Abstract
A retinal ganglion cell commonly expresses two spatially overlapping receptive field mechanisms. One is the familiar “center/surround,” which sums excitation and inhibition across a region somewhat broader than the ganglion cell's dendritic field. This mechanism responds to a drifting grating by modulating firing at the drift frequency (linear response). Less familiar is the “nonlinear” mechanism, which sums the rectified output of many small subunits that extend for millimeters beyond the dendritic field. This mechanism responds to a contrast-reversing grating by modulating firing at twice the reversal frequency (nonlinear response). We investigated this nonlinear mechanism by presenting visual stimuli to the intact guinea pig retina in vitro while recording intracellularly from large brisk and sluggish ganglion cells. A contrast-reversing grating modulated the membrane potential (in addition to the firing rate) at twice the reversal frequency. This response was initially hyperpolarizing for some cells (either ON or OFF center) and initially depolarizing for others. Experiments in which responses to bars were summed in-phase or out-of-phase suggested that the single class of bipolar cells (either ON or OFF) that drives the center/surround response also drives the nonlinear response. Consistent with this, nonlinear responses persisted in OFF ganglion cells when ON bipolar cell responses were blocked by l-AP-4. Nonlinear responses evoked from millimeters beyond the ganglion cell were eliminated by tetrodotoxin. Thus, to relay the response from distant regions of the receptive field requires a spiking interneuron. Nonlinear responses from different regions of the receptive field added linearly.
- in vitro retina
- guinea pig
- nonlinear subunit
- shift effect
- spiking amacrine cell
- bipolar cell
- tetrodotoxin
- l-AP-4
A retinal ganglion cell encodes information from at least two computational mechanisms. One is familiar, the “linear” receptive field, which computes local temporal contrast by combining excitatory and inhibitory signals over both a narrow region (the “center”) and a wider region (the antagonistic “surround”) (Barlow, 1953; Kuffler, 1953; Rodieck, 1965; Enroth-Cugell and Pinto, 1970). The other mechanism is less familiar, the “nonlinear” receptive field, which computes global changes in contrast magnitude by summing signals from independent regions (“subunits”) (Enroth-Cugell and Robson, 1966; Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Victor and Shapley, 1979a; Cox and Rowe, 1996). The subunit, described in detail for the cat's Y (α) cell, is considered nonlinear because it increases activity to a contrast increment more than it decreases activity to a contrast decrement (or vice versa); in other words, the subunit rectifies its input signal (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Victor, 1988). The subunit covers a region narrower than the ganglion cell's dendritic field, but the mosaic of subunits is much broader, extending for millimeters beyond the dendritic field. When the visual scene contains mostly high spatial frequencies, the nonlinear receptive field can dominate the ganglion cell's output to the brain (Enroth-Cugell and Robson, 1966; Hochstein and Shapley, 1976;Derrington et al., 1979; Victor and Shapley, 1979a).
Although the circuit for the center/surround receptive field is fairly well understood, the circuit for the nonlinear receptive field remains to be elucidated (Wässle and Boycott, 1991; Sterling, 1998). One would like to know: how does the subunit rectify; how does its signal travel millimeters across the retina; and how do signals from multiple subunits combine at the ganglion cell? To answer these questions, we recorded intracellularly from ganglion cells in the intact guinea pig retina in vitro. There, we could apply antagonists to transmitter receptors and ion channels to manipulate specific aspects of the circuit.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
In vitro retina. Our experiments employed a superfused, flattened preparation of the intact mammalian retina (Jensen, 1991; Dacey and Lee, 1994). A guinea pig (350–700 gm) was anesthetized with ketamine–xylazine and overdosed with pentobarbital. Both eyes were enucleated in room light and placed in oxygenated (95%–5% carboxy mixture) Ames medium (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) with sodium bicarbonate (1.9 gm/l) and glucose (0.8 gm/l). Each eye was hemisected, and the anterior half (cornea, lens, and vitreous) was gently peeled away from the posterior eyecup. The retina, with pigment epithelium, choroid, and sclera still attached, was flattened by cutting five to six radial slits and applied scleral side down to filter paper. The retina was placed in a chamber on the stage of an upright microscope and superfused (2–3 ml/min) with oxygenated Ames medium at 34°C. Drugs dissolved in superfusate were kept in reservoirs connected by valves to the chamber. Agents used were tetrodotoxin (TTX) (Sigma) andl-2-amino-4-phosphonobutyric acid (l-AP-4) (Research Biochemicals, Natick, MA). Glass electrodes (tip resistance of 150–400M[scap]Ω) were filled with 1% pyranine (Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR) to visualize the pipette tip and 2% Neurobiotin (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) in 1m KCl buffered with 0.1 mTris, pH 7.4. In some experiments, lidocaine N-ethyl bromide (QX-314) (Research Biochemicals) was added to the pipette solution.
