Abstract
Misalignment of the ocular axes induces double vision and rivalry. To prevent these unpleasant sensations, most subjects fixate preferentially with one eye and suppress entirely the deviating eye or else suppress portions of the visual field of either eye. To explore the mechanism of visual suppression, a divergent strabismus (exotropia) was induced in six normal, adult Macaca fascicularis by disinserting the medial rectus muscles. After 4–8 weeks, each animal was chaired to measure its exotropia and to determine its ocular fixation preference. Five of the monkeys developed a clearly dominant eye. It was injected with [3H]proline. Alternate sections from flat-mounts of striate cortex were then processed either for autoradiography to label the ocular dominance columns or for cytochrome oxidase (CO) to assess local metabolic activity. Two CO patterns were seen, often in the same cortex. The first consisted of thin dark columns alternating with wide pale columns. This pattern arose from reduced CO activity in the suppressed eye’s monocular core zones and both eyes’ binocular border strips. The second pattern consisted of thin pale bands from reduced metabolic activity in both eyes’ border strips. The thin dark–wide pale CO pattern was more widespread in the three animals with a strong fixation preference. The dark CO columns usually fit in register with the ocular dominance columns of the fixating eye, suggesting that perception was suppressed in the deviating eye. In most animals, however, the correlation switched in peripheral cortex contralateral to the deviating eye, implying local suppression of the fixating eye’s temporal retina (beyond 10°), as reported in humans with divergent strabismus. In the two animals with a weak fixation preference, pale border strips were found within the central visual field representation in both hemispheres. This CO pattern was consistent with alternating visual suppression. These experiments provide the first anatomical evidence for changes in cortical metabolism that can be correlated with suppression scotomas in subjects with strabismus.
- strabismus
- scotoma
- suppression
- cytochrome oxidase
- ocular dominance column
- visual cortex
- exotropia
- border strip
- core zone
- stereopsis
- binocular
- diplopia
Understanding the phenomenon of visual suppression is one of the keys to solving the vexing problem of strabismus. Children who acquire strabismus rarely complain of diplopia (double vision), because they quickly adapt to misalignment of the ocular axes by suppressing vision from the deviating eye (von Noorden, 1996). Thus, visual suppression plays a critical role in the advent of strabismus by eliminating the drive to fuse disparate images. In addition, suppression often leads to amblyopia, if it becomes constant in one eye, rather than alternating between the eyes.
The mechanism of visual suppression in strabismus is unknown. It is not even clear whether visual suppression originates in primary visual cortex (striate cortex, V1), in extrastriate cortex, or at a subcortical level. Since the original report of Hubel and Wiesel (1965), numerous investigators have confirmed that strabismus reduces the number of cells in striate cortex that can be driven by both eyes. More recently, electrophysiological studies have uncovered the existence of inhibitory binocular interactions in animals raised with strabismus (Freeman and Tsumoto, 1983; Crewther and Crewther, 1993;Chino et al., 1994; Sengpiel and Blakemore, 1994; Smith et al., 1997). In these animals, the response of neurons to an optimal stimulus in one eye was suppressed by stimulation of the other eye. Such residual inhibitory interactions may contribute to strabismic suppression.Sengpiel and colleagues (1994) have suggested explicitly that strabismic suppression depends on inhibitory interactions between cells in neighboring ocular dominance columns.
Few studies have used anatomical methods to probe how strabismus alters the visual cortex. Löwel (1994) has reported that strabismus increases the width of ocular dominance columns in kittens, presumably by reducing the correlation of activity between eyes (Goodhill, 1993). In addition, it has been shown by intracortical tracer injections in strabismic animals that intrinsic tangential clustered connections are lost between ocular dominance columns serving opposite eyes (Löwel and Singer, 1992; Tychsen and Burkhalter, 1995). This loss of connections presumably reflects the breakdown of cortical binocularity that occurs from strabismus. It does not, however, shed any light on the problem of strabismic suppression. If anything, a loss of projections between ocular dominance columns serving the left eye and the right eye would reduce the substrate for interocular suppression, unless only facilitatory connections were attenuated.
