Abstract
Previous simulations using a realistic model of a cerebellar Purkinje cell suggested that synaptic control of somatic spiking in this cell type is mediated by voltage-gated intrinsic conductances and that inhibitory rather than excitatory synaptic inputs are more influential in controlling spike timing. In this paper, we have tested these predictions physiologically using dynamic current clamping to apply model-derived synaptic conductances to Purkinje cells in vitro. As predicted by the model, this input transformed thein vitro pattern of spiking into a different spike pattern typically observed in vivo. A net inhibitory synaptic current was required to achieve such spiking, indicating the presence of strong intrinsic depolarizing currents. Spike-triggered averaging confirmed that the length of individual intervals between spikes was correlated to the amplitude of the inhibitory conductance but was not influenced by excitatory inputs. Through repeated presentation of identical stimuli, we determined that the output spike rate was very sensitive to the relative balance of excitation and inhibition in the input conductances. In contrast, the accuracy of spike timing was dependent on input amplitude and was independent of spike rate. Thus, information could be encoded in Purkinje cell spiking in a precise spike time code and a rate code at the same time. We conclude that Purkinje cell responses to synaptic input are strongly dependent on active somatic and dendritic properties and that theories of cerebellar function likely need to incorporate single-cell dynamics to a greater degree than is customary.
The near-crystalline anatomical organization of cerebellar cortex has traditionally been the main impetus for theoretical considerations of cerebellar function (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971). In these theories, single neurons are primarily treated as integrate-and-fire devices, a treatment that is still customary in large-scale theories of network function. Over the last 20 years, however, it has been well established that Purkinje cells have complex intrinsic dynamical properties (Llinás and Sugimori, 1980a, 1992). Most notably, a large voltage-gated calcium conductance is capable of producing dendritic plateau potentials and calcium spikes (Llinás and Sugimori, 1980b). The Purkinje cell takes a central position in theories of cerebellar function, because it integrates the various sources of input to cerebellar cortex and provides its only source of output. Therefore, a large contribution of active intrinsic properties of Purkinje cells in synaptic integration could be a key element in the elusive algorithm of cerebellar cortical computation. To examine how spike responses may be under the combined control of intrinsic properties and synaptic inputs, we use an interactive approach of computer modeling and physiological recordings.
In previous studies, we have developed a realistic model of a single Purkinje cell to establish a basis for considering the functional significance of its active properties (De Schutter and Bower, 1994a). The model was constructed using the morphology of a real Purkinje cell (Rapp et al., 1994) and includes the full set of voltage-gated conductances identified to date in this neuron. It was tuned to replicate physiological current injection data obtained from the brain-slice preparation (De Schutter and Bower, 1994a) and was subsequently tested by comparing its response to synaptic input with the activity found in real in vivo Purkinje cells (De Schutter and Bower, 1994b). It was found that the model quite closely replicates typical Purkinje cell responses to synaptic input (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c).
Recently, we have more closely analyzed the way in which synaptic input influences the somatic spiking of the model (Jaeger et al., 1997). We found that the model could only generate typical in vivospiking patterns, when the average inhibitory synaptic current exceeded the excitatory current. This requires the presence of a continuous baseline of inhibitory synaptic input. Furthermore, the model predicts that the largest contribution to fluctuations in the membrane potential in the soma as well as the dendritic tree is made by the large intrinsic voltage-dependent currents rather than by the currents associated with either excitatory or inhibitory synaptic inputs.
Modeling predictions rest on many assumptions made in the construction of the model (Bhalla and Bower, 1993). Therefore, modeling results need to be tested experimentally before they can be accepted. The present study was designed to allow such testing of our model predictions regarding the involvement of active cell properties in synaptic integration. To replicate the conditions of the model as closely as possible, we chose to apply synaptic conductances generated by the model to Purkinje cells in vitro using the dynamic current-clamp technique (Sharp et al., 1993). As in the model, the addition of a stochastic baseline of excitatory and inhibitory input resulted in a transition from an in vitro bursting spike pattern to ongoing irregular spiking, which closely resembles spontaneous spiking recorded in vivo (Jaeger and Bower, 1994). The condition of continuous random input used here and in our previous modeling study (Jaeger et al., 1997) reflects our best estimate of synaptic activity expected to be present as a continuous baseline in vivo. Because little is known about the specific input modulations occurring during task-related behavior, we restrict our analysis to a stochastic activation of all inputs. By manipulating the balance and amplitude of excitatory and inhibitory inputs, we address the question of how output spike rate and spike timing reflect synaptic input patterns in the presence of large intrinsic active properties.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Slice preparation and whole-cell recording. All animal procedures fully complied with the National Institutes of Health guidelines on animal care and use. Whole-cell recordings were obtained from the somata of Purkinje cells in 300 μm sagittal slices from 2- to 4-week-old rats under visual control using a fixed-stage microscope. The slice medium contained (in mm): NaCl 124, KCl 5, NaHCO3 26, K2HPO4 1.2, CaCl2 2.4, MgSO4 1.3, and glucose 10. Electrodes were filled with (in mm): K gluconate 120, KCl 10, HEPES 10, EGTA 10, CaCl 2, MgCl 2, Na-ATP 2, and Na-GTP 0.2, with impedances ranging from 6 to 12 MΩ. Recordings were obtained at 32°C. To study the effects of simulated synaptic input in isolation, we blocked endogenous excitatory and inhibitory inputs with 5 μm CNQX and 40 μm picrotoxin, respectively.
