Abstract
Although inhibitors of glutamate transport prolong synaptic currents at many glutamate synapses, the cause of the current prolongation is unclear. Transport inhibitors may prolong synaptic currents by simply interfering with synaptic glutamate binding to transporters, by inhibiting substrate translocation, or by promoting accumulation of ambient glutamate, which may act cooperatively at receptors with synaptic glutamate. We show that reversal of the membrane potential of astrocytes surrounding the synapse prolongs synaptic currents but does not decrease the apparent affinity of transporters or significantly alter glutamate-dependent kinetics of macroscopic transporter currents in excised membrane patches. Positive membrane potentials do not affect binding of a nontransported glutamate analog, nor do positive membrane potentials alter the number of transporters available to bind analog. We also test the hypothesis that glutamate accumulation during uptake inhibition by transporter substrates is the direct cause of synaptic current prolongations. Transporter substrates elevate ambient glutamate near synapses by fostering reverse transport of endogenous glutamate. However, increases in ambient glutamate cannot account for the prolongations of synaptic currents, because a nonsubstrate transport inhibitor does not foster reverse uptake yet it prolongs synaptic currents. Moreover, exogenous glutamate does not mimic synaptic current prolongations induced by substrate inhibitors. These results provide strong support for a major role of substrate translocation in determining the time course of the glutamate concentration transient at excitatory synapses.
Understanding the factors that dictate the time course of glutamate's actions at CNS synapses may help elucidate glutamate's double-edged role of neurotransmitter and neurotoxin. Recent experiments have revealed that pharmacological blockade of glutamate transporters increases the peak glutamate concentration in the synaptic cleft (Tong and Jahr, 1994; Diamond and Jahr, 1997) or prolongs the glutamate transient, leading to a slowed EPSC decay at some synapses (Barbour et al., 1994; Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994, 1995; Takahashi et al., 1995; Otis et al., 1996; Kinney et al., 1997). Slowed EPSC decays have been observed at a number of glutamate synapses in the presence of transport inhibitors, but the precise transporter action responsible for truncating synaptic glutamate transients remains incompletely understood.
Besides blocking endogenous substrate (glutamate) translocation from the extracellular to intracellular compartment, uptake inhibitors have two other actions that could account for prolonged synaptic currents. Based on turnover rates in the 10/sec range (three orders of magnitude slower than acetylcholinesterase at the neuromuscular junction), it has been suggested that transporters are too slow to transport a significant amount of glutamate to the intracellular compartment on the millisecond time scale of synaptic transmission (Lester et al., 1994; Wadiche et al., 1995b; Wadiche and Kavanaugh, 1998). Therefore, it has been hypothesized that during individual synaptic events, glutamate transporters normally act as stationary buffers. Once bound, glutamate is more likely to dissociate than to be transported (Wadiche and Kavanaugh, 1998), but on dissociation from the transporter, levels of glutamate are too low to activate a significant number of receptors. By preventing interaction of synaptic glutamate with the substrate binding site on the transporter, exogenous substrates would prolong the postsynaptic actions of glutamate. We have shown that a nonpharmacological manipulation of glutamate transport, glial membrane depolarization, also prolongs synaptic currents (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994, 1995). The effect of reversing glial membrane polarity is quantitatively similar to the actions of pharmacological uptake inhibitors (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994). Although the buffering hypothesis is not mutually exclusive of the translocation hypothesis (Tong and Jahr, 1994; Diamond and Jahr, 1997), for the buffering hypothesis to entirely account for all observed effects on EPSCs, membrane depolarization should alter the glutamate binding properties of transporters, a prediction tested in the present work.
A second hypothesis to account for EPSC prolongations is that transport inhibitors promote ambient glutamate accumulation, which may itself prolong synaptic currents, analogous to cooperative interactions between exogenous transmitter and synaptic transmitter observed at the neuromuscular junction (Hartzell et al., 1975). Most glutamate uptake inhibitors are transporter substrates and are expected to promote extracellular glutamate accumulation through heteroexchange of intracellular glutamate during transporter cycling (Kanner and Bendahan, 1982). In addition, transport inhibition in some preparations causes accumulation of synaptic glutamate released from surrounding cells (Isaacson and Nicoll, 1993; Takahashi et al., 1995). We have shown that both glial depolarization and lithium substitution prolong synaptic currents at hippocampal microculture synapses, presumably by interfering with the voltage dependence and sodium dependence (Barbour et al., 1991) of glutamate transporter actions, but these treatments are also expected to promote increases in ambient glutamate (Szatkowski et al., 1990).