The main objective of this study was to explore the phenomenon of strabismic suppression using the mitochondrial enzyme cytochrome oxidase (CO) as a functional anatomical label. CO levels fluctuate dynamically with neuronal activity (Wong-Riley, 1994; DeYoe et al., 1995), making it an ideal label for this purpose. Our interest in strabismic suppression was kindled by the discovery that macaque striate cortex is subdivided into monocular core zones and binocular border strips (Horton, 1984; Horton and Hocking, 1998a). In normal animals, these compartments contain equal CO activity in layer IVc (Fig. 1). We reasoned that strabismus, by disrupting binocular function, might cause greater loss of CO in border strips than in core zones. We also predicted that animals with a strong fixation preference might lose CO activity in the core zones of their deviated eye, as a result of suppression. Our goal was to compare the fixation behavior of each animal with the pattern of CO activity in its visual cortex.
To avoid diplopia and confusion, many subjects with strabismus appear to suppress information emanating from only part of the retina, instead of turning off the eye altogether. This phenomenon gives rise to local suppression scotomas, revealed when the visual fields are plotted dichoptically (Graefe, 1896). By unfolding striate cortex and stripping it from the white matter, one can prepare flattened sections containing the representation of the contralateral visual hemifield in a single piece of tissue (Horton and Hocking, 1996c). We hoped that by examining such flat-mounted sections from each hemisphere, we might find anatomical evidence for local suppression scotomas by correlating each animal’s predominant fixation pattern with (1) the pattern of CO activity in its cortex, (2) the ocular dominance columns, and (3) the cortical map of the visual field.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experimental animals. We used six normal Macaca fascicularis imported from Mauritius in 1995. Their ages were unknown because they were feral animals. All were male, fully adult, weighing 5–6 kg, and in prime condition. Before use, each animal received a complete eye examination to exclude any preexisting disease. The pupil responses, eye movements, ocular alignment, ocular media, and fundus were normal in every case. All procedures used in these experiments were approved by the Committee on Animal Research at University of California, San Francisco.
Strabismus can be induced experimentally at any age. We chose to begin this avenue of research on strabismic suppression by testing adult animals. Our ultimate goal is to understand the phenomenon of suppression in children. However, before investing the effort required to raise infant monkeys with strabismus, it seemed best to see whether promising results could be obtained first in strabismic adults. In Discussion we consider this issue further and compare the mechanisms of suppression in adults and children.
We induced strabismus by tenotomy of the medial rectus muscle in both eyes. The procedure was performed under general anesthesia with ketamine HCl (15 mg/kg, i.m.) and topical proparacaine HCl. After the muscle was released, the insertion site was examined carefully to ensure that the tendon was severed completely. Afterward, topical antibiotic ointment was applied liberally to the eyes. The animals were checked every day after the operation to verify rapid healing and to observe ocular alignment.
Once each animal had developed a stable, divergent strabismus, it was fasted for 12 hr and placed in a restraining chair to measure ocular alignment in primary gaze. Two methods were used to gauge the magnitude of the exotropia. The first relied on decentration of the corneal light reflex in one eye (Hirschberg test), estimated while the monkey fixated a small, handheld point light source held 60 cm away. Grapes were used to attract the animal’s attention and to win its cooperation. Each millimeter of light reflex decentration was equivalent to 14° of ocular deviation (Quick and Boothe, 1989). For the second method, we determined the magnitude of a base-in prism required to center the corneal light reflex in each eye (Krimsky test). The Hirshberg and Krimsky tests were done by several examiners to arrive at a consensus regarding the magnitude of each animal’s primary exodeviation. For documentation, photographs were also taken at a distance of 60 cm with a ring-flash mounted on the end of a 100 mm macro lens.
Fixation preference was judged independently by each author, as well as by two pediatric ophthalmologists. Assessments were made by watching each animal’s spontaneous behavior and by testing its fixation preference while reaching for food treats. Frequent observations of unrestrained animals in their home cages were recorded by each examiner over several weeks. Examiners were asked to decide whether the animal had a fixation preference for one eye. If so, the examiner rated the preference as strong or weak. An animal with a strong fixation pattern spent >80% of the time foveating with its preferred eye. An animal with a weak fixation pattern spent only 60–80% of the time looking with its preferred eye. Estimates were verified by reviewing videotapes of the animals. Observational assessment of fixation preference is simple and quite accurate when done by experienced strabismologists (Choi and Kushner, 1998).