Intracellular recording. To visualize ganglion cells, 5–10 drops of acridine orange (0.001%; Molecular Probes) were added to the superfusate. Dye accumulated in ganglion cell somas and fluoresced to near-UV light (400–440 nm) from a 50 W mercury arc lamp transmitted through the microscope's 40× objective. Large somas (15–25 μm in diameter) in the visual streak were selected for intracellular recording. The membrane potential was amplified (NeuroData IR-283; NeuroData Instruments Corp., Delaware Water Gap, PA), continuously sampled at 2 kHz, and stored on computer (AxoScope software; Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Following recording, Neurobiotin was injected (+0.5 nA with 50% duty cycle, 3–10 min).
The retina was fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 mphosphate buffer (PB), pH 7.4, for 45–60 min at room temperature and then stored in PB overnight at 4°C. To visualize the filled cells, the retina was isolated and reacted for streptavidin-CY3 at room temperature: 1 hr in 6% normal goat serum (NGS), 1% Triton X-100 (TX), and 0.5% DMSO in 0.05 m Tris-buffered saline (TBS); 2 hr in 0.2% streptavidin-CY3, 3% NGS, 1% TX, and 0.5% DMSO in 0.05m TBS; and rinsed for 30 min in 0.05 m TBS. The retina was mounted in Vectashield, and cells were visualized with fluorescence microscopy.
Stimuli. Cells were classified as ON or OFF center using spots and annuli, and then the nonlinear receptive field was probed using gratings. Sine wave or square wave gratings of various spatial frequencies drifted or contrast-reversed at 2 Hz. Stimuli were defined in terms of Michelson contrast: (Imax−Imin)/(Imax+ Imin), whereImax andImin are the peak and trough intensities. Thus, the mean intensity stayed constant over time, and stimulus intensity varied around the mean with a maximum possible contrast of 100%. We programmed the stimuli in Matlab (MathWorks, Natick, MA), using extensions provided by the high-level Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997) and the low-level Video Toolbox (Pelli, 1997).
The stimulus was displayed on a 1-inch-diameter computer monitor with green (P43) phosphor (Lucivid MR1–103; MicroBrightField, Colchester, VT), projected through the top port of the microscope and focused onto the retina with a 2.5× objective. The mean intensity of the stimulus was 28 nW/mm2 at 545 nm light. Given the peak sensitivity of M cones, which predominate in the guinea pig visual streak (530 nm) (Jacobs and Deegan, 1994; Rohlich et al., 1994), this translates to ∼106isomerizations per cone per second. The monitor resolution was 640 × 480 pixels with 60 Hz vertical refresh; stimuli were confined to a square region of 430 pixels on a side (3.7 mm on the retina). The relationship between voltage and monitor intensity was linearized in the software with a lookup table.
We measured the optical line spread at the plane of the retina. A bright edge was stepped across a 200-μm-diameter aperture mounted on a radiometer (IL1400A; International Light Inc., Newburyport, MA). The measured relative intensity at each position was fit by the expected relative intensity convolved with a gaussian with SD of 19 μm (full width at half height of 40 μm).