Implementation of the dynamic clamp. Synaptic conductances obtained from our Purkinje cell model were applied to Purkinje cellsin vitro using the dynamic current-clamp method (Robinson and Kawai, 1993; Sharp et al., 1993). We implemented this technique using dual whole-cell recordings in which one electrode was used to record membrane potential (Vm) and the other was used to inject simulated synaptic current. The recording electrode was connected to channel 1 on an Axoclamp 2A amplifier, and the feedback current injection was applied through channel 2 of this instrument. The two-electrode approach was chosen because it is not possible to inject sizeable fluctuating currents through an electrode while maintaining an accurate voltage recording with the same electrode. In the feedback condition of dynamic clamping, this problem can easily lead to ringing artifacts. The amount of simulated synaptic current injected (Iinj) was calculated at a 10 kHz refresh rate from the recorded membrane potential (Vm) and two stored synaptic conductance waveforms representing the sum of excitatory (Gex) and inhibitory (Gin) conductances obtained from computer simulations. The equation to calculate synaptic current is:Iinj = Iex +Iin = Gex * (Eex − Vm) +Gin * (Ein − Vm), where Eex andEin are the synaptic reversal potentials of 0 mV for excitation and −70 mV for inhibition, respectively. The recorded membrane potential (Vm) was low-pass filtered at 5 kHz using the Axoclamp 2A output filter before the potential was fed to the dynamic-clamp software to prevent possible aliasing. This software was custom written in Turbo Pascal with in-line assembly code for fast feedback control under the DOS operating system. It works in conjunction with a CIO-DAS 16/F analog-to-digital (AD) board (Computer Boards), which uses an on-board clock to time simultaneous AD conversion and digital-to-analog (DA) output. The acquisition program polls the AD conversion and calculates the feedback equation for the next time step while all external interrupts are disabled. The system was tested to be reliable for 20 kHz feedback rates with artificial input voltages consisting of square voltage pulses. The DAS 16/F DA converters put out voltages from 0 to 5 V. A hardware device was constructed to subtract 2.5 V from this signal and to provide low-pass filtering at a cutoff frequency of 10 kHz. This filtering was needed to eliminate very-high-frequency noise generated by the computer. The resulting output voltage from −2.5 to 2.5 V was used as input to the “current command” BNC connector on the back of the Axoclamp 2A amplifier. Because an HS2A 0.1LU head stage was used, the command voltage allowed dynamic current clamping for maximal current values of ±2.5 nA. The maximal value of +2.5 nA was never reached, whereas −2.5 nA was injected transiently at the peak of somatic spikes in some recordings.
Recording stability and potential experimental problems.Data acquisition was started directly after a dual whole-cell configuration was obtained and was continued for as long as the physiological characteristics of the cell remained stable. The characteristics used were spike size (>45 mV), a stable spike rate during dynamic-clamp stimuli, and the absence of depolarization-induced spike inactivation. Although a gradual change in cell properties might be expected because of the perfusion with the pipette solution, such a change was not observed for the spike responses with dynamic clamping examined here. On theoretical grounds, a junction potential of ∼10 mV can be expected for recording solutions based on K gluconate (Barry, 1994). This junction potential, however, can be mostly offset by a Donnan potential of opposite sign (Barry and Lynch, 1991). The large intracellular anions responsible for this Donnan potential equilibrate with the electrode solution over a few minutes in small cells (Pusch and Neher, 1988). This equilibration may not occur to a significant degree in large cells, such as Purkinje cells, during the period of time over which we acquired data. We examined a possible voltage offset in our whole-cell recordings by comparing the observed spike threshold with that of previous recordings we obtained in the same preparation with sharp electrodes (Jaeger and Bower, 1994). We found no consistent offset in spike threshold between the two methods of recording. Therefore we did not subtract a junction potential from the recorded Vm before feeding it into the dynamic clamp. We cannot exclude some error in the absolute membrane potential recorded in individual cells. With respect to dynamic current clamping, an offset in recorded Vm is equivalent to shifting the synaptic reversal potentials of excitation and inhibition by the same offset. Therefore differences in the absolute level of depolarization and the spike rate of different neurons obtained with the same input pattern should be interpreted with care.
The use of 10 mm EGTA in our electrode solution could potentially reduce the activation of Ca-dependent K conductances in the cell by buffering the required Ca. Thus intrinsic hyperpolarizing currents could be diminished. In a later set of experiments (D. Jaeger, unpublished observations), the internal solution was changed to contain only 0.1 mm EGTA. The results of these later experiments show that the intrinsic balance of inward and outward currents during dynamic current clamping is in the same range with 0.1 or 10 mm EGTA in the recording pipette. The lack of an effect of EGTA can be explained by its slow-buffering characteristics and by the possible activation of Ca-dependent K channels via specific Ca channels located in close proximity. Unfortunately, the dynamics of calcium handling in Purkinje cells is yet poorly understood.