Between 4–8 weeks after inducing strabismus, we labeled the ocular dominance columns in each monkey by [3H]proline transneuronal autoradiography. The tracer injection was always made into the eye that the monkey used preferentially for fixation. It was prepared by reconstituting 2 mCi ofl-[2,3,4,5-3H]proline (specific activity, 102–106 Ci/mmol; Amersham, Arlington Heights, IL) in 20 μl of sterile balanced salt solution. The pupil was dilated pharmacologically, and the eye was massaged gently to lower the intraocular pressure below 12 mmHg. The tracer was then injected through the pars plana into the midvitreous of the eye under anesthesia with ketamine HCl (10 mg/kg i.m.) and topical proparacaine HCl. Immediately afterward, the fundus was checked with an indirect ophthalmoscope for any sign of injury. The survival time for transport of the label was 7–9 d. Just before perfusion, the eye was examined again with an indirect ophthalmoscope. In each animal, the eye appeared unscathed by injection of the radioisotope.
Histological procedures. After receiving a lethal injection of sodium pentobarbital into the peritoneal cavity, each monkey was perfused through the left ventricle with 1 l of normal saline followed by 1 l of 2% paraformaldehyde in 0.1m phosphate buffer. Flat-mounts of striate cortex were prepared from each hemisphere (Horton and Hocking, 1996c), cut with a freezing microtome, mounted on slides, and air-dried. Alternating sections were reacted for CO (Wong-Riley, 1979) or dipped into NTB-2 emulsion (Eastman Kodak, Rochester, NY) for autoradiography. The autoradiographs were exposed for 10 weeks and developed with D19 developer (Kodak) (Wiesel et al., 1974). In three animals the lateral geniculate bodies were sectioned and reacted for CO.
Data Analysis. Cortical CO flat mounts were imaged at 600 dots per inch on an Agfa (Mortsel, Belgium) Arcus II flatbed scanner fitted with a transparency adapter. To compare regions at higher power, CO sections and autoradiographs were photographed through an Olympus (Tokyo, Japan) SZH10 microscope using Technical Pan film (Kodak). Negatives were processed with D19 developer and scanned into the computer using a Microtek (Redondo Beach, CA) Scanmaker 35t scanner. Images were imported into Photoshop 5.0 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA) to prepare illustrations. Measurements were made using Scion Image PC (Scion Corp., Frederick, MD).
In a previous paper, we used a series of neutral density filters to calibrate the optical density of each CO section (Horton and Hocking, 1998a). This approach allowed us to compare the absolute density of CO reaction product between sections and between animals. In this present study, the CO patterns were often quite subtle. To improve their visibility, we found it necessary to adjust the brightness and contrast of each section. For this reason, the images in this paper were not calibrated to optical density standards.
RESULTS
Induction of strabismus
The day after surgery, all the monkeys had a huge divergent strabismus measuring ∼60°. In each case the exotropia showed marked spontaneous improvement in the first 2 weeks after surgery (Sireteanu et al., 1993). The mechanism was unclear, but the animals may have learned to reduce lateral rectus muscle tone in each eye. In addition, postmortem examination showed some adhesion of Tenon’s capsule and the intermuscular septum at the site of the medial rectus tendon insertion. Although the muscle itself did not reinsert, it may have exerted some tension through these capsular attachments.
After the first few weeks, reduction of the exotropia became more gradual. Some monkeys improved more than others, with final deviations ranging between 8 and 25°. Although convergent strabismus is more prevalent clinically, we deliberately made all our monkeys exotropic, because we were concerned that esotropic monkeys with small deviations might fuse near targets. With exotropia, even if a monkey had only a small deviation, we were sure that its eyes were misaligned all the time. None of our exotropic monkeys appeared able to use accommodative convergence to fuse intermittently.
In five of six monkeys, a fixation preference emerged within a few days of surgery. Because the monkeys could not fully adduct their eyes, they usually made a slight head turn to the opposite side to fixate with each eye. To switch fixation, the monkeys bobbed their heads back and forth. This behavior became less striking as the exotropia diminished and the ocular motility improved. Even when subtle, it made it easy to see when the animals switched fixation. In one animal, observers did not agree on the eye fixation preference. It did not seem worthwhile to study this animal, given that we were unsure of its eye preference. Therefore, it was donated to another laboratory. The remaining five animals achieved a stable exotropia by 4–8 weeks after surgery, and observers agreed unanimously on their ocular fixation preference. Figure 2 shows each animal before [3H]proline injection into the preferred eye, ∼10 d before perfusion. Intraocular injection of [3H]proline did not affect the magnitude of the exotropia or the eye fixation preference in any monkey.