Data analysis. Data were analyzed with programs written in Matlab. Spikes were detected off-line by analyzing the first derivative of the membrane potential response and finding points above a threshold. Poststimulus time histograms were accumulated across 20 stimulus cycles (bin width of 16.7 msec). To analyze changes in the membrane potential, spikes were removed by linear interpolation of the voltage trace from 5 msec before each spike to 8–13 msec after each spike. This did not affect the subsequent Fourier analysis at the low stimulus temporal frequency. The average membrane potential was analyzed across 20 stimulus cycles (Fig.1C). To quantify the signal, we measured amplitude at the stimulus frequency, Fourier F1 component (2 Hz), and twice the stimulus frequency, Fourier F2 component (4 Hz).
RESULTS
Fifty ganglion cells were studied, mostly in the visual streak (Fig. 1A). Somas were 15–25 μm in diameter with monostratified dendritic fields spanning 350–700 μm; the tracer-filled axons could be followed toward the optic disk (Fig.1B). Most cells were OFF center (n = 42), depolarizing when a small spot dimmed over the dendritic field. The population included both “brisk” cells whose depolarizations peaked in 50–150 msec, and “sluggish” cells whose depolarizations peaked in 200–250 msec (Cleland and Levick, 1974). All cells exhibited nonlinear responses to contrast-reversing gratings (i.e., a dominant F2 response component), and so none were homologous to linear X cells in cat retina. Because drug effects were similar in brisk and sluggish cells, the results have been combined in the population analyses.
A cell was considered healthy as long as the membrane potential (Em) was more negative than −45 mV and stable. The average resting potential was −54 ± 8 mV (mean ± SD), and it often held steady for 0.5–4 hr. Nearly half of the cells lasted for >1 hr. Resting spike rates averaged 12 ± 11 spikes/sec. The most stable recordings gave slightly higher spontaneous rates (15 ± 7 spikes/sec; n = 11) and maximal evoked responses of 119 ± 48 spikes/sec. The guinea pig ganglion cells in our experiments fired spontaneously at the same rate as cat Y cells and gave evoked responses of similar magnitude (Troy and Robson, 1992). This seemed remarkable given that the cat recordings were made extracellularly in the intact animal; whereas the present cells were penetrated by a sharp electrode in a flattened retina bathed in artificial medium.
Linear and nonlinear responses are represented in the membrane potential
Our first finding was that guinea pig retina contains ganglion cells that express both linear and nonlinear responses (Figs.2-5). The linear response was evoked by a drifting grating, which strongly modulated the membrane potential at the drift rate, producing a large amplitude at the stimulus frequency (Fourier F1component) (Figs. 1C, 2). The nonlinear response was evoked by a contrast-reversing grating, which strongly modulated the membrane potential at twice the reversal rate, producing a large amplitude at twice the stimulus frequency (Fourier F2component) (Fig. 2). This distinction between the linear and nonlinear responses has been thoroughly described for Y cells and nonlinear W cells in cat (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Troy et al., 1989,1995; Rowe and Cox, 1993) and for “Y-like” cells in monkey (Kaplan and Shapley, 1982).
The specific properties of the F1 and F2 response components observed in the spike train were clearly evident in the membrane potential. Thus, the F1 component was sensitive to the spatial position of a contrast-reversing grating and was absent at certain positions (“null phases”); whereas the F2component was similar at all grating positions (Fig.3) (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976). We measured the ratio of the average F2 component to the maximal F1 component in response to a contrast-reversing grating of high spatial frequency at several grating positions (n = 12, 2 ON, 10 OFF). Across cells, the F2/F1 component ratio was similar for the membrane potential response (ratio, 2.0 ± 1.0) and the spike response (ratio, 2.2 ± 1.4). This ratio is similar to that reported for Y cells and Y-like cells (Enroth-Cugell and Robson, 1966; Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Kaplan and Shapley, 1982).
The membrane potential's F1 response component was maximal to a coarse contrast-reversing grating (bar width approximately equal to dendritic field width); whereas the F2 response component was maximal to a fine contrast-reversing grating (approximately one-twentieth of the dendritic field width) (Figs. 3, 4) (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976). The membrane potential also displayed the expected relative F1 and F2 response components to both central and peripheral contrast-reversing gratings. Thus, the F1 component was maximal to a central, coarse stimulus; whereas the F2 component was maximal to a fine stimulus in both center and periphery (Fig. 5) (Derrington et al., 1979). Because all key features of the F1and F2 response components are represented in the membrane potential, we could measure them when the ganglion cell spikes were blocked.