Here we use hippocampal microcultures and recombinant glutamate transporters to investigate the roles of glutamate binding, translocation, and heteroexchange in prolongations of synaptic currents. Using the recently identified ligand-gated ion channel properties of transporters, we show that membrane potential changes do not alter the binding properties of the transporter for glutamate and nontransported glutamate analogs. We also use the recently developed nonsubstrate transport inhibitordl-threo-β-benzyloxyaspartate (TBOA) and low concentrations of exogenous glutamate to explore the possibility that accumulated glutamate might explain the effects of these transporter manipulations. We conclude that the prolongation of synaptic currents observed in hippocampal microcultures is attributable to a direct effect of inhibiting glutamate translocation rather than to an effect of buffering by transporters or to an indirect effect of accumulated glutamate.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Microcultures. Hippocampal cells were prepared from 1–3 d postnatal Sprague Dawley rats and grown on microcultures as described previously (Mennerick et al., 1995). Briefly, slices of hippocampus 500–800 μm thick were incubated at 37°C with stirring in 1 mg/ml papain in oxygenated Leibovitz's L-15 medium. Single-cell suspensions were prepared by mechanical trituration in modified Eagle's medium supplemented with 5% horse serum, 5% fetal calf serum, 17 mmd-glucose, 400 μmglutamine, 50 U/ml penicillin, and 50 μg/ml streptomycin. Cells were plated at 75/mm2 onto plastic culture dishes coated with a layer of 0.15% agarose and collagen droplets as described previously and incubated at 37°C in 5% C02. Cultures were treated with cytosine arabinoside (5–10 μm) after 3 d in vitroand were used for experiments 4–14 d after plating.
Culture electrophysiology. Except where noted, the extracellular bath solution for whole-cell recording contained (in mm): 140 NaCl, 4.0 KCl, 3.0 CaCl2, 1.0 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, 0.05 d-APV. The standard whole-cell pipette solution for synaptic studies and for whole-cell glial recordings contained (in mm): 130 potassium gluconate, 4.0 NaCl, 0.5 CaCl2, 5.0 EGTA, and 10 HEPES, 2.0 MgATP2, and 0.5 GTP. For examination of neuronal responses to exogenous applications of drugs, the patch-pipette solution contained cesium methanesulphonate or cesium chloride in place of potassium gluconate, and nucleotide triphosphates were excluded. Other alterations of the extracellular and pipette solutions are as given in the text and figure legends. The pH of solutions was adjusted to 7.25. Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings of excitatory autaptic currents were performed from solitary neurons using 1.5–4 MΩ pipettes. Whole-cell access resistance was compensated 90–100% using the compensation circuitry of an Axopatch 1-D patch-clamp amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Neurons were stimulated with a 1.5 msec voltage pulse to 0 mV from a typical holding potential of −70 mV. Extracellular solutions were exchanged with a gravity-driven local perfusion system consisting of six separate lines connected to a common delivery port. Solutions were delivered at a rate of 240 μl/min except where noted.
Oocyte expression. Coding regions of the mouse glutamate transporter GLAST were subcloned into the pOX expression vector, which contains 5′ and 3′ untranslated regions from the Xenopusβ-globin gene to improve message stability (Shih et al., 1998). Transporter transcripts (15–50 ng) synthesized in vitro(Ambion mmessage mmachine) were injected into mature defolliculated oocytes. Oocytes were incubated in ND96 solution supplemented with 0.5 mm theophylline and 0.55 mg/ml pyruvate. The ND96 solution contained (in mm): 96 NaCl, 2 KCl, 1.8 CaCl2, 1.0 MgCl2, and 10 HEPES.
Oocyte electrophysiology. Oocytes were used for experiments 1–7 d after injection. Whole-cell transporter currents were recorded in two-electrode voltage-clamp mode using a Warner OC 725C amplifier in virtual ground mode. Voltage-recording and current-injection electrodes were filled with 3 m KCl and had resistances of ∼1 MΩ. Recordings were standardly performed in ND96 extracellular solution; alterations to this solution are as given in the text and figure legends. Solution exchanges on whole oocytes were achieved by placing the oocyte in a linear chamber (100 μl volume) and perfusing solutions from the common tip of a multibarrel pipette at a rate of 1.5 ml/min. For patch recordings, oocytes were mechanically stripped of vitelline membrane after shrinking in hypertonic saline. For excised patch recordings, the extracellular solution contained 96 NaSCN, 2.0 KCl, 3.0 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, and the patch-pipette solution contained (in mm): 100 KSCN, 10 KCl, 3 MgCl2, 10 EGTA, 10 HEPES (Wadiche and Kavanaugh, 1998). Calcium was omitted from solutions to prevent contribution of an endogenous oocyte Ca2+-activated anion conductance to records. Rapid applications were made to outside-out membrane patches using a piezo element (Burleigh). On-rate and off-rate constants from excised patch currents were measured by fitting single exponential functions to the raw data beginning at 10% of the final current amplitude. An agar-bridge ground was used for all experiments in which extracellular chloride concentration was manipulated. Errors in text and figures represent SEM.