Monkey 1 (right exotropia, 8°)
This animal developed the smallest exodeviation, measuring only 8°, but all observers concurred that it had a strong preference for the left eye, fixating with it virtually all the time. [3H]Proline was injected into the left eye 4 weeks after eye muscle surgery, followed by perfusion 9 d later. Figure 3, A andC, shows single CO-stained tangential sections cut through flat-mounts of the left and right striate cortex. The sections pass mostly through layer IVc, revealing the CO pattern in that layer over a wide expanse of cortex, representing most of the visual field. In animals with normal ocular alignment and visual acuity, no pattern of CO activity is seen in layer IVc (Horton, 1984). In this strabismic animal, a remarkable pattern of alternating dark and light CO columns was visible everywhere, following the general layout of the ocular dominance columns (LeVay et al., 1985). The columns were quite faint, resembling those seen after eyelid suture, rather than after enucleation (Horton and Hocking, 1998a).
Eyelid suture produces a low-contrast CO pattern of thin dark columns alternating with wide pale columns in layer IVc (Horton, 1984; Hendry and Jones, 1986; Crawford et al., 1989; Trusk et al., 1990; Tigges et al., 1992). This pattern results from loss of CO activity in the sutured eye’s monocular core zones and in both eyes’ binocular border strips (Horton and Hocking, 1998a). We wondered whether strabismus might have induced the same effect in striate cortex. On close inspection of the opercular cortex in Figure 3, the dark columns looked slightly thinner than the pale columns. This impression was confirmed by measuring the width of 50 light columns and 50 dark columns, selected arbitrarily from each opercular cortex. The average widths were 297 ± 51.3 (SD) μm for the dark columns and 408 ± 73.7 μm for the light columns. Assuming that the dark columns represented the core zones of the fixating eye, a pair of border strips measured 55.5 μm, yielding for a single wide pale column 55.5 + 297 + 55.5 = 408 μm.
To interpret the CO pattern in Monkey 1, it was vital to know its relationship to the ocular dominance columns. Figure 3, Band D, shows autoradiographs prepared from adjacent sections superficial to the CO sections illustrated in Figure 3, Aand C. Ocular dominance columns were visible throughout the section in layer IVc. On the operculum the ocular dominance columns were about equal in width. In the periphery, beyond the blind spot representation, the columns serving the ipsilateral eye became shrunken and fragmented, a normal feature described by LeVay et al. (1985).
To compare CO patterns with autoradiographs, one must examine local regions at high power, so that blood vessel patterns can be matched precisely to ensure accurate alignment of adjacent sections. Figure4A shows the CO pattern in layer IVc from the left operculum of Monkey 1, in a region between 5 and 8°, near the lower vertical meridian. Figure 4Bshows the corresponding region from the adjacent autoradiograph. The thin dark CO columns match the proline-labeled ocular dominance columns, although they are slightly thinner, because they represent core zones only. In this region of cortex, we concluded that CO activity was richer in the core zones of the fixating eye.
This metabolic bias in layer IVc favoring the fixating eye was perpetuated through the full thickness of the cortex, implying that visual processing was affected in both afferent and efferent layers. Figure 4C shows the CO pattern in layer IVa. Bands of label were present, with each band running down the middle of an ocular dominance column. In normal animals, bands are never found in layer IVa (Horton, 1984). Instead, one sees a continuous “honeycomb” of CO activity. Bands emerged in Monkey 1 because strabismus reduced metabolic activity within the border strips, sparing activity in the core zones. In addition, CO staining appeared slightly darker in the core zones of the fixating eye (matching the dark core zones in layer IVc), giving rise to a pattern of alternating dark and light core zones in layer IVa, separated by pale gaps representing the border strips.
In layers II and III, the CO patches appeared almost to merge as they ran down the middle of the ocular dominance columns (Fig.4D). This tendency for patches to form “pearls on a string” is seen in normal animals, but it was exaggerated in all our strabismic monkeys. Again, we believe that loss of CO activity in border strips accentuated each row of CO patches by silhouetting it against a paler CO background. Strabismus also caused the rows of patches in register with the core zones of the fixating left eye to appear slightly darker than the rows serving the deviating right eye. This effect was subtle but definite and even visible in layers V and VI. Therefore, metabolic hegemony of the fixating eye was evident in all cortical layers (except layer I). It is worth emphasizing that although strabismus mimicked the pattern produced by eyelid suture in layer IVc, outside layer IVc it yielded a lower-contrast pattern of light and dark rows of patches. Curiously, eyelid suture in adult monkeys induces a high-contrast pattern outside layer IVc, rivaling that seen after enucleation (Horton, 1984; Horton and Hocking, 1998a).