Nonlinear response to a peripheral grating can be initially depolarizing or hyperpolarizing
The nonlinear response measured in the spike train to a peripheral contrast-reversing grating was generally considered an excitatory response, i.e., firing above the background rate (Kruger and Fischer, 1973; Derrington et al., 1979) (but see Fischer et al., 1975; Watanabe and Tasaki, 1980). However, the responses measured in the membrane potential demonstrated two distinct patterns. As expected, some cells (14 of 41) initially depolarized 50–100 msec after each contrast reversal of a peripheral grating, and this increased spiking above the mean level (Fig. 6, left columns). However, most cells (27 of 41) initially hyperpolarized with a sharp transient 50–100 msec after each contrast reversal that suppressed spiking. This was followed by depolarization that drove spiking above the mean rate (Fig. 6, right columns). This grouping, based on positive or negative changes following contrast reversal, necessarily divides the cells into two groups, but this separation may be meaningful because l-AP-4 affected the two cell groups differently (see below).
The two patterns of response to a peripheral contrast-reversing grating did not correspond to whether the ganglion cell was ON versus OFF center. For example, an OFF cell could display either one of the two response patterns (Fig. 6, top row). However, there was some relationship between the response to a peripheral grating and the time course of the center response. Thus, most OFF cells with an initially depolarizing response were sluggish (7of 9), whereas, most OFF cells with an initially hyperpolarizing response were brisk (14 of 20).
The nonlinear response measured to a central contrast-reversing grating [500 μm outer diameter (OD)] was typically biphasic. The response could be initially hyperpolarizing then depolarizing, or vice versa, and its waveform varied markedly across cells. A qualitative grouping suggested four to five types of waveform (n = 15), but a detailed classification remains to be done. For the current study, however, the drug effects on the nonlinear response to a central grating were similar across cells, and so they have been combined in the population analyses.
Evidence that a single class of cone bipolar cell can generate the nonlinear response
The ganglion cells studied here are monostratified, branching in either the inner or outer strata of the inner plexiform layer. Thus, each receives synapses from a single class of cone bipolar cell (ON or OFF), which can generate the ganglion cell's classical center/surround response (Wässle and Boycott, 1991; Sterling, 1998). A center spot (bright for ON ganglion cells, dim for OFF ganglion cells) would increase the bipolar cell's glutamate release, whereas an annulus (bright for ON ganglion cells, dim for OFF ganglion cells) would decrease the bipolar cell's glutamate release. A monostratified amacrine cell's contribution to the surround response would also be driven by the same class of bipolar cell. Physiological evidence for this model comes from measurements of ganglion cell center/surround responses while blocking ON bipolar responses with l-AP-4 (Shiells et al., 1981; Slaughter and Miller, 1981). Both the center response to a spot and the surround response to an annulus were blocked by l-AP-4 in ON ganglion cells but not in OFF ganglion cells (Schiller, 1982; Knapp and Mistler, 1983; Bolz et al., 1984).
Might the ganglion cell's nonlinear response also arise from a single class of bipolar cell (ON or OFF)? To investigate this, we presented two sets of bars that reversed contrast over time (i.e., black ← → white). Each set of bars occupied half the area of the receptive field center, and they were spatially complementary. Combined in-phase, they created a spot and combined out-of-phase, they created a contrast-reversing grating (Fig. 7A). The responses to the two sets of bars when summed in-phase matched the shape of the response to a spot and nearly matched the amplitude. Presumably, the match arises because both a spot and the complementary sets of bars excite the same OFF bipolar cells. The responses to the two sets of bars when summed out-of-phase matched the shape of the response to a contrast-reversing grating and most of the amplitude. In short, over the dendritic field, both the center response to the spot and the nonlinear response to the contrast-reversing grating could be predicted simply by summing the responses elicited by the same sets of bars.