The construction of simulated synaptic conductances. The synaptic conductance patterns that we applied in the present study via dynamic clamping were computed with our Purkinje cell model and were identical to the inputs we used in our previous modeling study (Jaeger et al., 1997). The model was constructed to replicate the best estimate in the number and location of excitatory parallel fiber and inhibitory stellate cell inputs to a single Purkinje cell. The modeling methods used to generate synaptic conductances were described in detail in our previous publications (De Schutter and Bower, 1994a,b; Jaeger et al., 1997). In brief, the excitatory input from a granule cell was modeled as an alpha function with an opening time constant of 0.5 msec, a closing time constant of 1.2 msec, a reversal potential of 0 mV, and a fixed maximal conductance of 0.7 nS. Inhibitory inputs as expected from stellate cell input had an opening time constant of 0.9 msec, a closing time constant of 9 msec, a reversal potential of −70 mV, and a variable amplitude proportional to the surface area of the postsynaptic compartment (resulting in a mean amplitude of 3 nS). The reversal potential of inhibitory input was shifted by 10 mV from the value of −80 mV used previously in the model, which is the only difference between synaptic conductance patterns used here and those in our modeling study (Jaeger et al., 1997). Each synapse was activated randomly at a specified mean rate, which resulted in an exponential distribution in the duration of intervals between inputs. All excitatory synapses were activated with the same mean rate, as were all inhibitory synapses. The rate of activation of each of the 1474 excitatory synapses was increased compared with the expected in vivo situation to compensate for the reduced number of synapses present in the model (De Schutter and Bower, 1994b; Jaeger et al., 1997). To apply synaptic conductances through a single electrode with dynamic current clamping, we pooled conductances from all synapses in the model by summing. On average, conductance waveforms consisted of 848 inhibitory and 16,225 excitatory inputs per second (Jaeger et al., 1997).
The choice of synaptic parameters. In our original modeling efforts, the number of parallel fiber inputs to a single Purkinje cell were based on known anatomical data (Harvey and Napper, 1988). The location of synapses on the dendrite was based on EM data (Palay and Chan-Palay, 1974). The number of stellate cell inputs and the choice of conductance parameters for single inputs were partly based on best-guess estimates, however (De Schutter and Bower, 1994b). Since the original model was constructed, several groups have reported amplitudes and time constants for both excitatory (Barbour, 1993) and inhibitory (Vincent et al., 1992; Barbour, 1993; Häusser and Clark, 1997) synaptic inputs to Purkinje cells. These data show that the original values chosen in the model were quite accurate, except for the decay time constant of inhibitory inputs. In the modeling and experimental results reported in this paper, this decay time constant was shortened from 26 to 9 msec to conform with the experimental finding (Vincent et al., 1992). The number of 1695 inhibitory synapses used in the model was approximately realistic (Jaeger et al., 1997; Sultan and Bower, 1998).
The combined reversal potential of excitatory and inhibitory conductances defines a partial voltage clamp. The combined reversal potential of excitatory and inhibitory inputs (Etot) applied with dynamic clamping can be calculated as the weighted sum of the reversal potentials of excitation and inhibition: Etot = (Gex * Eex +Gin *Ein)/(Gex +Gin). This combined reversal potential (Etot) was termed Vclamp in our modeling publication (Jaeger et al., 1997) because the dynamic clamp creates a voltage clamp such that any deviation in membrane potential from Vclamp is counteracted by an opposing change in synaptic current. If Vm followed this trajectory of Vclamp exactly, no synaptic current would be injected at any time. As with a voltage-clamp amplifier, the amplitude of injected current is proportional to the deviation of Vm from Vclamp. The gain of the voltage clamp exerted by synaptic input corresponds to the total amplitude of synaptic conductanceGex + Gin. Because this gain is rather low compared with a regular voltage clamp and easily allows the escape of Vm from Vclamp, we refer to dynamic clamping as a partial voltage clamp. This analysis also applies to a baseline of continuous synaptic input conductances expected to be present in vivo.
The validity of using focalized input to examine the function of natural inputs. The technique of dynamic current clamping requires that simulated synaptic input be applied via one command current at a single point in the cell. In contrast, Purkinje cells in vivo receive distributed synaptic input. Distributed input is the situation we recreated in our previous modeling study (Jaeger et al., 1997) to examine the interaction of intrinsic cell properties with synaptic input under conditions expected in vivo. To interpret the results of dynamic current clamping, it is crucial to establish the effects of focalizing synaptic input at the soma in comparison with distributed dendritic inputs. In the Purkinje cell model, the effect of distributed dendritic input is critically dependent on the activation of dendritic voltage-gated currents (Jaeger et al., 1997). Therefore we need to establish whether synaptic input focalized at the soma can still evoke these dendritic currents.
When only the passive properties of Purkinje cells are considered, these cells are electrotonically very compact. The average length from the soma to the tip of a dendrite is only 0.13 length constants (Rapp et al., 1994). Therefore, a prolonged current injected into the soma will lead to a nearly uniform change in membrane potential throughout the cell (Rall, 1959). The dendritic attenuation increases for higher frequencies of changes in membrane potential (Vm) at the soma, however (Spruston et al., 1993). As a result, dendritic Vm follows slow fluctuations (<10 Hz) in somatic Vm very well, whereas fast fluctuations (>100 Hz) are severely attenuated in the distal dendrites (Spruston et al., 1993). Even for a total dendritic length of 1.0 length constants, this relationship still holds. This is the maximum length expected for Purkinje cell dendrites in the presence of voltage-gated conductances (De Schutter and Bower, 1994a). The fluctuations in our simulated synaptic input are almost entirely contained in the frequency band from 0 to 100 Hz. Thus, a large part of this signal is expected to spread to most of the dendritic tree and to influence the activation of dendritic conductances. To establish in detail how much the difference between distributed and focalized input may affect Purkinje cell responses, we analyzed the results from both input conditions in the model.
