Abstract
EphA family receptor tyrosine kinases and their ephrin-A ligands are involved in patterning axonal connections during brain development, but until now a role for these molecules in the mature brain had not been elucidated. Here, we show that both the EphA5 receptor and its ephrin-A ligands (2 and 5) are expressed in the adult mouse hippocampus, and the EphA5 protein is present in a phosphorylated form. Because there are no pharmacological agents available for EphA receptors, we designed recombinant immunoadhesins that specifically bind to the receptor binding site of the ephrin-A ligand (antagonist) or the ligand binding site of the EphA receptor (agonist) and thus target EphA function. We demonstrate that intrahippocampal infusion of an EphA antagonist immunoadhesin leads to impaired performance in two behavioral paradigms, T-maze spontaneous alternation and context-dependent fear conditioning, sensitive to hippocampal function, whereas activation of EphA by infusion of an agonist immunoadhesin results in enhanced performance on these tasks. Because the two behavioral tasks have different motivational, perceptual, and motor requirements, we infer the changes were not caused by these performance factors but rather to cognitive alterations. We also find bidirectional changes in gene expression and in electrophysiological measures of synaptic efficacy that correlate with the behavioral results. Thus, EphA receptors and their ligands are implicated as mediators of plasticity in the adult mammalian brain.
EphA receptors represent the largest subfamily of receptor tyrosine kinases (Friedman and O'Leary, 1996). The ephrin-A ligands interact with EphA receptors to mediate repulsive axonal guidance in patterning the developing nervous system (Drescher et al., 1995; Winslow et al., 1995; Gao et al., 1996; Zhang et al., 1996; Meima et al., 1997; Orioli and Klein, 1997; Zhou, 1997; Flanagan and Vanderhaegen, 1998; Frisén et al., 1998; P. P. Gao et al., 1998). It is not known whether EphA receptors play any role in the adult brain in vivo. In this report, we use in situ hybridization and quantitative (TaqMan) reverse transcription (RT)-PCR to show that both the EphA5 receptor and its ligands, ephrin-A5 and -A2, are expressed in the mature mouse brain. We also investigate whether EphA5 is or can be activated, i.e., phosphorylated, in the adult mouse brain.
EphA receptors were shown to play a role in determining patterns of synapse formation in development (Drescher et al., 1995; Gao et al., 1996; Zhang et al., 1996; Zhou, 1997; Flanagan and Vanderhaegen, 1998;Frisén et al., 1998; P. P. Gao et al., 1998), and recently, these receptors and their ligands were found to have PDZ (postsynaptic density-95/Discs large/zona occludens-1) recognition motifs and to bind to and colocalize with PDZ proteins at synaptic sites of the mammalian neuron in vitro (Torres et al., 1998). Initial evidence for in vitro effects on synaptic plasticity, including long-term potentiation (LTP), has also been obtained (W.-Q. Gao et al., 1998). Given the continued expression of EphA5 and its ligands in the adult brain, these results raise the possibility that EphA receptors are not only involved in embryonic development of the brain but also in synaptic remodeling and plasticity thought to underlie learning and memory (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993). Therefore, we sought to determine whether EphA receptors regulate neuronal function in vivo in the mature brain and, in turn, influence learning.
To target EphA5 function, we use a novel strategy termed protein targeting (Gerlai et al., 1998b) based on in vivoapplication of recombinant fusion proteins, the immunoadhesins (Chamow and Ashkenazy, 1996). The immunoadhesins used in the present study contain the full native ligand binding domain of the EphA5 receptor (EphA5-IgG) or the receptor binding domain of the ephrin-A5 ligand (ephrinA5-IgG). These immunoadhesins have opposing effects on the receptor: EphA5-IgG, by scavenging the endogenous ligand, acts as an antagonist and ephrinA5-IgG, by dimerizing and initiating the autophosphorylation cycle of the receptor, as an agonist of EphA function (Winslow et al., 1995; Meima et al., 1997).
In the present paper, we focus on the analysis of behavioral changes elicited by intrahippocampal infusion of the EphA antagonist and agonist immunoadhesins and also study potential underlying mechanisms, including gene expression changes and synaptic plasticity. We demonstrate, for the first time, that EphA receptors play a significant role in the adult brain in which they influence cognitive function.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals and housing. Experimental mice (males) from inbred strains C57BL/6 and DBA/2 were 3–4 months old and were housed in groups of 10 under standard conditions described previously (Gerlai et al., 1998b). All experiments involving comparison of genotype or treatment groups were performed blind in a randomized manner.
In situ hybridization. For EphA5 in situhybridization, 4-month-old DBA/2 and C57BL/6 mouse brains were fresh frozen with powdered dry ice. Sections were processed by a method described previously (Melton et al., 1984; Phillips et al., 1990). Probes were synthesized according to Melton et al. (1984) from a 407 DNA fragment that included nucleotides 3040–3446 (I. W. Caras and J. W. Winslow; patent WO9613518-A1; accession number P-T18893; 1996).