In Figure 4, CO patterns are compared with ocular dominance columns in only a small region of cortex. To confirm the assignment of the dark CO columns to the ocular dominance columns of the fixating left eye, we compared CO sections and autoradiographs throughout each striate cortex. Everywhere, the dark CO columns fit in register with the labeled ocular dominance columns. Figure5 shows examples of three other regions matched at high power. Note that in the periphery of the right cortex (Fig. 5E), the CO pattern appeared as wide dark columns alternating with thin pale columns. This reversal occurred because, beyond 15° (Fig. 5F), the ocular dominance columns serving the ipsilateral eye become naturally shrunken, whereas those serving the contralateral eye become expanded (LeVay et al., 1985). As a result, in the right peripheral cortex, the usual pattern of thin dark–wide pale columns was replaced by wide dark–thin pale columns (Fig. 5E). Parenthetically, it is worth noting that in peripheral cortex, the existence of well organized border strips is uncertain. Neurons with tuned disparity selectivity are more common in the central visual field representation, where binocular input is closely balanced (Poggio, 1995). In peripheral cortex, border strips may be rudimentary, because disparity-tuned cells are sparse, and ocular input is biased strongly in favor of the contralateral eye. If so, core zones may become nearly equivalent to ocular dominance columns, and CO patterns from strabismus may correspond almost exactly to the actual boundaries of ocular dominance columns.
To summarize the findings in Monkey 1, the fixating left eye had stronger metabolic activity throughout the left and right striate cortex. Even in the monocular crescent of the left cortex, where the deviating right eye did not have to compete with the fixating left eye, CO staining appeared pale. Metabolic activity in the deviating eye’s ocular dominance columns was reduced globally, and the left eye was dominant everywhere in the cortex (and hence in the visual field).
Monkey 2 (left exotropia, 25°)
This animal had the largest deviation, a left exotropia measuring 25°. All observers agreed that it had a strong right eye fixation preference. The right eye was injected with [3H]proline 6 weeks after induction of strabismus. As in Monkey 1, there were faint columns of CO activity everywhere in layer IVc of the left (Fig.6A) and the right striate cortex (Fig. 7A). On the opercula, the dark columns were slightly thinner than the pale columns, consistent with loss of CO activity in border strips, and in the core zones of the deviating left eye. To check the assignment of the dark CO columns, they were compared at high power with adjacent autoradiographs. In the left cortex, the dark columns corresponded to the labeled ocular dominance columns of the fixating right eye, both on the operculum and in the periphery (Fig. 6B–E). In the right cortex, the same result was found on the operculum (Fig.7D,E). However, in peripheral cortex, the opposite was seen: the pale CO columns matched the labeled ocular dominance columns of the right eye (Fig. 7B,C). The most straightforward explanation for this result would be that the deviating left eye was suppressed everywhere, except in the peripheral right visual cortex (representing the left peripheral visual field). In this region it enjoyed perceptual dominance, and the right eye was locally suppressed. Note that the switchover occurred at an eccentricity of 10–12° in the left visual cortex (Fig. 7A), which did not equal the magnitude of the divergent strabismus (∼25°). This means that the peripheral temporal retina of the fixating right eye was actively suppressed in favor of the nasal retina of the deviating left eye.
Monkey 3 (left exotropia, 20°)
This animal also had a left exotropia, but it was slightly smaller than the previous animal’s exotropia. His fixation preference for the right eye was strong. [3H]Proline was injected into the right eye 4 weeks after the medial rectus tenotomies. On the operculum of the left visual cortex, there were alternating thin dark and wide pale CO columns in layer IVc, but they were quite faint (Fig. 8A). At high power, it was verified that the thin dark CO columns matched the ocular dominance columns of the fixating right eye (Fig. 8, compareB, C). In the periphery of the left cortex, the CO columns were higher in contrast. Again, the dark CO columns coincided with the ocular dominance columns of the dominant right eye (Fig. 8, compare D, E).
In the right visual cortex, from the representation of the fovea to 10°, the pattern of CO staining in layer IVc was extremely subtle (Fig. 9A). It appeared homogeneous, except for a faint pattern of parallel pale strips, ∼50–70 μm wide, spaced about every half-millimeter. Comparison with adjacent autoradiographs showed that the pale strips ran right along the borders between ocular dominance columns (Fig. 9, compareD, E). We concluded that the pale strips corresponded to pairs of border strips. Their metabolic activity was reduced because binocularity was disrupted by strabismus. Only pale border strips were seen in the right operculum, because metabolic activity was equal in the core zones serving each eye.