Figure 1 compares the model’s response when synaptic input was present throughout the dendrite (A–C) with data obtained when synaptic input was focalized at the soma as is the case with dynamic current clamping (D–F). The focalized input was obtained by summing all synaptic conductances of the simulation using distributed synapses and by applying the sum to the soma. To achieve a spike rate with focalized input that matched the spike rate observed with distributed input, we adjusted the reversal potential of inhibitory input from −80 to −72 mV. This requirement for a less-hyperpolarized inhibitory reversal potential indicates that inhibitory input focalized at the soma is somewhat more effective in suppressing spiking than is distributed dendritic input. Note that the inhibitory reversal potential of −72 mV in the focalized model is quite close to the −70 mV we used during dynamic current clamping. Shifting the reversal potential of inhibition between the distributed and focalized input conditions allowed us to use the same conductance patterns while retaining a matching spike rate. The resulting voltage traces show that both types of input induced the irregular pattern of spiking characteristic of the in vivo state (see Figs.1A, 3). Furthermore, the total synaptic current and the evoked intrinsic voltage-gated currents (Fig.1C,F) were very similar in both cases. This means that the focalized synaptic inputs at the soma still resulted in the activation of dendritic voltage-gated currents. In both cases the voltage-gated conductances in the dendrite produced a sustained inward current (ICa+K; Fig.1C,F). This current resulted in a similar plateau depolarization of the dendrite for distributed and focalized input (Fig. 1A,D). The mean outward synaptic current (Fig. 1C,F) indicates that the depolarizing currents in the cell pushed Vm in the soma above the net synaptic reversal potential. Therefore, the net function of synaptic conductances was to counteract intrinsic inward conductances. Although the exact timing of individual spikes was different for the distributed and focalized synaptic input, periods of increased spike rate clearly matched. In both input conditions, these periods of spiking were carried by a relative increase of inward dendritic current (horizontal bars in Fig.1C,F). These similarities indicate that dendritic input in Purkinje cells does not lead to large nonlinear local effects in the dendrite. An absence of differences in cell response to clustered or distributed dendritic input was observed previously in the model (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c). As may be expected from the preferential dendritic attenuation of high-frequency components in somatic input, focalized input resulted in maintained slow fluctuations in dendritic responses compared with that in distributed input. In contrast, the fastest components of the response to input were partly different between the two conditions.
The observed similarities in the response of the model to focalized or distributed synaptic input suggest that dynamic current clamping is a reasonable approximation to distributed synaptic input for the modeling predictions we want to test. We therefore proceed to use this method to apply model-based synaptic input patterns to Purkinje cells in vitro.
RESULTS
Stochastic synaptic inputs are sufficient to elicit in vivo spike patterns in slice recordings
As shown in Figure2A and in previous publications (Llinás and Sugimori, 1980a,b), in vitrocerebellar Purkinje cells exhibit spontaneous periods of fast regular somatic spiking that gradually lead to dendritic calcium spike bursting. In contrast, in vivo patterns of spiking are slower, more irregular, and never include spontaneous calcium spikes (Jaeger and Bower, 1994).
Our modeling results predict that the addition of continuous excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input converts the spontaneous in vitro spike pattern of Purkinje cells to an in vivospike pattern (De Schutter and Bower, 1994b; Jaeger et al., 1997). This modeling prediction was tested here by applying the same conductances used in the model via the dynamic clamp in vitro. We found that the in vitro spike pattern (Fig. 2A) was immediately transformed into irregular ongoing spiking (Fig.2C). In contrast, when only excitatory inputs were applied with the dynamic clamp (Fig. 2B), the calcium spike burst pattern of in vitro neurons was enhanced in all cells tested (n = 8). Similar results were obtained from the model (Jaeger et al., 1997).
A comparison of interspike interval (ISI) histograms documents the similarity between in vivo spiking patterns (Fig.3A) and those obtained in the model (Fig. 3B,C) and in thein vitro dynamic current-clamp experiments (Fig.3D). In each case, ISI distributions showed a sharp modal peak followed by a tail of longer intervals. Nevertheless, there is not a perfect match between ISI histograms from the model and the data collected in vivo or in vitro. In particular, the exponential falloff from the modal peak appears to be somewhat less steep in the recordings than in the simulations. This may indicate that the activation of voltage-gated currents that underlies periods of fast spiking in the model (Jaeger and Bower, 1994) does not follow exactly the identical kinetics as in the cells for which recordings are shown. Note that the identical synaptic input used in the model produced an ISI distribution in vitro that more closely resembles the in vivo recording than the modeling results.