RT-PCR. Total RNA was isolated using the Qiagen (Hilden, Germany) RNeasy Mini Kit from 50 mg mouse hippocampi freshly frozen in liquid nitrogen. RNA was aliquoted and stored at −70°C until used. Oligonucleotide probes and primers were designed to recognize the genes using the Primer Express software [Applied Biosystems (Foster City, CA) and Perkin-Elmer (Emeryville, CA)]. Real-time quantitative RT-PCR (TaqMan) was performed and analyzed using the Promega (Madison, WI) Access RT-PCR System in the ABI Model 7700 Sequence Detection system (Gibson et al., 1996; Heid et al., 1996). Samples were run in duplicate with standard curves of Clontech (Cambridge, UK) mouse brain total RNA on every plate for all genes, and the linearity of amplification was confirmed (Gibson et al., 1996; Heid et al., 1996). The data shown are calculated from the standard curves, and each sample is normalized to GAPDH, a housekeeping gene, as described previously (Gibson et al., 1996; Heid et al., 1996).
Western blot. The methods are described in detail previously (Winslow et al., 1995; Gerlai et al., 1998b). Briefly, freshly harvested hippocampal tissue was homogenized, and EphA5 was immunoprecipitated using an anti-EphA5 antibody (Genentech, San Francisco, CA). Phosphorylation levels were tested using anti-phosphotyrosine kinase antibody (4G10; Upstate Biotechnology, Lake Placid, NY). In the first Western blot experiment to detect basalin vivo phosphorylation levels of EphA5, both hippocampi (∼60 mg tissue) of each individual mouse was used for protein extraction for each lane of the gel electrophoresis. Because the signal strength was high, to detect further increases in phosphorylation levels induced by ephrinA5-IgG, in the second Western blot experiment, a smaller amount (∼15 mg) of hippocampal tissue was used and the blot was exposed to anti-phosphotyrosine kinase antibody for an 85% shorter time (2 hr vs the previously applied 12 hr incubation).
Stereotaxic surgery and infusion. Using a stereotaxic frame, mice received bilateral intrahippocampal implantation of cannulas as described in detail previously (Gerlai et al., 1998b) at position anteroposterior −1.5, mediolateral +1.8, as measured in millimeters from bregma, and dorsoventral 1.8 mm from flat skull surface. Cannulas were connected to a pair of ALZET 1007D micro-osmotic pumps (volume of 99 μl; pumping rate of 0.51 μl/hr; delivery period of ∼8 d; ALZET brain infusion kit; Alza, Palo Alto, CA) placed subcutaneously on the back. For C57BL/6 mice, CD4-IgG (control) or EphA5-IgG (antagonist) immunoadhesins were infused [4.8 mg/ml in artificial CSF (ACSF) containing (in mm): 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 2.4 CaCl2, 2.4 MgSO4.7H2O, 1.25 KH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, and 10 d-glucose]. For DBA/2 mice, CD4-IgG or ephrinA5-IgG was infused (0.5 mg/ml in ACSF). In addition, ephrinA5-IgG (0.5 mg/ml in ACSF) was also infused in C57BL/6. The length of infusion was chosen to be 8 d in both C57BL/6 and DBA/2, which allowed the large and slowly diffusing immunoadhesins to fully infuse the dorsal part and to also reach the majority of the ventral part of the hippocampus (Gerlai et al., 1998b). To demonstrate behavioral effects, a large portion, especially the dorsal part, of the hippocampus must be influenced, as shown by lesion studies (Moser et al., 1995) or tetrodotoxin application (Lorenzini et al., 1996a,b). Thus, continuous and prolonged infusion decreased the hippocampal areas remaining uninfused and the possibility that such areas may render immunoadhesin effects unobservable.
Immunoadhesins. The design and production of immunoadhesins, as well as their advantages, including stability and detection, have been described and discussed in detail previously (Winslow et al., 1995; Chamow and Ashkenazi, 1996; Meima et al., 1997; Gerlai et al., 1998b). We used three different immunoadhesins in this study. EphA5-IgG contains the full ligand binding domain of the EphA5 receptors, but its signaling domain is replaced by the Fc portion of the IgG1 molecule, which includes the hinge region. This protein specifically binds to the ephrin-A ligands recognized by the EphA5 receptor, thus making them inaccessible to the endogenous receptor (antagonist). The second immunoadhesin is ephrinA5-IgG, which contains the receptor binding domain of the ephrin-A5 ligand linked to the Fc of IgG1. Because the hinge region of Fc confers flexibility and because the Fc is made up of two chains, this molecule has two conformationally correct and mobile receptor binding domains that mimic the behavior of membrane bound ephrin-A ligands in that, unlike a soluble ligand, they are capable of dimerizing, and thus activating, endogenous EphA receptors that ephrin-A5 recognizes. The third immunoadhesin used is CD4-IgG, a control molecule that is similar in molecular weight and composition to the active immunoadhesins but has no binding activity relevant in the CNS (Winslow et al., 1995; Chamow and Ashkenazi, 1996).
Immunohistochemistry. The methods followed standard protocols (Gerlai et al., 1998b). A biotinylated donkey anti-human IgG antibody (catalog #709–65-149; Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA) 1:200 in PBS was used to detect the IgG portion of the immunoadhesins. Brains were sectioned, and the sections were incubated in avidin—biotin–peroxidase complex (Elite ABC kit; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA), rinsed, and developed using a standard diaminobenzidine reaction (Enhanced Metal; Pierce, Rockford, IL).