In the peripheral right cortex, a clear pattern of dark and light CO columns emerged from 10° to the monocular crescent representation (Fig. 9A). The more peripheral in the cortex, the greater the contrast of the columns. At high power, we found that the pale CO columns matched the ocular dominance columns of the injected, dominant right eye (Fig. 9, compare B, C). To summarize: in this monkey the fixating right eye was dominant throughout the left visual cortex; both eyes held equal sway on the right operculum; and the deviating left eye was supreme in the right peripheral cortex.
Monkey 4 (right exotropia, 10°)
This monkey preferred to fixate with the left eye, but he alternated fixation frequently. All five observers rated his fixation preference as weak. A [3H]proline injection was made into the left eye 4 weeks after induction of strabismus. Figure 10 shows the pattern of CO activity in the left visual cortex. On the operculum, pale border strips were visible throughout layer IVc. In the periphery, beginning at ∼10°, alternating dark and pale columns emerged. Moving toward the monocular crescent representation, their contrast increased steadily. The pale CO columns matched the ocular dominance columns labeled by tracer from the preferred left eye.
In the right visual cortex the same pattern of CO activity was seen, but in reverse (Fig. 11). On the operculum, pale border strips were present. Beginning at 10°, a pattern of alternating dark and light CO columns began to appear. On this side, it was the dark CO columns that matched the labeled ocular dominance columns of the left eye. In this animal, therefore, the eyes had equal metabolic influence within the central field representation. Presumably the CO method was not sensitive enough to show a subtle bias in favor of the left eye, which one might have expected from the animal’s weak fixation preference. The only sign of strabismus was the loss of CO activity within border strips. In the peripheral field representation, CO activity on each side was richer in ocular dominance columns serving the contralateral eye. This pattern of staining was consistent with suppression of the temporal retina in each eye, which, of course, would be expected in divergent strabismus.
Monkey 5 (right exotropia, 10°)
The findings in this monkey are not illustrated, because they resembled closely those described in Monkey 4. The animal had a right exotropia of similar magnitude and a weak fixation preference for the left eye. The [3H]proline injection was made into the left eye 8 weeks after eye muscle surgery. On each operculum, a CO pattern of pale border strips was seen. In the periphery of each cortex, dark and pale CO columns were present. The dark columns coincided with the ocular dominance columns of the contralateral eye on each side. Therefore, as illustrated for Monkey 4, the temporal retina appeared to be suppressed in each eye of this animal.
DISCUSSION
The main finding in this study was that induction of strabismus produced an abnormal pattern of metabolic activity in primary visual cortex of macaques. In each animal, for example, we could identify regions of cortex where CO activity was greater in the dominant, fixating eye’s ocular dominance columns. In these regions, we infer that neurons were less active within ocular dominance columns serving the deviating eye. The most logical interpretation would be that cortical activity driven by the deviating eye was suppressed to avoid diplopia and confusion. In the paragraphs below we outline the evidence in favor of this notion.
The first issue is whether suppression even occurs in adult subjects. Classically, it is taught that adults with acquired ocular misalignment can learn to ignore a troublesome second image but never exhibit true strabismic suppression (von Noorden, 1996, page 215). There is evidence from evoked potentials, however, for true suppression of cortical activity in adults with acquired strabismus (Wright et al., 1990). Some investigators have suggested that strabismic suppression may harness the same innate cortical mechanism that prevents diplopia under normal conditions of binocular rivalry (Franceschetti and Burian, 1971; Harrad et al., 1996), although this idea is controversial (Smith et al., 1985). If correct, the difference between suppression in children and adults may be simply that children can invoke suppression with greater facility. Until the mechanisms of strabismic suppression are better understood in both children and adults, it is probably best to keep an open mind on these questions. It is certainly worth comparing cortical CO patterns induced by strabismus in adults and children. Examination of young macaques raised with strabismus will be our next step.