In vivo–type spiking behavior requires a net outward synaptic current
Our modeling results further predicted that in vivospiking patterns are specifically dependent on synaptic inputs summing to a mean outward (inhibitory) current (Jaeger et al., 1997). As shown in Figure 4, this prediction is supported by the dynamic-clamp experiments. In all recordings (n= 20), a mean outward synaptic current was necessary to generatein vivo–type spike patterns. The net outward synaptic current trace shown in Figure 4D is very similar to that seen in simulations (Fig. 1). For nine cells analyzed quantitatively with the same stimulus, the mean outward current duringin vivo–type spiking was −0.45 nA (SD between cells, 0.08 nA). This hyperpolarizing synaptic current indicates that on average intrinsic depolarizing currents push the cell’s membrane potential above the combined reversal potential of inhibition and excitation (Fig. 4C). In fact, the recorded Vmwas on average 1.3 mV more depolarized than was the combined synaptic reversal potential (Fig. 4B,C). This result indicates that in the real cell, as in the model, intrinsic inward plateau currents produce the depolarization required to reach spike threshold and that inhibitory synaptic input acts to limit the activation of these conductances. Candidates for the required noninactivating inward conductances were identified as a dendritic P-type calcium conductance and a somatic persistent Na conductance byLlinás and Sugimori (1980b, 1992). In the absence of inhibitory input, the activation of these conductances is unstable because of positive feedback, which ultimately leads to dendritic calcium spiking.
Interspike interval lengths are related to the strength of inhibitory conductances
Our modeling study (Jaeger et al., 1997) predicted that inhibitory synaptic conductances have a direct influence on the expected duration of intervals between somatic action potentials. Consistent with this prediction, the vertical dashed lines in Figure4 indicate that periods of increased inhibition resulted in a cessation of spiking in the experimental data. This relationship is further quantified in Figure 5 in which we use the technique of spike-triggered averaging to compare the average levels of synaptic conductances associated with short, medium, and long interspike intervals. Figure 5, A and B, reproduces the results of the same analysis reported previously for model data (Jaeger et al., 1997). Figure 5, C andD, shows the experimental data. In both cases inhibitory synaptic conductances were larger, and Vm was more hyperpolarized very early in the time course for long intervals compared with that for short intervals. This relationship was present in all cells analyzed (n = 8). Thus, as in the model, the length of the interspike intervals was correlated with the level of inhibitory input. A direct correlation of natural inhibitory input with prolonged interspike intervals was recently demonstrated in an elegant dual-recording study from interneurons and Purkinje cells (Häusser and Clark, 1997).
Excitatory inputs have relatively little direct effect on spike initiation
The spike-triggered averages shown in Figure 5 also allowed us to examine the relationship between somatic spiking and simulated excitatory synaptic conductances. In the model (Fig. 5B) there was virtually no change in the average level of excitatory conductance before, during, or after an action potential. This modeling result led to the somewhat surprising suggestion that spike initiation does not reflect a previous increase in excitatory input (Jaeger et al., 1997). Furthermore, the mean level of excitation was not related to the control of ISI duration. The spike-triggered averages obtained from physiological records in these experiments support the same conclusion (Fig. 5D). The level of excitatory conductances during an ISI was at no time correlated with ISI duration in any of eight cells analyzed.
The fact that excitatory inputs in the conductances we use are more frequent, smaller in amplitude, and shorter in duration than are inhibitory inputs may account for their ineffectiveness in controlling spike timing. Because of these statistical properties, the total excitatory conductance exhibits much less fluctuation than the total inhibitory conductance does (Fig. 4A). The chosen input properties are not arbitrary, because they were based on well established anatomical and physiological data (see Materials and Methods). Nevertheless, our assumption of independent stochastic activation of synapses represents only one possible input condition. Many excitatory inputs firing together at the same time does result in a precisely timed spike (De Schutter and Bower, 1994c). Whether and under what conditions such highly correlated inputs may occur cannot be answered by the present study.
The level of excitatory synaptic conductance strongly affects the rate of spiking via the activation of intrinsic currents
The absence of a correlation between excitatory inputs and the duration of individual interspike intervals does not imply that excitatory input is without function. In fact, the model predicts that Purkinje cells should show a dramatic increase in spike rate for only moderate increases in the amount of excitation received (De Schutter and Bower, 1994b). Such a high gain in the change of output spike rate would make the Purkinje cell a very sensitive rate-coding device for the average level of excitatory input received (Jaeger et al., 1997). In the model, this high gain in the spike output rate is primarily caused by dendritic P-type Ca channel activation (Jaeger et al., 1997). To investigate the gain function of the output spike rate in vitro, we increased the amount of baseline excitation in the dynamic current clamp (Fig. 6). This was done by adding a constant excitatory conductance such that the fluctuations in the synaptic conductances remained identical. An increase of 10 or 30% in the mean excitatory conductance led to an increase in spike rate of 43 or 115%, respectively (average of eight cells). Thus, as predicted by the model, the in vitrorecordings showed a high gain function for the rate coding of excitatory input levels. The increases in spike rate of 43 and 115% were accompanied by a depolarizing shift in the average Vmof 1.3 and 3.8 mV, respectively.