T-maze continuous spontaneous alternation task. The procedure followed the methods developed and described previously (Gerlai, 1998a). Note that, as conducted here, T-maze alternation test has been shown to be highly sensitive to hippocampal dysfunction. Mice were allowed to alternate between the left and right goal arms of the T-maze throughout a 15 trial session. Once they have entered a particular goal arm, a guillotine door was lowered to block entry to the opposite arm. The door was removed only after the mice returned to the start arm, thus allowing a new alternation trial to be started. Alternation rate was calculated as a ratio between the alternating choices and total number of choices (50%, random choice; 100%, alternation at every trial; 0%, no alternation). Time to complete 15 choices was also measured.
Fear conditioning. The methods have been described in detail previously (Gerlai, 1998b). Note that these methods were designed to maximize the context dependency and hippocampal sensitivity of the paradigm (Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Abeliovich et al., 1993; Aiba et al., 1994; Paylor et al., 1994; Bach et al., 1995). Briefly, the paradigm had three phases: a training phase, a context-dependent test, and a cue-dependent test. For training, mice received three electric foot shocks (1 sec, 0.7 mA), each preceded by an 80 dB, 2900 Hz, 20-sec-long tone cue in the Gemini avoidance apparatus (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) as shown in Figures 5and 7. Context and cue test apparatuses received olfactory cues different from those of training. The context test was performed in the training chamber, but no shock or tone was delivered. The cue test was performed in another chamber identical in size but different in visual, olfactory, and tactile cues from those of the training chamber. Tone signals identical to the one used in training but without a shock were given. Behavior was video-recorded and later quantified using an event recording computer program (Observer; Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands). Behavior elements, as indicated in Figures 5, 7, and 8, were analyzed. In a separate set of fear conditioning experiments, only one tone and shock stimulus pair was given during training, and only one tone cue was presented during cue testing. Other parameters and procedural details remained unchanged.
Electrophysiology. Transverse 300 μm hippocampal slices were submerged in a recording chamber (Fine Science Tools Inc., Foster City, CA), continuously perfused with 30°C oxygenated (95% O2–5% CO2) ACSF for at least 1 hr before recording. A bipolar glass electrode filled with ACSF stimulated Schaffer collaterals. Field EPSPs (fEPSPs) elicited by single-pulse stimulation at 0.2 Hz were recorded in CA1 stratum radiatum by a glass electrode filled with ACSF. Input/output (I/O) characteristics and ratio of the fEPSP slope to the presynaptic fiber volley (PSFV) amplitude were used to estimate basal synaptic transmission. I/O characteristics, recorded by applying gradually increased (in 20 steps increments) stimulus intensity to evoke fEPSP from minimum to maximum slope, were estimated by Michaelis–Menten sigmoid curve fit. Km50 was taken as a 50% point between the threshold and maximal response (A/DVANCE software; McKellar Designs). Baseline was recorded for 1 hr with the stimulus intensity set to evoke a fEPSP that was 30–50% of the maximal slope. Paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) was evoked by applying paired pulses of the same intensity as for baseline recording with interpulse intervals of 50, 100, 150, 200, and 250 msec. LTP was elicited by applying four trains of 100 Hz tetanus (1 sec duration, 20 sec apart), with the same stimulus intensity as for the baseline and PPF. In our attempt to test the potential improving effects of ephrinA5-IgG on synaptic plasticity in slices from C57BL/6 mice, we used another protocol of tetanization that is subthreshold for LTP induction. In this set of experiments (for results, see Fig. 12), one train of 100 Hz tetanus was applied for 1 sec with stimulus intensity set to evoke a fEPSP 10–15% of maximal slope. Recordings in all experiments were made with an Axoprobe-1A amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA) interfaced with a Power Macintosh 7100/66 computer (Apple Computers, Cupertino, CA). Data were acquired, digitized, and analyzed using A/DVANCE software (McKellar Designs).
Statistical analysis. Data were analyzed by ttest, repeated-measures ANOVA, and post hoc Tukey's honestly significant difference (HSD) test. Variance homogeneity was tested by Bartlett's test. Only relevant main effects are presented; detailed data analysis results are available on request.
RESULTS
Expression pattern of EphA5 and its ligands in the adult mouse brain
Using in situ hybridization (Melton et al., 1984;Phillips et al., 1990), we found strong expression of EphA5 mRNA in pyramidal neurons of all hippocampal CA fields and in dentate gyrus granule cells in the hippocampal formation in adult mice from inbred strains C57BL/6 (Fig.1B,C) and DBA/2 (Fig. 1E,F). In addition, expression was detected in the cortex (e.g., piriform cortex), amygdala, thalamus, and hypothalamus (Fig. 1). In situ hybridization could not reveal a clear signal, but a more sensitive quantitative RT-PCR (Gibson et al., 1996; Heid et al., 1996) demonstrated the presence of ephrin-A5 and -A2 (the preferred ligands of EphA5) (Frisén et al., 1998) mRNA in the hippocampus of both mouse strains (Fig. 1G), and the expression levels were not different between strains (ephrin-A5, t = 0.447; df = 10; p > 0.60; ephrin-A2, t = 0.008; df = 10; p > 0.99). Furthermore, using Western blot analysis (for review, see Winslow et al., 1995; Gerlai et al., 1998b), we revealed that EphA5 protein is present in the mature hippocampus in both strains in a phosphorylated, activated form (Fig. 1I), implying that EphA5 is involved in some aspect of neural function in the adult mouse CNS.