Strabismus induced two different patterns of CO activity in layer IVc of opercular cortex. The first pattern consisted of slender pale bands, spaced every half-millimeter (Fig.12A). By alignment with ocular dominance columns labeled by [3H]proline in adjacent sections, we showed that these pale bands represented pairs of border strips straddling the boundaries between ocular dominance columns. Strabismus caused selective loss of CO activity in the border strips because they are rich in binocular cells, which are generally excited maximally when driven by both eyes at a preferred disparity. A pattern of pale border strips, similar to that seen in strabismus, has also been found in newborn macaques (Horton and Hocking, 1996a). It probably occurs because ocular alignment and binocular function are immature in newborns (Chino et al., 1997). The pale border strips fade a few weeks after birth, around the time when stereopsis begins to emerge (O’Dell and Boothe, 1997). Weak CO staining along border strips has also been observed in a single adult macaque with mild amblyopia (Horton et al., 1997). This animal may have had a small angle strabismus that reduced CO activity within border strips.
The second CO pattern in layer IVc consisted of thin dark columns alternating with wide pale columns (Fig. 12B). It was generated by loss of CO activity in the border strips of both eyes and in the core zones of one eye. We believe that CO was lost in the core zones of the eye that was locally suppressed. The pattern induced by strabismus looked exactly like the pattern induced by monocular eyelid suture. From the point of view of CO activity, therefore, strabismic suppression had an effect tantamount to closure of one eye. It was, in metabolic terms, as if the brain achieved strabismic suppression by invoking a mechanism equivalent to local occlusion of one retina.
The abnormal CO patterns seen in the strabismic monkeys were faint, compared with the vivid columns seen after monocular enucleation, TTX injection, or retinal laser lesions (Horton, 1984; Wong-Riley and Carroll, 1984; Horton and Hocking, 1998a). These manipulations eliminate input from one eye, resulting in drastic reduction of CO activity within geniculate cell bodies and axon terminals, accounting for the high-contrast pattern seen in layer IVc. In three strabismic monkeys we examined CO staining in the lateral geniculate bodies. It was entirely normal. The CO patterns induced by strabismus, therefore, arose within the cortex. Although they were quite subtle (and difficult to illustrate), they were robust, in the sense that they were present everywhere in striate cortex of both hemispheres in all five animals.
Figure 13 summarizes the patterns of abnormal CO activity induced by strabismus in each monkey. It is important to remember that CO levels fluctuate over a time scale of days, not minutes or hours. At any given moment, the actual pattern of neuronal activity in any animal’s cortex might have looked quite different from the staining pattern depicted in Figure 13. The CO pattern represents only the prevailing pattern of neuronal activity over 24–48 hr and cannot capture momentary fluctuations that occur from switches in fixation or attention. Although it is valuable to know the predominant pattern of cortical activation, it would also be exciting to explore the phenomenon of strabismic suppression using techniques with better time resolution. One could use electrophysiological recordings (or optical imaging) in awake, behaving monkeys (Thiele et al., 1997) or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans. fMRI has poorer spatial resolution than CO, but given the findings in this study, it should be quite easy to map regional suppression scotomas in striate cortex.
The monkeys in this study all had divergent strabismus, but a range of CO patterns was seen. Each pattern made sense, in terms of the animal’s fixation behavior, which strengthens our hypothesis that it correlated with suppression in the visual field. Three monkeys had a strong fixation preference (Monkeys 1–3). Monkey 1 had darker CO staining in the fixating eye’s ocular dominance columns everywhere in both cortices, implying complete suppression of the exotropic eye. Monkey 2 also had darker CO staining in the fixating eye’s ocular dominance columns, except in the peripheral right cortex, where a switch took place in favor of the deviating left eye. This CO pattern signified perceptual dominance of the fixating right eye everywhere in the visual field, except in the left periphery. This is a common pattern of suppression in human exotropes (Melek et al., 1992). In Monkey 3, the CO pattern was similar, except that border strips were present on the right operculum. In this monkey, the temporal retina of the deviating left eye was entirely suppressed. The temporal retina of the fixating right eye was also suppressed, except for the temporal macula, where suppression alternated with the nasal macula of the left eye.
Monkeys 4 and 5 had weak fixation preferences. Not surprisingly, their cortical CO patterns revealed pale border strips in the central field representations bilaterally. Border strips may have appeared because their fixation preferences were too weak to produce a strong CO pattern favoring the dominant eye (i.e., thin dark–wide pale columns). Alternatively, the animals’ central suppression scotomas may have shifted back and forth between the two eyes, without an actual change in fixation. Our methods could not distinguish between these two possibilities. Outside the maculae, the temporal retinas of both animals were suppressed, as one would expect in exotropia.