The depolarization and increase in spiking seen with increased excitatory conductance were not caused by an inward current via the simulated synaptic conductance. In fact the mean synaptic current remained outward with nearly identical amplitude for the lowest and highest amount of excitation (−0.45 and −0.41 nA, respectively;n = 8 cells). This result was also predicted by the model. The lack of an increased inward synaptic current with an increase in excitatory conductance proves that intrinsic inward current is responsible for the additional depolarization. As the cell is allowed to depolarize more by a depolarizing shift in the combined synaptic reversal potential, inward voltage-dependent currents are activated more strongly. The P-type calcium channel in particular shows a steep activation increase in the relevant voltage range of −50 to −35 mV (Regan, 1991). This increased intrinsic inward current again pushes the cell beyond the synaptic reversal potential, and a net outward synaptic current is maintained. In principle, this mechanism allows neurons to achieve arbitrarily high gain functions in spike rate for small increases in excitatory input. Decreases in inhibitory input were similarly effective in increasing spike rates (data not shown). This is not surprising, because decreases in inhibition have a similar effect on shifting the combined synaptic reversal potential as do increases in excitation. The appearance of dendritic calcium spikes in the absence of inhibitory input (Fig.2A,B) indicates that outward synaptic current is important in preventing a runaway activation of intrinsic inward current. The Purkinje cell model further suggests that intrinsic inward current is also balanced by intrinsic voltage and Ca-gated K conductances.
The precision of spike timing is dependent on the absolute amplitude of synaptic conductances
The strong participation of intrinsic currents in mediating the effect of synaptic input poses the question of how precisely spike timing can still be controlled by the synaptic conductance pattern. The precision of spike timing has recently received much attention as a possible mechanism for neural coding (Mainen and Sejnowski, 1995;König et al., 1996). To test for the control of precise spike timing by synaptic conductances in Purkinje cells, we applied synaptic conductances of different amplitudes. Specifically, we applied synaptic input conductances with different absolute amplitudes while maintaining an identical time course in the trajectory of the combined reversal potential. This was achieved by multiplying both inhibitory and excitatory conductance patterns by constant gain factors of 0.5, 1.0, or 2.0.
By applying the same conductance traces repeatedly via the dynamic clamp, we could assess the relationship between a given simulated synaptic input and the precision of spike timing (Fig.7). Spike rasters show that an increase in the synaptic gain results in an improved alignment of individual spikes between stimulus repetitions (Fig. 7A). This effect was quantified by a cross-correlation analysis, in which the timing of spikes for successive pairs of stimulus repetitions was correlated (Fig. 7B). For all stimulus gains, we find a very sharp peak in the cross-correlograms, indicating the presence of precisely timed spikes. The proportion of spikes precisely timed within ±1 msec was much higher for the input gain of 2 (78%) than for the input gain of 0.5 (50%; average of four cells). In addition, several cells showed a total loss of correlation with the synaptic input at a gain of 0.5 by entering an oscillatory bursting pattern (data not shown). Thus the gain of 0.5 was near the minimum of synaptic conductance needed to prevent runaway activation of intrinsic currents. These findings indicate that at a sufficient amplitude of synaptic conductances, a substantial proportion of spikes can be timed precisely in relation to the input even in the presence of large intrinsic currents.
A striking effect of applying conductances with different gain factors was consistently found; increasing the conductance amplitude led to a decrease in spike rate (Fig. 7A). This effect further substantiates the presence of inward intrinsic currents that push the membrane potential above the synaptic reversal potential. The ability of this current to depolarize the cell is reduced in the presence of large synaptic conductances, because the amount of synaptic current that opposes the intrinsic current to push the cell away from the synaptic reversal potential is increased (Fig. 7C). In our data, the average Vm for the input gain of 1 was −47.7 mV, whereas for a gain of 2, it was −49.6 mV (n = 4 cells). The resulting spike rates were 32 and 26 Hz, respectively.
Spiking rates are controlled independently of spiking precision
The previous sections indicate that Purkinje cells are a sensitive rate encoder with respect to the balance of inhibition and excitation but that they can also initiate precisely timed spikes with respect to synaptic conductance fluctuations. We now ask the further question of whether these two aspects of coding information about the input are independent of each other. This question can be addressed by a more detailed analysis of the data with increasing amounts of excitatory input conductance (Fig. 6). This figure allows the important observation that the precise timing of particular spikes, which are present at a low level of excitation, remains largely intact when additional excitatory conductance is added (Fig.6A). Because the synaptic reversal potential is more depolarized with increasing excitation, more spikes are added. The proportion of these additional spikes that are timed precisely within ±1 msec is identical to that seen at a low level of excitation. The cross-correlograms show that the proportion of spikes that occurred with a precision of ±1 msec was 66–67% for all amounts of excitation (average of eight cells) and was thus maintained at low and at high spike rates (Fig. 6B).
Taken together with the findings shown in Figure 7, these data suggest that spike rate and spike precision are primarily controlled by two different properties of the input. Spike rate is mainly a function of the relative level of excitation and inhibition, which determines the average combined reversal potential. In contrast, spike precision depends on the total amplitude of synaptic conductance changes. Therefore, Purkinje cells in principle can transfer information about the input via precisely timed spikes independent of spike rate. Whether information is actually transferred via precisely timed spikes depends on the decoding mechanism at postsynaptic cells and the presence of precisely timed spikes across different Purkinje cells. These issues remain to be addressed in future studies.