RESULTS
Our previous results have shown that inhibition of glutamate transporters by various pharmacological and nonpharmacological treatments prolong synaptic currents (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994,1995). In most cases, we used transport inhibitors in the presence of cyclothiazide, which we have proposed unmasks an effect of glutamate transport inihibitors by sensitizing AMPA receptors to residual glutamate (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994). However, cyclothiazide may have other effects, such as presynaptic potentiation (Diamond and Jahr, 1995) or other undescribed effects. Therefore, we reexamined the effect of nonpharmacological inhibition of glutamate transporters on EPSCs, in the absence of cyclothiazide (CYZ). Figure1A shows that reversing the glial membrane potential, a nonpharmacological means of inhibiting transporters, prolongs some synaptic currents even in the absence of cyclothiazide. Consistent with our previous suggestion that transport inhibition under these conditions detectably alters the EPSC decay most in neurons with slow baseline EPSC decays, the average baseline 10–90% decay time (24 ± 4 msec) was correlated with the effect of depolarizing underlying glia (r = 0.94) (Fig. 1). Over all 11 neurons, the increase in 10–90% decay time was only 4 ± 3 msec, although in the three cells with the slowest baseline decay (43 ± 9 msec), the increase in decay was 17 ± 8 msec. These results are very similar to our previous results using pharmacological transporter substrates (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1995). Figure 1B shows an example in which reversal of the glial membrane potential had no detectable effect on the time course of an autaptic current. Also shown in Figure 1 are records of glial currents recorded at a membrane potential of −70 mV. These currents are largely transporter-associated and may represent the electrogenic uptake (translocation) of glutamate into astrocytes (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994). However, other ionic and capacitive currents not strictly linked to uptake are associated with transporter binding and activation (Wadiche et al., 1995a,b). Furthermore, it is not clear whether substrate translocation is responsible for the truncated EPSC when the astrocytes are clamped to −70 mV. Therefore, although these results confirm our previous observations that glial depolarization can prolong EPSCs in the absence of cyclothiazide (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1995), they beg the question of whether substrate translocation is responsible for EPSC prolongations.
Buffering by transporters cannot explain prolonged ESPCs
Displacement of glutamate binding with competitive pharmacological inhibitors of transport may be sufficient to account for synaptic effects of transport inhibitors (Tong and Jahr, 1994; Diamond and Jahr, 1997; Wadiche and Kavanaugh, 1998). Given that manipulation of glial membrane potential has qualitatively effects similar to those observed with pharmacological uptake inhibitors, we sought to determine whether membrane depolarization alters glutamate binding to transporters. Because glutamate transporters also exhibit an anion conductance that, like transport itself, is gated by ligand binding to the transporter (Wadiche et al., 1995a), electrical currents in response to substrate are composed of both the electrogenic transport current and the anion current. To determine whether the transporter current and channel mode of the transporter share similar ligand concentration dependence, we coexpressed the GLAST glutamate transporter and GABAA receptors (α1,β2,γ2L) inXenopus oocytes. GABA responses were used to determine the chloride reversal potential (ECl) in oocytes (−13 ± 1.8 mV, n = 3). Next, glutamate concentration–response curves were obtained with oocytes voltage clamped at ECl to obtain a pure transport current. To estimate EC50 values, the data for individual cells were fit to the Hill equation (Fig.2, legend). The EC50 for current generated at the chloride reversal potential did not differ significantly from the EC50 in the presence of NO3−, a condition in which anion flux should dominate the current (35.9 ± 2.9 μm at ECl,n = 6 and 53.6 ± 11 μm in NO3−,n = 7 oocytes; p > 0.1). In addition, when ECl was shifted to −40 mV by overnight dialysis in 0 Cl− saline, simlilar results were obtained (EC50 values 41 ± 7.2 μm, n = 6 vs 29 ± 4.7 μm, n = 5;p > 0.1). Average Hill coefficients for all conditions were similar and ranged from 1.2 to 1.4. Thus, the anion current and transport current show the same concentration dependence, and this is true at different voltages.