Studies of the visual fields in subjects with strabismus have documented a confusing range of findings, depending on the technique used to map suppression scotomas (Jampolsky, 1955; Pratt-Johnson and MacDonald, 1976; Sireteanu and Fronius, 1981; Sireteanu, 1982; Cooper and Record, 1986; Joosse et al., 1990, 1997; Melek et al., 1992). Mapping of the visual fields in strabismus is tricky, because one must dissociate the eyes enough to plot suppression scotomas, but not too much, or else they vanish. The conflicting results from various studies have been blamed on methodological differences (Mehdorn, 1989). It is also likely, judging from the range of CO patterns seen in our monkeys, that suppression scotomas vary widely in size, depth, and location, depending on the degree of ocular misalignment and the strength of fixation preference displayed by the subject. In general, clinical studies agree that the temporal retina is suppressed in exotropia (von Noorden, 1996). This finding prevailed in our monkeys.
Two other reports have described CO patterns in strabismic monkeys.Tychsen and Burkhalter (1997) reported “spontaneous” ocular dominance columns in an exotropic M. mulatta raised with daily alternate occlusion of one eye. No second label was used, so the CO pattern was difficult to interpret. In a second animal, a M. nemestrina with naturally occurring esotropia, wheat germ agglutinin-horseradish peroxidase (WGA-HRP) was injected into one eye to label the ocular dominance columns. Unfortunately, this tracer produces an intense uveitis, factitiously reducing CO activity in the injected eye’s ocular dominance columns (Horton and Hocking, 1996b). Tychsen and Burkhalter reported that CO rich stripes corresponded to “nasal ODCs” (i.e., ocular dominance columns serving the contralateral eye) in both V1s, a result that cannot be attributed to damage from WGA-HRP injection. Their finding was consistent with the theory that functional dominance of the nasal retina contributes to esotropia (Tychsen and Lisberger, 1986) but was exactly opposite to the pattern of CO activity one would expect from regional suppression in convergent strabismus. Tychsen and Burkhalter (1997) did not illustrate both labels in the same region of cortex, so their experiment is difficult to assess, but it highlights the need for further studies of CO patterns in esotropia.
In a second report, Fenstemaker et al. (1997) examined CO staining in three monkeys with strabismic amblyopia. They found a pattern of alternating thin dark and wide pale columns, but paradoxically, the thin dark columns were thought to match the amblyopic eye’s ocular dominance columns. This assignment was made by stimulating the amblyopic eye for 2 hr, while the monkey was still anesthetized at the end of a recording session, and then immunostaining alternate sections for the transcription factor EGR-1 (Zif-268) (Chauduri et al., 1995). It is possible that 2 hr of visual stimulation were not enough to erase a preexisting EGR-1 bias in favor of the fixating eye’s ocular dominance columns. EGR-1 patterns can linger long after stimulus conditions change, mandating caution when using this approach to assign CO columns to one eye or the other (Horton and Hocking, 1998a). That is why we elected to use [3H]proline as a second label in our experiments. Recently, we have used EGR-1 successfully, under different stimulus conditions, to show that darker CO staining is associated with the fixating eye in most of striate cortex (Horton and Hocking, 1998b).
Induction of strabismus in adults creates binocular rivalry. Leopold and Logothetis (1996) have reported that ∼20% of neurons in V1 and V2 of normal, trained monkeys modulates responses according to which eye is perceiving during binocular rivalry. An even greater percentage of neurons modulates activity in downstream visual areas such as V4. To explore further the issues raised by our findings, the best approach would be to map the visual fields dichoptically in strabismic monkeys. One could then record from single neurons to correlate changes in firing rates with switches in fixation, suppression scotomas, and cortical patterns of CO activity. This strategy could provide direct evidence that the CO patterns we have described in this report arise from perceptual suppression.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Eye Institute Grant RO1 EY10217 and Core Grant EY02162, That Man May See, and Research to Prevent Blindness. We thank Dr. Douglas R. Fredrick and Dr. Creig S. Hoyt for their help in evaluating the animals’ ocular fixation preferences and Robin Troyer for assistance with animal care and surgery. We also thank the California Regional Primate Research Center (especially Dr. Celia Valverde, Jenny Short, and David Robb). The California Primate Center is supported by National Institutes of Health Base Grant RR00169. William T. Newsome, Arthur J. Jampolsky, Nélida B. Melek, and Anne K. Churchland provided valuable comments on this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jonathan C. Horton, Beckman Vision Center, 10 Kirkham Street, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94143-0730.