Variations in responses to applied inputs between Purkinje cells
Finally, we used the dynamic current clamp to examine variations in the response of different in vitro Purkinje cells to the same simulated synaptic input. Figure 8indicates that although each cell again responded consistently with a near-identical spike pattern for repeated presentations of the same stimulus, there were clear response differences between different cells. These differences included overall lower or higher spike rates in the response as well as different patterns of spiking. In particular, the amount of burstiness during periods of spiking was quite variable such that cells with overall low spike rates could show extremely rapid spiking at some times (Fig. 8C). Similar differences in spike patterns were observed in a previous modeling study (De Schutter and Bower, 1994a), when either the density of specific ion channels or the relative somatic or dendritic size of the cell was changed. These findings indicate that intrinsic conductances make an important contribution to spike timing. Because intrinsic currents are activated in a deterministic pattern by a repeated identical stimulus, the precision of spike timing in relation to the stimulus can be maintained. Because the dynamics of multiple interacting intrinsic currents is complex, the spike timing resulting from applied synaptic conductances may be a nontrivial function of fluctuations in the synaptic conductance. The diversity of observed spike responses teaches the important lesson that Purkinje cells may not be a homogeneous population of cells responding to inputs in an identical manner. Whether the input–output function of individual cells is the result of an adaptive learning process beyond the mere adjustment of synaptic strength remains to be studied. Recent experimental and modeling work in other cell types has clearly indicated that intrinsic currents can be dynamically regulated (Turrigiano et al., 1994, 1995; Liu et al., 1998).
DISCUSSION
Testing the validity of modeling predictions
In any modeling study, the usefulness and accuracy of simulation results are to a large degree dependent on the choice of modeling parameters (Bhalla and Bower, 1993). Because these parameters are never fully constrained by experimental data, the results of computer simulations aimed at replicating a biological system need to be carefully tested for their validity. On the other hand, modeling can clearly enhance our grasp of interacting dynamic variables, and only in a model can all state variables be accessed at the same time. The present study was designed to test modeling predictions about the integration of synaptic inputs in the presence of strong intrinsic currents, which we published previously (Jaeger et al., 1997). The best experimental method we could find to test our model predictions was to apply the synaptic conductances produced by the model to Purkinje cellsin vitro by way of dynamic current clamping. In addition to testing our model predictions, this technique allowed us to pursue the question of how precisely spike timing can be controlled via synaptic input in a cell with strong intrinsic conductances. This issue of precisely timed spikes as a channel of neural coding has recently attracted much attention (Softky and Koch, 1993; Mainen and Sejnowski, 1995; König et al., 1996; Yuste and Tank, 1996; Shadlen and Newsome, 1998). Our finding that the control of spiking may to a large degree depend on intrinsic cell properties indicates that the spike coding of input may be quite different for neurons with different active properties. The experimental methods introduced here provide a means by which both the input and the output of a cell can be assessed at the same time and may thus prove useful in studying the control of spiking in various cell types. For Purkinje cells, our findings indicate that both rate coding and precise spike time coding can coexist at the same time. The specific interaction of synaptic conductances and intrinsic currents has important implications for cerebellar cortical function, as discussed below.
Mechanisms underlying the synaptic control of Purkinje cell spiking
The influence of intrinsic conductances
The experimental results reported here support our modeling prediction that Purkinje cells generate an intrinsic inward plateau current that pushes the cell above the combined synaptic reversal potential of excitation and inhibition. This intrinsic inward current then forces the net synaptic current to be outward. As in the model, we found that this relationship holds for the whole range of realistic spike rates (Fig. 6). The intrinsic inward current is unstable and results in spontaneous bursting in the absence of synaptic input (Fig.2). Thus an ongoing balance of inhibitory and excitatory input is required to result in an irregular spike train typically seen in vivo. Our experiments cannot identify the types of ion channels responsible for the plateau depolarization in vitro. Previous studies show that the major candidates for a persistent inward current are given by a dendritic P-type Ca conductance and a somatic persistent Na conductance (Llinás and Sugimori, 1992). The model indicates that a large contribution of P-type Ca current is inevitable on the basis of the voltage dependence of this channel (Regan, 1991). In fact, in the model this current needs to be counterbalanced by Ca-dependent K currents [Jaeger et al. (1997), their Fig. 6] as well as by synaptic current. In the model, the interplay of dendritic Ca and K conductance activation leads to a complex time course of synaptically induced voltage transients. The role of intrinsic dendritic currents therefore goes much beyond a constant plateau depolarization. The detailed model predictions in this regard could not be tested in the present study because we cannot measure the time course of dendritic conductances. The variability in spike patterns between different cells recorded in vitro with the same input conductances (Fig. 8) suggests that the interaction of intrinsic and synaptic currents follows a different time course in different cells.
Persistent somatic sodium currents also provide a considerable inward current in the model and possibly in vitro. The model indicates, however, that these sodium currents are deactivated during spike afterhyperpolarization, and a depolarized dendrite is required to initiate the next spike (Jaeger et al., 1997).
The role of inhibition
Our experimental results show that a tonic baseline of inhibitory inputs needs to be present to allow the spike mode observed in vivo (Jaeger and Bower, 1994). This tonic input is required as otherwise intrinsic depolarizing currents push Purkinje cells into a burst mode that is not usually present in vivo. Our data show that the mean combined reversal potential of inhibition and excitation has to fall into a narrow range below spike threshold to allow natural spike patterns. This range in reversal potential is obtained when there are on the order of 20 times as many unitary excitatory as there are inhibitory inputs over a given time period.
A second important prediction of the model was also borne out in the experimental data, namely, that the time-varying level of inhibitory conductance has a direct control over the length of interspike intervals (Fig. 5). This finding is supported by the recent demonstration of a strong effect of the input from single inhibitory interneurons on Purkinje cell spike intervals (Häusser and Clark, 1997). These authors find that even the activation of interneurons in the outer molecular layer can have such an effect, and thus spike time control is not limited to basket cell inputs onto the soma. Basket cell input was not included in our modeling study, and further studies are needed to assess its influence in detail.