These results suggest that the ligand dependence of transport and anion flux through the channel mode of the transporter is similar, consistent with previous results (Wadiche et al., 1995a; Billups et al., 1996;Eliasof and Jahr, 1996). Therefore, we exploited the ligand-gated channel properties of the transporters to examine glutamate binding using techniques traditionally reserved for the examination of ligand-gated ion channels. First, we examined the effect of voltage on the apparent affinity of transporters for glutamate inXenopus oocytes expressing glutamate transporters. Because we previously found that GLAST is the predominant transporter expressed in microculture glial cells (Mennerick et al., 1998), we examined recombinant GLAST expressed in oocytes. As shown in Figure 2, the apparent affinity for glutamate to gate the anion conductance was similar at +60 mV compared with −80 mV. The EC50fit to the average responses was 15.6 μm at +60 mV versus 17.3 μm at −80 mV in the same oocytes. The concentration–response curves were similar whether chloride or nitrate was used as a permeant anion (Fig.2B), suggesting that manipulating the permeant anion does not influence the concentration–response relationship of the ligand-gated current.
We performed similar experiments on native transporters in microculture astrocytes. To ensure good voltage clamp, we chose isolated, singly nucleated astrocyte islands for study. AMPA receptors were blocked with 5 μm NBQX and 50 μm GYKI 52466. Barium (6 mm) replaced calcium and magnesium to diminish voltage-gated potassium conductances in the astrocytes. Transporter currents in native cells showed a somewhat higher apparent affinity for glutamate at positive potentials compared with negative potentials (Fig. 2C). From fits to average responses the EC50 was 10.0 μm at −90 mV (n = 3) and 1.7 μm at +60 mV (n = 5).
Results from Figure 2 suggest that total binding of glutamate by transporters is not affected by membrane potential. However, examination of steady-state concentration–response relationships suffer from several limitations (Colquhoun, 1998), especially when drawing conclusions related to the nonequilibrium conditions of a synaptic glutamate pulse. One limitation is that the equilibrium concentration–response curves can be influenced by steps in the transport cycle other than the microscopic binding dissociation constant, including anion channel opening and closing rates (Colquhoun, 1998). Therefore, it is possible that voltage may alter binding to the transporter, but opposite compensatory changes in other kinetic steps cause a lack of shift in EC50 values. We addressed this problem by examining the influence of voltage on presteady-state currents isolated with varied concentrations of the nonsubstrate transport inhibitor TBOA. TBOA binds the glutamate binding site (Seal and Amara, 1998; Shimamoto et al., 1998) but is not transported. By binding the transporter, nonsubstrate analogs block capacitive charge movements that represent early steps in the transport cycle (Parent et al., 1992; Wadiche et al., 1995b). Thus, we used TBOA blockade of these capacitive currents to assess the voltage dependence of analog binding in the absence of late kinetic steps, including anion channel gating, in the transport cycle.
Figure3A,Bshows that the TBOA concentration–response relationship is not altered by voltage. The absolute amplitude of instantaneous presteady-state current blocked by TBOA was used as a proportional measure of the number of transporters blocked by a given TBOA concentration. The instantaneous current showed the same concentration dependence whether TBOA was allowed to equilibrate with transporters at −80 or +60 mV (Fig. 3A,B). This result strongly suggests that total binding of the glutamate analog TBOA is not affected by membrane potential. In addition, the IC50 for TBOA blockade of steady-state glutamate-gated transport currents was not affected by reversing membrane polarity (Fig. 3C,D), further suggesting no voltage dependence on total binding of either glutamate or TBOA (Colquhoun, 1998).
One possible explanation for the data in Figure3A,B is that when preapplied at a negative potential, TBOA is locked onto the transporter by a transporter conformational change during depolarization and that TBOA would be unable to bind the transporter if applied during depolarization. To test whether TBOA can bind the transporter at positive membrane potentials, we performed the experiment shown in Figure 4. In one protocol (Fig.4A) 5 μm TBOA was applied at −70 mV before a voltage pulse to +60 mV. Subtraction of traces in the presence of TBOA from control traces in the absence of TBOA yielded symmetrical presteady-state currents (Fig. 4A) similar to those in Figure 3A. In a complementary protocol on the same oocyte, TBOA was applied 100 msec after the onset of the pulse to +60 mV. In this case, subtraction still yielded a negative-going presteady-state current on return to −70 mV that was similar in amplitude to the presteady-state current obtained in the standard protocol. In five oocytes the instantaneous current extrapolated from biexponential fits to data obtained from the protocol depicted in Figure 4A was 118 ± 21% of the current produced in the protocol in which TBOA was applied at +60 mV (p > 0.1, n = 5 oocytes; pairedt test). As expected from this result, there were no differences in the time contants or amplitudes of the exponential fits to presteady-state current decay (at −70 mV) in the two protocols (p > 0.1, n = 5; pairedt tests). These results strongly suggest that equilibrium binding of a nonsubstrate glutamate analog is not voltage dependent.