Infusion of immunoadhesins into the hippocampus
To confirm that the antagonist immunoadhesin EphA5-IgG and the agonist immunoadhesin ephrinA5-IgG binds to their relevant targets under conditions similar to those of the in vivo application we were planning (37°C; ALZET micro-osmotic pump), ELISA essay was performed. The immunoadhesin solution was injected into a micro-osmotic pump, the pump was placed in a thermostat at 37°C, and the solution was sampled over a 10 d period. The results (data not presented) showed that both immunoadhesins retained a significant and specific binding capacity.
To study EphA function, we infused the antagonist or agonist immunoadhesin bilaterally into the hippocampus using micro-osmotic pumps (Gerlai et al., 1998b). The result of infusion, analyzed by immunostaining, showed homogeneous immunoadhesin distribution within the hippocampus (Fig.2A,B). Cortical areas within 1 mm surrounding the cannula insertion site were also stained in some samples, but other cortical areas or infrahippocampal structures remained uninfused. As shown previously by magnetic resonance imaging and immunocytochemistry (Gerlai et al., 1998b), immunoadhesin infusion did not lead to detectable physical damage, edema, or inflammation in the brain. Furthermore, no gross morphological changes were observed at the light microscope level using immunostaining for NMDA-R1, GluR1, PKC, GABA, synaptobrevin, and synaptotagmin, or Nissl stain (data not shown). Last, all mice appeared well groomed and healthy, exhibited no abnormal behaviors in their home cage, and achieved normal life span when allowed to age (data not shown).
Consistent with its antagonist action, EphA5-IgG was shown previously to impair phosphorylation of the EphA5 receptor (Winslow et al., 1995;Meima et al., 1997). The evidence for the contrary, i.e., for an agonist action, was lacking. Using Western blot analysis, here we show that EphA5 phosphorylation can be elevated above basal levels in vivo by ephrinA5-IgG infusion in the hippocampus (Fig.3). The correct molecular weight for the monomer EphA5 protein is shown, and the signal above it represents the dimerized form of the protein.
To determine whether EphA receptors mediate neural function, we focussed our attention to the hippocampus because of the expression pattern of EphA5 and also because of the availability of behavioral paradigms and electrophysiological methods sensitive to detect dysfunction of this structure. We put considerable emphasis on behavioral analysis because it offers a sensitive way to detect neural functional changes (Gerlai, 1996c; Gerlai and Clayton, 1999).
Behavioral changes elicited by immunoadhesin infusion
We use the availability of two inbred strains of mice, C57BL/6 and DBA/2, that exhibit striking differences in hippocampal function at the behavioral level (Paylor et al., 1994; Gerlai, 1998a,b). First, we infused EphA5-IgG or a control immunoadhesin, CD4-IgG, bilaterally into the hippocampus of adult C57BL/6 mice for 8 d and then tested behavioral performance in the T-maze continuous spontaneous alternation task (T-CAT), a newly developed paradigm sensitive to detect hippocampal dysfunction and abnormalities that affect spatial learning and working memory (Gerlai, 1998a). C57BL/6 mice infused with the control CD4-IgG immunoadhesin exhibited an excellent alternation rate (Fig. 4A), a performance typical for intact mice of this strain (Douglas, 1990;Gerlai, 1998a). In contrast, mice infused with EphA5-IgG exhibited a significantly impaired alternation rate (Fig. 4A). The impairment is unlikely to be caused by altered motor performance or motivation levels, because both groups of mice finished the task within the same amount of time (Fig. 4B). Furthermore, the impairment is also unlikely to be caused by altered sensory capabilities because the task can be solved using multiple modalities of cues whose processing is distributed in several brain areas left unaffected by the immunoadhesin infusion.
The deficit in EphA5-IgG-infused mice was further characterized using context-dependent fear conditioning (CDFC), a configural learning task also found sensitive to hippocampal dysfunction (Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Abeliovich et al., 1993; Aiba et al., 1994; Bach et al., 1995; Gerlai, 1998b). The reason to conduct this additional behavioral test was that CDFC and T-CAT are associated with different motor (T-maze: activity; CDFC: passivity), perceptual (T-maze: extra-maze visual cues; CDFC: intramaze visual, olfactory, tactile, and auditory cues), and motivational (T-maze: spontaneous curiosity; CDFC: negatively reinforced fear) requirements, yet both tests are sensitive to hippocampal dysfunction. Thus, comparison of T-maze and CDFC performance may allow one to dissociate simple performance deficits or improvements from alterations in hippocampal (cognitive) function. In the CDFC paradigm, hippocampal damage results in the loss of freezing (a natural response to pain or fear) to contextual cues, although sparing response to a simple associative cue (Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992) (see also Gerlai, 1998b). After training, control (CD4-IgG-infused) C57BL/6 mice exhibited a substantial freezing response to both a simple associative cue (cued task) and complex contextual cues (context-dependent task) (Fig.5B,C). Compared with controls, mice treated with EphA5-IgG showed a significant deficit in the context-dependent task but not in the cued task (Fig. 5B,C). The performance deficit of the EphA5-IgG-treated mice is unlikely to be caused by sensory, motor, or motivational disturbances, because no differences were seen between responses of EphA5-IgG and CD4-IgG mice during either the training session or in the cued task (Fig.5A,C). Furthermore, because the deficit was specific to the context task, it is unlikely to be attributable to a generalized learning defect or health-related problems. To further ascertain that changes in the freezing response was not simply caused by altered motor performance, i.e., ability to remain motionless, other behavioral elements associated with fear (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1969; Gerlai et al., 1993), including locomotion and grooming (negative correlation with fear), and long-body posture (positive correlation with fear) (Fig. 5, bottom panels) were also measured. Analysis of these elements confirmed the context-specific nature of the learning performance deficit.