Spike rate coding and spike time coding
We find that Purkinje cells are very sensitive rate encoders in that small changes in the balance of excitation and inhibition lead to large changes in spike rate (Fig. 6). The larger changes in output than in input rate represent an amplification function. This amplification is carried by the activation of intrinsic plateau currents. Independent of spike rate, precise spike timing is found to be mediated via rapid fluctuations in synaptic conductances when the overall amplitude of synaptic conductance is high (Fig. 7). This spike precision in our data is limited to a narrow window of maximally ±3 msec, making it a distinctly different process from rate coding. Thus it seems possible that the overall spike rate and the timing of individual spikes may transmit different signals to the next level of neural processing. Unfortunately, we are still almost entirely ignorant about the decoding of Purkinje cell spike trains by the neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei to which they project.
Significance for cerebellar network function
Reconsidering inhibition
Our results indicate that constant inhibitory input is required in the control of Purkinje cell spiking and that inhibitory input makes an important contribution to spike timing as well as to spike rate control. In contrast, most recent efforts of modeling cerebellar cortical network function make no use of inhibitory inputs to Purkinje cells (Kawato and Gomi, 1992; Buonomano and Mauk, 1994; Schweighofer et al., 1996). This may be inspired by the view of neurons as summation devices, in which the presence of inhibitory input is equivalent to a reduced level of excitatory input. Our data indicate that Purkinje cells cannot operate in this manner. We conclude that pauses in Purkinje cell spiking are necessarily coupled to the presence of inhibitory input. (An exception would be a pause in spiking because of K conductance activation after dendritic calcium spiking.) Such pauses in spiking may be of great significance, because Purkinje cells are themselves inhibitory neurons. Thus, a pause in spiking would disinhibit neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei and lead to a positive output from the cerebellum. Pauses in spiking were found to be a dominant Purkinje cell response after somatosensory stimulation in anesthetized rats. This was true even along the center of an excited “beam” of parallel fibers (Bower and Woolston, 1983).
The indirect effects of parallel fiber inputs
Our experiments support the model prediction that parallel fiber (pf) inputs have a much more indirect effect on Purkinje cell behavior than is usually assumed. Since the first anatomical descriptions of the granule cell–parallel fiber system, most theories of cerebellar function (e.g., Marr, 1969; Fujita, 1982) have assumed that sets of activated pf synapses directly force Purkinje cells to cross spike threshold. Our model analysis, now supported by the experimental results presented here, suggests that pf inputs do not directly produce somatic action potentials. Instead, continuous pf input contributes one-half of a partial voltage-clamp mechanism, which stabilizes Vm at the combined reversal potential of inhibition and excitation. The trajectory of the combined reversal potential and the total conductance amplitude in turn regulate the activation of large intrinsic voltage-dependent conductances. The activation of intrinsic inward currents then depolarizes the cell and leads to the initiation of somatic spikes.
What do the parallel fibers do?
Viewing parallel fiber synapses as modulating the state of dendritic conductances suggests a major change in how to think about cerebellar cortex (Bower, 1997a). The data presented here suggest that parallel fiber input acts in the context of counterbalancing inhibition. In this way parallel fibers can be seen as playing a more modulatory role, as we originally proposed (Bower and Woolston, 1983). We do, however, believe that the granule cell synapses associated with the ascending granule cell axon can produce a direct excitatory drive on Purkinje cells (Bower and Woolston, 1983; Jaeger and Bower, 1994;Gundappa-Sulur et al., 1999). Recent cerebellar circuit simulations suggest that in addition to providing multiple and synchronous excitatory synaptic inputs, synapses of the ascending axon may activate too quickly after mossy fiber inputs to be counterbalanced by inhibition via local interneurons (F. Santamaria and J. M. Bower, unpublished observations). An elegant experimental demonstration of the predominant activation of Purkinje cells via ascending granule cell axons was recently seen with voltage-sensitive dye imaging in a guinea pig whole-brain preparation (Cohen and Yarom, 1998). In these experiments, electrical stimulation of a patch of granule cells resulted in the activation of overlying Purkinje cells but not along the extended beam of activated parallel fibers.
The challenge of understanding cerebellar cortical computation
The specialized architecture of cerebellar cortex suggests that a particular computational algorithm may take place in this structure (Bloedel, 1992; Bower, 1997b). The strong interaction of synaptic input and intrinsic conductances that we found in our model is supported by the present experimental findings. Because this interaction changes the amplitude and time course of Purkinje cell responses to synaptic input, active properties likely play an important role in the implementation of the cerebellar computational algorithm. One key challenge in further elucidating cerebellar computation consists of determining the control of spiking in other cerebellar cell types and in the combined network. We expect that realistic network modeling in conjunction with experimental testing will be the method of choice in tracing the complex dynamics of cerebellar computation.
Footnotes
This work was supported by a Sloan fellowship to D.J., by a grant from the Human Frontier Science Program to J.M.B., and by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH57256-01 to D.J. We thank Erik De Schutter for helpful comments and simulation scripts.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Dieter Jaeger, Department of Biology, 1510 Clifton Road, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322.