Measurement of TBOA-sensitive presteady-state currents and glutamate currents are still equilibrium measurements of total binding and therefore may not reflect the nonequilibrium conditions occurring with synaptic release of glutamate. It is possible that although equilibrium binding is not affected by voltage, on and off rates of glutamate binding are each affected by an equal and opposite factor. For ligand-gated receptors, the binding rate constant of ligand can be estimated by examining the concentration dependence of current onset rate in response to step applications of low to moderate concentrations of ligand, where ligand binding is rate limiting (Lavoie and Twyman, 1996; Jones et al., 1998; Wadiche and Kavanaugh, 1998). Similarly, the deactivation rate of currents on the rapid removal of ligand are typically inversely correlated with agonist affinity (Lester and Jahr, 1992; Jones et al., 1998). Thus, to examine the effect of membrane voltage on transporter affinity more directly, we examined the on and off rates of currents in response to step applications of low to moderate glutamate concentrations (Fig.5) in patches excised from oocytes, where expression levels were sufficient to observe macroscopic currents. We used the permeant anion thiocyanate as the major charge carrier for these experiments.
The on rate of current development in GLAST has been reported to be linear with glutamate concentrations through 60 μm(Wadiche and Kavanaugh, 1998). In agreement with this, we observed a nearly linear increase in the rate constant of current rise from 10 to 50 μm glutamate (Fig. 5B). Although in Figure5B a linear regression was fit through 50 μm glutamate, to avoid using glutamate concentrations possibly near saturation, we systematically analyzed the glutamate dependence of the rate of current development between 10 and 30 μm at −70 and +60 mV. There was a slight voltage dependence on the rate of current development at all glutamate concentrations (Fig. 5A), possibly reflecting voltage-dependent steps in transport or anion channel gating. However, the glutamate dependence of the on rate was not significantly altered by voltage (Fig. 5B) (p > 0.1; n = 8 patches at 10 μm and 4 patches at 30 μm). The rate obtained by linear regression between on rates at 10 μmglutamate (rate constants of 54.4 ± 5.8/sec at −70 mV and 34.3 ± 7.2/sec at +60 mV; n = 8 patches) and 30 μm glutamate (224.6 ± 35.7/sec at −70 mV and 174.4 ± 57/sec at +60 mV; n = 4 patches) was 9.5 × 106m/sec at −70 mV and 8.7 × 106m/sec at +60 mV. These estimates are similar to a recent estimate at negative potentials using the human GLAST homolog (Wadiche and Kavanaugh, 1998).
Excised patches exhibited inward rectification in steady-state responses (Fig. 5A). Although the inward rectification in patches could represent fewer available transporters at positive potentials, this possibility is rendered unlikely by the presteady-state measurements in oocytes (Figs. 3A, 4), which suggests transporters are available to bind TBOA at positive potentials. Therefore, inward rectification in patches likely represents the slowed turnover of transporters at positive potentials and/or inherent rectification of the conductance underlying the anion current.
Deactivation rate is correlated with agonist affinity for ligand-gated channels (Lester and Jahr, 1992; Jones et al., 1998). No influence of either glutamate concentration or voltage on deactivation of currents could be detected in excised patches (Fig. 5C). Average deactivation time constant across glutamate concentrations was 36.7 ± 5.3 msec at −70 mV and 45.8 ± 7.7 msec at +60 mV (n = 12 patches; p > 0.1). The above results suggest that glutamate binding rate is not reduced by depolarization and thus that simple buffering does not underlie the prolonged EPSCs when glia are depolarized. We conclude that prevention of buffering is not the major reason that glial depolarization prolongs EPSCs.
Accumulation of glutamate cannot explain prolonged EPSCs
We next addressed the hypothesis that glutamate accumulation may cause EPSC prolongations. Accumulation of endogenous agonist might be expected to prolong EPSC decays through a cooperative action of ambient and synaptic agonist (Hartzell et al., 1975). This effect is expected to be particularly strong under conditions in which synaptic transmitter lingers longer or has larger spatial spillover, precisely the conditions under which transporter inhibition has been shown to have its strongest effects on EPSC time course (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1995; Takahashi et al., 1995).
Most transport inhibitors described, including glial membrane depolarization, should promote extracellular glutamate accumulation (Kanner and Bendahan, 1982; Kanner and Marva, 1982; Szatkowski et al., 1990; Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994). In many preparations, glutamate may accumulate from surrounding synapses in the presence of transport inhibitors (Isaacson and Nicoll, 1993; Sarantis et al., 1993; Barbour et al., 1994; Maki et al., 1994). This problem is reduced in microcultures where a solitary neuron is examined. Although the importance of glutamate efflux through transporters is controversial (Szatkowski et al., 1990; Zerangue and Kavanaugh, 1996), microcultures are potentially susceptible to glutamate release via heteroexchange in the presence of exogenously applied substrates (Kanner and Bendahan, 1982) and to reverse uptake during depolarizations or manipulation of extracellular or intracellular ions (Szatkowski et al., 1990). Our strategy was to determine whether significant heteroexchange occurs in response to substrate application and whether a nonsubstrate inhibitor, which should not promote heteroexchange, also prolongs EPSCs. For these experiments we used the nonsubstrate inhibitor TBOA (Lebrun et al., 1997; Shimamoto et al., 1998). We compared TBOA with the parent compound threo-3-hydroxyaspartate (THA) andl-trans-pyrrolidine-2,4-dicarboxylate (PDC), both of which share similar potency to TBOA at the GLAST transporter but are transporter substrates (Arriza et al., 1994; Tanaka, 1994;Shimamoto et al., 1998).
