontal lesion of Petrides (1995)] failed to do so.
Neither DL nor DM lesions produced impairments on the OBJ-SO task, suggesting that neither subregion is critical for accomplishing this task. This result differs from those obtained by Petrides (1995) in which monkeys with mid-frontal lesions were profoundly impaired on a nonspatial SO task similar to that used in the present study. However, the two studies differ in several ways. The resection in the Petrides study (1995) corresponds closely to our DM lesion because it removed mainly areas 9 and 8B and largely spared the principal sulcus in most animals. However, even if a small part of area 46 were included in the lesion, this fact cannot explain the discrepancy between the two studies because there is no reason to suspect that a DM + DL lesion would produce a deficit that neither the DM nor DL lesions produce separately. We believe that methodological differences may be the more significant factor in explaining the discrepancies between the two studies. Thus, in Petrides’ study, the monkeys’ performance was evaluated in 40 trials (one trial per day over 40 d), whereas in the present study, monkeys were tested for 250 trials (10 trials per day for 25 d), unless they reached criterion earlier. When we performed an analysis on only the first 40 postoperative trials, the performance of the monkeys with DM lesions was ∼55–60% correct for both OBJ-SO and POS-SO tasks, similar to Petrides’ result. However, monkeys in the present study received six times as many trials as did monkeys in the Petrides study, and as training progressed, performance progressively increased to criterion on the two self-ordering tasks. Thus, the deficit produced on these tasks by DM lesions is transient. We also observed a transient decrease in the level of performance on the same task after DL lesions. Monkeys with DL lesions performed at ∼57% correct on the first 40 trials and thereafter reached criterion. Altogether, these findings suggest that both DM and DL lesions induce a transient and nonspecific deficit in SO tasks, but only DL lesions produce a profound and lasting domain-specific deficit that is present throughout training.
Toward a model of functional segregation of WM within the lateral PFC in nonhuman primates
One model of functional organization of the lateral PFC proposes that it is a unitary association cortex that integrates information in a supramodal manner. Fuster and colleagues (Fuster and Bauer, 1974;Bauer and Fuster, 1976; Quintana and Fuster, 1993) have shown that cooling of area 46 produced both spatial and nonspatial short-term memory deficits and also impairment on a delayed matching task with cross-modal (visual and somesthetic sensory modalities) contingencies. However, as recognized by the authors (Fuster, 1989), a functional dissociation within the lateral PFC is not ruled out because of the large portion of the PFC cooled in these studies. Several authors have recorded activation in dorsolateral PFC in response to nonspatial stimuli (Tanila et al., 1992, 1993; Carlson et al., 1997; Rao et al., 1997). Moreover, some neurons throughout the dorsolateral PFC are activated by both spatial and nonspatial visual stimuli during WM tasks if these stimuli are presented within central vision (Rao et al., 1997;Rainer et al., 1998). Neuronal responses in PFC for both spatial and nonspatial items are not necessarily inconsistent with a domain-specific model because the specificity of the DL region for spatial WM operations could be relative, and some neurons within this area may communicate with other PFC domain-specific regions. However, even the existence of such responses would not specify their functional importance for that area. Lesion studies emphasize this concept: DL lesions consistently produce a deficit restricted to visuospatial cognition (for review, see Goldman-Rakic, 1987). Rarely, if ever, have monkeys with DL lesions exhibited permanent impairment on object-based tasks.
Another model proposed by Petrides and Owen (Petrides et al., 1993;Owen et al., 1996a,b, 1998) postulates that the PFC is fractionated into two areas, according to the nature of the operations processed (see introductory remarks). Although recent functional imaging studies in humans have been interpreted as support for this model, evidence for this model from experimental studies in monkeys is weak. Indeed, the one study supporting this hypothesis (Petrides, 1995) did not compare the effects of a mid-dorsolateral lesion to sequelae of either area 46 or inferior convexity lesions. The present study, using a task similar to that of Petrides, failed to detect a deficit in OBJ-SO tasks after DM and DL lesions. In contrast to the hierarchical model described above, the present finding of a dissociation of deficits on nonspatial and spatial tasks after DL lesions and the marked impairment observed in both simple and complex spatial WM tasks confirm and extend the evidence for a specialization of DL cortex for spatial WM, regardless of the level of processing within this domain. This specialization is supported by a wealth of single-unit recording studies of areas 46/8a neurons engaged in short-term spatial-mnemonic functions (Fuster and Alexander, 1971; Kubota and Niki, 1971; Kojima and Goldman-Rakic, 1982; Funahashi et al., 1989, 1990, 1991; Carlson et al., 1990, 1997; Wilson et al., 1993), as well as numerous anatomical studies (Mesulam et al., 1977; Barbas and Mesulam, 1985; Selemon and Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic, 1989). Although the present study does not specify a particular area for nonspatial WM processing, several studies indicate that the inferior convexity (mainly Walker’s areas 12/45) is specialized for nonspatial processing (Passingham, 1975; Mishkin and Manning, 1978; Bachevalier and Mishkin, 1986; Wilson et al., 1993; O’Scalaidhe et al., 1997, 1999).
One may raise the question of the location of a “central executive” processor (Baddeley, 1996). Our data clearly demonstrate that increasing the load and the manipulation within WM did not recruit the DM region. However, these results do not rule out the involvement of another PFC area in the most complex aspects of executive functions. Alternatively, it is reasonable and appealing that each prefrontal module, networked with sensory, motor, limbic, and association areas (Selemon and Goldman-Rakic, 1988), can support domain-specific executive operations. As mentioned above, the present study does not fully resolve this question because monkeys with DL lesions exhibited a profound impairment even at the level of basic WM processes. Nevertheless, a wide range of operations integral to working memory, including preparation, inhibition, or sequencing of motor responses and context contingencies have recently been observed in DL cortex (Funahashi et al., 1993, 1997; Wilson et al., 1994; Watanabe, 1996). Thus, the intrinsic neuronal circuitry within the DL area provides for a spectrum of functions compatible with all levels of WM operations, including executive operations. Finally, a single area devoted exclusively to supramodal executive functions has not yet been unequivocally identified in either the nonhuman primate brain or the human brain. At the same time, considerable evidence from neurophysiological and anatomical investigations in monkeys (see introductory remarks) and functional imaging studies of human cognition (Smith et al., 1995, 1996; Courtney et al., 1996, 1998; McCarthy et al., 1996; Belger et al., 1998; Kelley et al., 1998; R. Adcock, T. Constable, J. Gore, and P. S. Goldman-Rakic, unpublished observations) is compatible with a parallel organization of domain-specific modules within the PFC.
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants from Fyssen and Philippe Fondations and Servier to R.L. and by National Institutes of Health Grant MH 38546 and EJLB Foundation grants to P.G.-R.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Patricia S. Goldman-Rakic, Section of Neurobiology, Yale University School of Medicine, 333 Cedar Street, New Haven, CT 06510.
Dr. Levy’s present address: Federation de Neurologie and Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale U.289, Hopital de la Salpetriere, 75013 Paris, France.