It is unlikely that a context-specific learning deficit induced by the EphA antagonist may result from nonspecific disruption of brain function unrelated to learning. Nevertheless, to rule out this possibility, one may need to show improved learning performance in response to EphA activation. Thus, ephrinA5-IgG, the agonist immunoadhesin (Winslow et al., 1995; Meima et al., 1997), was bilaterally infused into the hippocampus of DBA/2 mice, a strain that displays deficits in hippocampal learning tasks and other measures of hippocampal function (Crusio et al., 1990; Matsuyama et al., 1997;Gerlai, 1998b). DBA/2 mice infused with ephrinA5-IgG exhibited an improvement in performance in both T-CAT and CDFC paradigms (Figs.6, 7). Whereas control DBA/2 mice infused with CD4-IgG did not alternate above chance level on the T-CAT, a performance typical of this strain (Gerlai, 1998a), the alternation rate of mice treated with ephrinA5-IgG was increased significantly above chance and the rate of the control mice (Fig. 6A), but the time to complete the task remained unchanged (Fig. 6B). In the CDFC paradigm, ephrinA5-IgG infusion elicited a selective improvement on the context-dependent task (Fig. 7B), a performance enhancement most probably attributable to cognitive rather than sensory, motor, or motivational effects (Fig. 7A,C). Again, the changes were observable not only in freezing but in the other behavioral measures of fear (Fig. 7, bottom panels).
To investigate whether the improving effects of intrahippocampal ephrinA5-IgG infusion are generalizable to other mouse strains, we infused C57BL/6 mice with this immunoadhesin or the control CD4-IgG. Normal C57BL/6, or CD4-IgG-infused C57BL/6, mice exhibit good contextual and cue-dependent learning performance in fear conditioning (Gerlai, 1998b; Gerlai et al., 1998b) (Fig. 5). The high freezing values may represent a ceiling problem in a learning paradigm in which further improvements are expected. Indeed, ephrinA5-IgG-infused C57BL/6 mice showed excellent performance in both the context and the cue tests (reaching 80–90% freezing), but they were statistically indistinguishable from control mice infused with CD4-IgG, which also froze at that level (repeated-measures ANOVA; training immunoadhesin effect, F(1,15) = 2.042;p > 0.170; time × immunoadhesin interaction,F(5,75) = 1.874; p > 0.10; context test immunoadhesin effect,F(1,15) = 0.017; p > 0.890; immunoadhesin × time interaction,F(5,75) = 1.088; p > 0.370; cue test immunoadhesin effect, F(1,15) = 2.458; p > 0.135; immunoadhesin × time interaction, F(5,75) = 1.905;p > 0.10). Although these results suggest that ephrinA5-IgG has no deleterious effects on contextual or cued learning performance, they do not allow us to ascertain whether this immunoadhesin can lead to improvements in C57BL/6 mice. To achieve submaximal performance in C57BL/6 and avoid this ceiling problem, we modified the fear conditioning paradigm and presented only one tone–shock stimulus pairing instead of three. This stimulation has been shown to lead to a lower freezing performance (30–40%) level in the context test (Silva et al., 1996), presumably indicating the formation of a less robust memory.
The results of the one stimulus pair CDFC test convincingly demonstrated that ephrinA5-IgG improved performance in C57BL/6 mice in a context-dependent manner (Fig. 8). Figure 8A shows that all mice responded to a single tone–shock presentation with increased freezing, although the increase was less robust compared with when three such stimulus pairs were administered. Freezing response to context 1 d after training was at 30–40% level in the control CD4-IgG-infused mice (Fig.8B), a performance similar to that reported previously in normal mice in this training paradigm (Silva et al., 1996). However, freezing performance in the context task was significantly increased to 60–70% (Fig. 8B) in mice infused with ephrinA5-IgG. Cued task performance was unaltered, and both groups of mice responded to the presentation of a single tone cue with a significant and equivalent increase of freezing (Fig.8C). The context-specific performance enhancement was confirmed by the results of the other behavioral elements (Fig. 8,bottom panels), including locomotion, long-body, and grooming, all suggesting an elevated fear response to context in ephrinA5-IgG-infused mice but unaltered responses in training and cue test compared with CD4-IgG-infused animals. These results confirm that the performance enhancement is not caused by changes in motor or perceptual abilities but by improved cognitive function and that this improvement is not unique to DBA/2 but can also be observed in C57BL/6 mice.