To determine whether substrates induce significant efflux via heteroexchange, we used neuronal NMDA receptors in microcultures as a sensitive detector of local glutamate accumulation. Substrates and other drugs were applied to a microculture in a solution designed to potentiate NMDA receptor-mediated responses while inhibiting synaptic release of glutamate. The extracellular solution contained no added calcium or magnesium, 10 μm glycine, 1–5 μm NBQX, 25 μm bicuculline, and 250 nm tetrodotoxin. Under these conditions, the substrates THA and PDC consistently produced APV-sensitive inward currents in microculture neurons. This inward current could be attributable to accumulating glutamate released via heteroexchange. However, we also found that at high micromolar concentrations, these substrates produced currents in excised outside-out membrane patches raised above the cell layer, confirming previous suggestions that these transporter substrates directly activate NMDA receptors (Tong and Jahr, 1994). Because of this, we sought a way to determine whether neuronal whole-cell currents in response to substrate application are caused at least partly by heteroexchange of glutamate through transporters.
We reasoned that if accumulating endogenous glutamate explains at least part of the whole-cell NMDA receptor current, the constant perfusion typically used in our experiments might be washing away accumulating glutamate and that ceasing the perfusion should allow more accumulation. As predicted, the amplitude of the current usually increased in a PDC- or THA-containing static bath (19 of 29 cells) (Fig.6A,B), suggesting buildup of an endogenous substance. The current amplitude of responses to 1–3 μm NMDA, an NMDA receptor agonist without transporter activity, increased only slightly as perfusion was halted (Fig. 6A). In six cells the PDC response was potentiated 266 ± 70%, whereas NMDA responses in the same cell changed by only 43 ± 23%. The smaller potentiation during NMDA-elicited responses was not caused by response saturation, because NMDA responses were always smaller than transporter substrate responses (−35.6 ± 8.5 pA vs −109.2 ± 27 pA). The small apparent accumulation with NMDA application could be caused by accumulation of glutamate from spontaneous synaptic release or other sources.
Figure 6A1,A2 shows an example in which 10 μm PDC was perfused onto a microculture neuron for 5 sec, and then solution flow was halted. We have shown previously that glia can be destroyed acutely while neural integrity is preserved in microcultures (Mennerick et al., 1998). To determine whether glia are the source of accumulating glutamate, in the case shown in Figure 6A2, the underlying glia were destroyed by mechanically rupturing the glial membrane with a sharp pipette. The response to PDC was greatly reduced, whereas the response to 3 μm NMDA was preserved (Fig. 6A1,A2). In five microcultures, the PDC response after astrocyte elimination was 46 ± 19% of the baseline PDC response before elimination, whereas the control NMDA response after elimination was 108 ± 17% of the baseline response. These results suggest that a significant amount of accumulation observed in response to substrate application is from microculture glia.
Response potentiation in static bath conditions was observed with both PDC and THA and was totally abolished by D-APV (Fig.6B). In cells with a strongly potentiated static bath response, both the steady-state response during perfusion and the potentiated static-bath current were diminished by coapplication of TBOA (Fig. 6B). Average inhibition by 50 μm TBOA of the static-bath response was 49 ± 9% in seven cells treated with either 5 μmTHA or 5 μm PDC. The inhibition by TBOA suggests that the accumulation of agonist is largely caused by TBOA-sensitive heteroexchange rather than accumulation of glutamate from another source.
TBOA (50 μm) alone had no effect on NMDA-induced responses (Fig. 6C) and had either no effect on baseline membrane currents (Fig. 6B) or induced a small inward current in stop-flow conditions (four of eight neurons, 53.6 ± 24.6 pA). This result suggests sources of glutamate accumulation other than heteroexchange or a weak agonist effect of TBOA. Similar glutamate receptor activation in the presence of TBOA has recently been reported and preliminarily attributed to a cystine/glutamate exchanger (Jabaudon et al., 1999).
These results suggest that glutamate accumulation is significant during application of transporter substrates, and previous results suggest that glial depolarization can promote glutamate release via reverse uptake (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1994). Therefore, we used a comparison of THA and TBOA effects on EPSCs to determine whether accumulating glutamate might explain EPSC prolongations. TBOA and its parent compound THA produced similar prolongations of AMPA receptor-mediated EPSCs when used at equimolar concentrations in the presence of 10 μm cyclothiazide (Fig. 7). On average, THA prolonged EPSC 10–90% decay time by 41 ± 9%, and TBOA prolonged EPSCs by 57 ± 17% in the same cells. Examination of THA and TBOA effects in individual cells showed a high correlation between the effects of the two drugs (Fig. 7B), suggesting that THA and TBOA prolong EPSCs through a similar mechanism.
It is still possible that glutamate accumulation is promoted in the presence of TBOA, as suggested by inward current shifts in some cells. Therefore, we asked whether low concentrations of glutamate mimic EPSC prolongations observed in the presence of transport inhibitors. Figure8A shows the effect on an EPSC (no CYZ present) of applying 2.5 μml-glutamate and 50 μmPDC. PDC caused a small inward shift in the baseline holding current (−12 pA) but no decrease in the peak amplitude of the EPSC (Fig.8A, inset). In contrast, application of 2.5 μm glutamate caused a larger inward current (−50 pA) and slight suppression of the EPSC peak amplitude, indicating that exogenous glutamate reached synaptic regions in excess of endogenous glutamate concentrations achieved during PDC application. Suppression of the EPSC was 22 ± 3% by 2.5 μm glutamate and 2 ± 4% by 50 μm PDC (n = 10;p < 0.05, paired t test). Prolongation of the EPSC was more pronounced in the presence of PDC than in the presence of glutamate (Fig. 8). In 10 cells, the 10–90% decay time was prolonged by 21 ± 8% with PDC and −1 ± 5% by glutamate (p < 0.05, paired t test). Taken together, the results indicate that glutamate at levels likely to accumulate during uptake inhibition in microcultures is unlikely to account for the EPSC prolongations observed. The results also suggest that properties of AMPA receptors, including low affinity for glutamate (Patneau and Mayer, 1990) and profound desensitization (Trussell et al., 1988), help limit the cooperativity observed between exogenous and synaptic transmitters observed at other synapses (Hartzell et al., 1975).
As a positive control, we tested whether under extreme conditions, glutamate accumulation itself can indeed prolong EPSCs through a cooperative interaction with synaptic glutamate. We examined the effect of exogenous glutamate on EPSCs in lithium solutions to block uptake and in the presence of 10 μm CYZ to diminish AMPA receptor desensitization. Under these conditions, applications of 2.5 μm glutamate prolonged EPSCs slightly (Fig.8B), indicating that with transport and receptor desensitization blocked, a cooperative interaction between ambient and synaptic agonist occurs under these extreme experimental conditions. Three of six neurons treated with 2.5 μmglutamate in Li+ responded with reliable, reversible EPSC prolongations similar to the cell shown in Figure 8. We conclude that although glutamate accumulation may participate in prolonged EPSCs, blockade of glutamate transporters accounts most strongly for EPSC prolongations in the presence of uptake inhibitors.
DISCUSSION
Glutamate transport inhibitors prolong EPSCs at various synapsesin vitro and in situ. Our results suggest that neither a glutamate buffering effect nor accumulation of ambient glutamate explains the prolongation of EPSCs caused by glutamate transporter inhibitors and substrates. We observe a slight increase in apparent affinity of transporters for glutamate at positive membrane potentials; therefore, simple binding of glutamate is not sufficient to explain the role of transporters in truncating synaptic glutamate transients. In addition, although we have shown that accumulation of glutamate occurs with application of transporter substrates, accumulation of glutamate via heteroexchange or reverse uptake cannot explain EPSC prolongations, because the nonsubstrate inhibitor TBOA does not promote glutamate efflux but has effects similar to the substrate THA on EPSC time course. Also, exogenous glutamate applications do not mimic effects of transporter substrates. Our results do not address the very rapid effects of transporter substrates observed by others (Tong and Jahr, 1994; Diamond and Jahr, 1997), whereby the peak concentration of glutamate achieved is potentiated by inhibition of transporters. It is therefore likely that these rapid effects, evidenced by changes in the rise time and peak of the EPSC at physiological temperatures, are entirely consistent with the present observations. In this case a stationary buffer property of transporters functions on the fastest time scales, and translocation functions on the slower time scale of EPSC decay.