Gene expression changes induced by immunoadhesin infusion
Given the presynaptic and postsynaptic localization of EphA receptors (W.-Q. Gao et al., 1998), their role in synapse formation during development, and their suggested involvement in cytoskeletal processes (Meima et al., 1997; Torres et al., 1998), to investigate potential mechanisms underlying the observed behavioral changes, we tested gene expression of neuronal, synaptic, and cytoskeletal markers. RT-PCR for synaptophysine and NMDA-R1 performed on hippocampal tissue samples from mice infused in vivo for 8 d with the immunoadhesins showed no differences in expression levels (data not shown). However, mRNA expression of the tubulin gene (M-α1), whose product is a cytosolic microtubular protein involved in dendritic and axonal cytoskeletal processes, showed significant immunoadhesin-induced changes. Tubulin mRNA expression tested by RT-PCR showed that EphA5-IgG increases and ephrinA5-IgG decreases transcription (Fig.9A) from this gene, a finding consistent with the growth-arresting repulsive effects of the ephrin-A5 ligand during CNS development. Furthermore, another cytoskeletal protein, the microtubule-associated protein MAP2 involved in neuronal activity-dependent dendritic structural changes (Quinlan and Halpain, 1996) also showed a similar trend. Although nonsignificant, MAP2 mRNA expression appeared higher in hippocampal tissue infused in vivo for 8 d with EphA5-IgG and lower in hippocampal samples infused with ephrinA5-IgG compared with CD4-IgG control (Fig.9B). Note that mRNA expression is shown as the number of PCR cycles required to reach a threshold level of cleavage of a fluorescent reporter probe (Gibson et al., 1996; Heid et al., 1996), and this measure is inversely related to the original amount of mRNA present in the sample. Because EphA receptors are involved in neuronal cytostructural mechanisms during the embryonic development of the brain (Friedman and O'Leary, 1996; Gao et al., 1996; Zhang et al., 1996;Meima et al., 1997; Orioli and Klein, 1997; Zhou, 1997; Flanagan and Vanderhaegen, 1998; Frisén et al., 1998; P. P. Gao et al., 1998), the above results suggest that these receptors retain this function in the adult brain.
Immunoadhesin infusion alters synaptic plasticity
Hippocampal LTP is a cellular mechanism proposed to underlie spatial and configural learning in rodents (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993). Therefore, we also studied whether potential alterations in LTP may accompany the observed behavioral changes. Hippocampal slices prepared from mice infused in vivo with EphA5-IgG or ephrinA5-IgG for 8 d were analyzed. In slices from C57BL/6 mice infused with EphA5-IgG, the induction of LTP appeared normal, but the potentiated response decayed significantly faster compared with slices obtained from CD4-IgG control C57BL/6 mice (Fig.10A). The decay was not caused by a putative inability of the slice to exhibit larger fEPSPs because a second tetanus applied at the end of the recording session was able to reinduce a high level of LTP (data not shown). Other synaptic parameters, such as paired-pulse facilitation, and basal synaptic transmission (Fig. 10B,C) remained unaltered by EphA5-IgG infusion.
The enhancement of cognitive function by ephrinA5-IgG infusion seen in DBA/2 mice was also accompanied by changes in hippocampal synaptic function. Slices from DBA/2 mice infused with ephrinA5-IgG exhibited a significant augmentation of LTP, beginning at induction and persisting up to 4.5 hr (Fig.11A). EphrinA5-IgG treatment also increased PPF (Fig. 11B), suggesting the involvement of a presynaptic mechanism, whereas basal synaptic transmission was unchanged (Fig. 11C). Furthermore, although LTP induced by the standard four train stimulation protocol (see Materials and Methods) appeared almost identical in C57BL/6 mice infused with CD4-IgG or ephrinA5-IgG (immunoadhesin effect,F(1,8) = 0.043; p > 0.80; immunoadhesin × time interaction,F(30,240) = 0.917; p> 0.50), probably attributable to a robust LTP response leading to a ceiling effect, LTP induced by a single train and lower amplitude of electric stimulation showed a trend in C57BL/6 similar to the observations in DBA/2, suggesting a facilitatory effect of ephrinA5-IgG (Fig. 12) in both strains. It is notable, however, that although the electrophysiological and behavioral findings are generally in agreement with each other, the correlation is not perfect. For example, ephrinA5-IgG infusion was able to elicit a significant behavioral improvement in both DBA/2 and C57BL/6. This may be attributable to the genetic background of the two mouse strains differentially affecting behavioral and electrophysiological phenotypes and/or to different sensitivity and resolution of the electrophysiological and behavioral methods applied.
DISCUSSION
Our in situ hybridization findings demonstrating strong EphA5 expression in neurons of the adult mouse hippocampus, RT-PCR results showing hippocampal expression of two relevant ligands of this receptor, ephrin-A2 and -A5, and our Western blot analysis revealing the presence of EphA5 receptor in a phosphorylated active form in hippocampal tissue from adult mice, strongly suggest that EphA receptor function is not restricted to the developing nervous system. Confirming this suggestion, intrahippocampal infusion of an EphA antagonist led to significant impairment in behavioral function in adult mice, whereas infusion of an agonist resulted in improved behavioral performance. Furthermore, infusion of the antagonist led to increased expression of a cytostructural protein, tubulin, and also impaired some electrophysiological measures of hippocampal synaptic plasticity, whereas infusion of the agonist resulted in decreased tubulin expression and improved electrophysiological performance.