For glutamate turnover to explain the EPSC prolongations observed, transporters must be present at high density near synaptic sites, or turnover must be substantially faster than suggested. It is possible that both propositions are accurate. Recent physiological and anatomical results suggest that in culture and in situtransporter density are very high, approaching 11,000 transporters per μm−2 in CA1 stratum radiatum of the rat hippocampus (Lehre and Danbolt, 1998). Alternative methods for calculating substrate turnover have also recently suggested a somewhat faster turnover rate than previous techniques: ∼40/sec at 24°C (Bergles and Jahr, 1998). These observations also suggest that the glial transporter-associated currents recorded during synaptic transmission (Fig. 1) represent significant glutamate translocation on the time scale of an EPSC.
Our results suggest that the GLAST glutamate transporter and the transporters responsible for glutamate uptake in microculture astrocytes (largely GLAST) (Mennerick et al., 1998) do not change their affinity for glutamate with changes in membrane potential. Other steps in the transport cycle, such as sodium binding or closely related steps, may impart the observed voltage dependence of transport (Brew and Attwell, 1987). However, it is noteworthy that if, as suggested, voltage alters only steps in the translocation of glutamate, membrane potential must decrease the probability that a molecule of glutamate transports rather than unbinds. In this case, the meaning of buffering becomes more complicated, and it is precisely because glutamate can dissociate from the transporter (the transporter no longer effectively buffers glutamate) that EPSCs are prolonged.
A particularly surprising finding of the present results is that the nonsubstrate inhibitor TBOA binds equally well to the transporter at positive potentials and at negative potentials (Figs. 3, 4). In current models of the transporter, sodium must bind to the extracellular-facing transporter before glutamate or glutamate analogs can bind the transporter (Kanner and Bendahan, 1982; Wadiche et al., 1995b; Otis and Jahr, 1998). It is also generally believed that presteady-state currents represent the binding (with hyperpolarization) and unbinding (with depolarization) of sodium at a site on the transporter at least partway through the transmembrane electric field (Wadiche et al., 1995b). The results of Figure 4 suggest either that sodium binding is not necessary for TBOA binding to the transporter or sodium binding and unbinding is not the basis for the presteady-state charge movements. Further work will be needed to distinguish these possibilities.
The evidence for significant glutamate efflux via heteroexchange and reverse transport is controversial. The current work shows clear evidence for rapid heteroexchange of substrates for endogenous glutamate in native cells and shows that glutamate accumulation through this mechanism can be substantial (Volterra et al., 1996). Although accumulation of synaptic glutamate released from surrounding synapses may also be a source of accumulated glutamate in many preparations, in our studies this route of accumulation is minimized by restricting analysis to solitary neurons and by removing extracellular calcium to diminish synaptic glutamate release.
Three additional lines of evidence suggest that the glutamate accumulation observed in the present study occurs via heteroexchange. First, the nonsubstrate inhibitor TBOA does not induce significant accumulation currents in neurons at concentrations 5- to 10-fold higher than substrates. Second, eliminating underlying glia nearly eliminates the substrate-induced currents. Finally, TBOA diminishes the accumulation currents induced by substrates.
The present results suggest that although ambient glutamate is not likely to be a large contributor to prolonged EPSCs, efflux of glutamate through astrocyte transporters is significant. Ambient transmitter accumulation may be expected to interact cooperatively at postsynaptic receptors with synaptic transmitter at time points during the EPSC when receptors are not saturated (Hartzell et al., 1975). These time points are likely to be late in the EPSC when glutamate levels have dropped or have diffused beyond the synapse, precisely the time points affected by transport inhibitors (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1995; Takahashi et al., 1995; Otis et al., 1996). Our results rule out an important contribution of ambient glutamate to the time course of individual EPSCs, although a small cooperative effect of glutamate was observed with transporters blocked and with receptors sensitized with CYZ. It is likely that under normal conditions the low affinity of AMPA receptors (Patneau and Mayer, 1990) and their desensitization to low glutamate concentrations (Trussell and Fischbach, 1989) limit the cooperative interaction between ambient and synaptic transmitter.
Despite the lack of effect of glutamate accumulation on the time course of AMPA receptor-mediated EPSCs, it is clear that glutamate released from astrocytes in the presence of transport substrates can activate neuronal NMDA receptors, and previous work has suggested that substrates can depress transmission through glutamate accumulation (Maki et al., 1994). Other pathological or physiological conditions under which efflux might occur and the contribution of this efflux to modulation of neurotransmission or initiation of neurotoxicity await further study.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants MH-00964 and MH-45493 and a gift from the Bantley Foundation (C.F.Z.) and a Lucille P. Markey postdoctoral fellowship (S.M.). We thank Joe Tucker, Amy Zarrin, Yuki Izumi, and Bob Cormier for discussion, and Aguan Wei for the gift of the pOX expression vector.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Steven Mennerick, Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, 4940 Children's Place, St. Louis, MO 63110. E-mail:menneris{at}psychiatry.wustl.edu.