These results were achieved using protein targeting, a novel strategy in behavioral neuroscience that may have some advantages over more conventional methods (Gerlai et al., 1998b). The applied genetically engineered immunoadhesins recognize the ligand, or the receptor, on the basis of high-affinity natural ligand receptor binding interaction (Winslow et al., 1995; Chamow and Ashkenazi, 1996); thus, they obviate the lack of EphA-selective pharmacological agents. Furthermore, as a result of the unaltered binding sites, these immunoadhesins are capable of binding all relevant proteins that the endogenous EphA5 or ephrin-A5 molecules would bind. Because EphA receptors are promiscuous and may respond to several ephrin-A ligands, and conversely a ligand may activate several receptors (Orioli and Klein, 1997), these immunoadhesins allowed us to manipulate all functionally relevant ligands or receptors without the confounding effects of compensation by closely related ligands or receptors as occur in gene targeting (Gerlai 1996a,b; Frisén et al., 1998). Furthermore, stereotaxic delivery of the immunoadhesins into the hippocampus eliminated the possibility of inducing developmental defects during ontogenesis and also reduced the confounding effects of nonspecific changes in tissues not associated with hippocampal function (Gerlai, 1996a).
Our results indicate that inhibition and augmentation of EphA function by intrahippocampal infusion of the immunoadhesins in adult mice impairs and facilitates, respectively, behavioral responses in two tasks, T-CAT and CDFC. These tasks depend on the hippocampus (Gerlai, 1998a,b) but have different motivational, perceptual, and motor requirements. Therefore, the consistent behavioral changes we observed imply an underlying mechanism associated with hippocampal function and not with idiosyncratic characteristics unique to one or the other task, a notion also supported by the lack of indication of altered performance factors detected in these tasks. We infer, therefore, that the observed behavioral changes were attributable to altered cognitive processes. Together, these results support a role for EphA in adult neural function associated with learning, one which can be augmented by ephrinA5-IgG and impaired by EphA5-IgG.
The potential neurobiological mechanism underlying the observed behavioral effects is speculative at this point. The recent observation showing that Eph receptors and their ephrin ligands contain PDZ recognition motifs and are bound and clustered by PDZ proteins at presynaptic and postsynaptic sites of neuronal synapses in vitro suggests that Eph receptors may mediate synaptic plasticity (Hsueh and Sheng, 1998; Torres et al., 1998). In vitro,acute application of EphA5-IgG (EphA antagonist) and ephrinA5-IgG (agonist) has also been shown to influence parameters of synaptic plasticity on the hippocampal slice, implying the functional relevance of EphA receptors (W.-Q. Gao et al., 1998). These suggestive findings are now confirmed by our present electrophysiological results. The Both our and W.-Q. Gao et al.'s (1998) results generally revealed an inhibitory role for EphA5-IgG and a facilitatory effect of ephrinA5-IgG at the synaptic physiology level. W.-Q. Gao et al. (1998) found that application of ephrinA5-IgG impaired PPF, probably as a result of a facilitatory effect of ephrinA5-IgG on neurotransmitter release leading to depletion of synaptic vesicles, as these authors suggested based on other studies (Manabe et al., 1993). This suggestion was in accordance with another observation made by W.-Q. Gao et al. (1998) showing facilitated baseline synaptic responses to ephrinA5-IgG. In our present study, we could not measure preimmunoadhesin application baseline responses and thus were unable to compare the effects of ephrinA5-IgG treatment to it. However, unlike W.-Q. Gao et al. (1998), we detected increased PPF in slices taken from hippocampi chronically infused with ephrinA5-IgG. The apparent discrepancy may be explained by a common underlying mechanism, namely, facilitated neurotransmitter release. Although it has been shown that acutely enhanced neurotransmitter release leads to decreased PPF (Manabe et al., 1993), a correlation between chronic enhancement of neurotransmitter release and enhanced PPF has also been demonstrated (Geppert et al., 1997). Furthermore, W.-Q. Gao et al. (1998) could detect impaired LTP in EphA5-IgG-treated slices, similar to our findings, but they could not demonstrate improved LTP in slices bathed in ephrinA5-IgG, whereas we found increased LTP in response to chronic ephrinA5-IgG infusion. This apparent discrepancy again may reflect differences in the mode of application, i.e., acute versus chronic infusion, of the immunoadhesins leading to differential effects of EphA receptor function on time-dependent synaptic processes. These processes may involve cytostructural mechanisms in which EphA receptors have been suggested to play roles.
EphA receptors may interact with a number of proteins through their PDZ binding domains that mediate cytoskeletal processes (Torres et al., 1998) and potentially affect a range of subcellular mechanisms influencing synaptic transmission and/or plasticity. Such mechanisms may include, for example, the trafficking and docking of presynaptic vesicles (Calakos and Scheller, 1996), the clustering of neurotransmitter receptors, e.g., AMPA-R and NMDA-R (Allison et al., 1998), and the formation of “perforated” synapses associated with LTP (Geinisman et al., 1991, 1993; Edwards, 1995). Importantly, EphA5 receptor has been shown to mediate actin polymerization, and its activation by administration of ephrinA5-IgG leads to actin depolymerization and axonal growth cone collapse in neuronal cell cultures and cortical explants (Meima et al., 1997). Actin is a crucial component of the cytoskeleton present in presynaptic and postsynaptic terminals (Fifkova and Delay, 1982; Landis and Reese, 1983; Cohen et al., 1985). It has been shown to be associated with structural changes underlying synaptic plasticity (Westrum et al., 1980; Fifkova and Delay, 1982; Landis and Reese, 1983; Hayashi et al., 1996) affecting both presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms, including paired pulse facilitation and LTP (Kim and Lisman, 1999). In the latter study, phalloidin, an actin filament stabilizing agent, when applied postsynaptically, significantly impaired LTP. Remarkably, and perhaps consistent with this observation, our results showed that application of the EphA agonist ephrinA5-IgG, which destabilizes actin filaments (Meima et al., 1997), has the opposite effect, i.e. it improves LTP. Thus, it is possible that EphA receptor activation mobilizes the synapse by destabilizing actin filaments and thus allows it to undergo structural modifications necessary for plastic changes to take place, a suggestion that will need to be confirmed by detailed electronmicroscopic or confocal analyses.
The possibility that EphA receptors play roles in cytostructural processes is consistent with the changes we observed in the expression of tubulin gene in response to EphA5-IgG or ephrinA5-IgG treatment. Our results suggested that tubulin, a component of the cytoskeleton, was overexpressed in response to EphA receptor inactivation and was underexpressed in response to receptor activation in the adult mouse hippocampus, findings compatible with the known arresting effects of ephrin-A ligands on axonal and dendritic growth during CNS development (Winslow et al., 1995; Meima et al., 1997; Frisén et al., 1998). In the adult brain in which major developmental alterations do not take place, transcriptional regulation of tubulin and perhaps other genes of cytoskeletal proteins may subserve the development of new or altered synaptic connections, i.e., neural plasticity. Based on our findings, we suggest that tubulin, perhaps mediating axonal and dendritic transport, or microstructural localization of gene products required for synaptic remodeling, plays a role in such plasticity and in turn learning as assumed previously by Rose and associates (Mileusnic et al., 1980; Scholey et al., 1992; Rose, 1993).
An intriguing aspect of our findings is the observed cognitive improvement and facilitated LTP both induced by ephrinA5-IgG. LTP has been suggested to be a cellular mechanism that underlies learning and memory (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993) (but see Shors and Matzel, 1997), and, for example, numerous studies using gene targeting have shown an association between impaired LTP and impaired cognitive function in mice (for examples, see Gerlai, 1996a,c). However, the two mouse mutants in which chronically improved LTP has been observed [e.g., the GluR2 mutant (Jia et al., 1996) or the postsynaptic density-95 mutant (Migaud et al., 1998)] also exhibited impaired behavioral function (Gerlai et al., 1998a; Migaud et al., 1998). For instance, GluR2-less neurons of the GluR2 mutant mice showed increased permeability to calcium, causing an increased calcium influx leading in turn to elevated LTP upon tetanic stimulation. Gerlai et al. (1998a), argued that the nonspecific nature of the facilitation of LTP led to impaired behavioral function because it decreased the signal-to-noise ratio of stimulus processing in the brain. Migaud et al. (1998) also concluded similarly and explained that a proper balance between LTP and long-term depression induction threshold must be maintained for the neurons to process information correctly. According to this argument, experimentally improved synaptic plasticity can lead to improved cognitive function only if it does not impair the specificity and selectivity of synaptic strengthening or weakening as defined by behaviorally relevant stimulus processing. Plausibly, EphA function may not influence which synaptic connections become strengthened or weakened but may facilitate the ensuing microstructural changes only after proper behavioral or electrophysiological stimulation has occurred. Although speculative at this point, if the working hypothesis that chronic facilitation of EphA function primes synapses for synapse-specific use-dependent modification is correct (for a conceptually similar hypothesis, see Abraham and Tate, 1997), EphA receptors may become an important therapeutic target for a number of human conditions in which cognitive function is affected.
Regardless of the precise identity of the molecular and neurobiological mechanisms, the present findings now have revealed a role for EphA receptor tyrosine kinases in cognitive function in the adult mammalian brain. These findings open an unexpected avenue into the functional analysis of this large receptor protein family and may lead to novel targets for therapeutic intervention in human brain and behavioral disorders.
Footnotes
We thank Nerrisa Mendoza and Alisha Eisert for technical help; Louis Tamayo and Allison Bruce for figures; Steven Chamow for CD4-IgG; and Ingrid Caras, Nicola Clayton, Wim Crusio, and David Shelton for discussions on earlier versions of this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to R. Gerlai, Mailstop #72, Room 10–413, 1 DNA Way, South San Francisco, CA 94080. E-mail:gerlai{at}gene.com.