We performed a similar experiment in the periphery. Two complementary sets of contrast-reversing bars were presented that combined in-phase to create an annulus and combined out-of-phase to create a peripheral contrast-reversing grating (Fig. 7B). The responses to the two sets of bars when summed in-phase matched the shape of the response to an annulus. Presumably, the match arises because both an annulus and the complementary sets of bars inhibit the same OFF bipolar cells. The responses to the two sets of bars when summed out-of-phase matched the shape of the response to a peripheral contrast-reversing grating. In short, beyond the dendritic field, both the surround response to the annulus and the nonlinear response to the contrast-reversing grating could be predicted simply by summing the responses elicited by the same sets of bars.
This result, that the summation of bar responses could predict both center/surround responses to a spot/annulus and nonlinear responses to a grating in both center and periphery held for all cells studied (seven OFF, one ON), including both sluggish and brisk (Fig. 7). That the same component responses, added in-phase or out-of-phase, could predict both center/surround and nonlinear responses, suggested that the input driving the two mechanisms is the same. Because the center/surround is driven by a single class of bipolar cell (ON or OFF), it follows that the nonlinear mechanism is driven by the same single class of bipolar cell.
l-AP-4 does not reduce the nonlinear response in OFF ganglion cells
If the nonlinear response in an OFF center ganglion cell were driven solely by OFF bipolar cells, the response should be undiminished when ON bipolar cell light responses are blocked by l-AP-4 (Shiells et al., 1981; Slaughter and Miller, 1981; Nawy and Jahr, 1990). To test this, we applied l-AP-4 (10 μm, n = 2; 40 μm,n = 12). An ON ganglion cell's light responses were blocked in 20 sec, indicating full block of ON cone bipolar cells (Fig.8A). In OFF ganglion cells, the resting potential was unchanged (initial, −51 ± 9mV;l-AP-4, −50 ± 11 mV; n = 14), but to a central contrast-reversing grating, the average F2 response component increased by fourfold (4.2 ± 2.9) (Fig. 8). When the drug was washed out, the F2 response component declined to 2.1 ± 1.1 times the initial level (Fig. 8B). To a peripheral contrast-reversing grating, the average F2response component in cells with an initially hyperpolarizing response increased in the presence of l-AP-4 by fourfold at the three highest contrasts (4.3 ± 3.1 times initial) (Fig.9). To the same stimulus, the average F2 response component in cells with initially depolarizing responses decreased ∼25% in the presence ofl-AP-4 at the three highest contrasts (0.75 ± 0.76 times wash) (Fig. 9), but responses were significantly lower only at 12.5 and 25% contrast (t = 2.47 and 2.51; bothp < 0.05; one-tailed t test; df = 4). In these cells, the F2 component in the response to a central grating was not affected byl-AP-4.
On the whole, the nonlinear response in OFF ganglion cells is not blocked by l-AR-4's blocking of ON bipolar cells. For most cells and in most conditions, the nonlinear response is enhanced, possibly because of a general reduction of inhibition attributable to l-AP-4 effects on type III metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluR) that distribute widely on amacrine processes (Hartveit et al., 1995; Koulen et al., 1996). This agreement between the bar summation experiment and thel-AP-4 effects suggests that nonlinear responses arise from a single class of bipolar cell.
In those cells with an initially depolarizing response to a peripheral contrast-reversing grating, l-AP-4 significantly reduced responses at low contrast. This might suggest that ON bipolar cells contribute to these responses at low contrast. However, it seems more plausible that this effect could also arise from the effect ofl-AP-4 on amacrine cell type III mGluRs. In Figure 7, both cells displayed a depolarizing response to a peripheral grating, which could be predicted in the bar summation experiment. Therefore, it seems most likely that, even in these cells, the nonlinear response to a peripheral grating is driven by the same single class of bipolar cells that drives the center/surround.
Nonlinear response to a peripheral grating requires action potentials
The nonlinear response to a peripheral, contrast-reversing grating is relayed to a ganglion cell over more than a millimeter. To test whether action potentials are required, we evoked this nonlinear response while applying TTX (100 nm). Approximately 20 sec after TTX reached the retina, spiking ceased in the ganglion cell. Although the resting potential changed only slightly (initial, −57 ± 9mV; TTX, −60 ± 8mV; n = 15), the F2 response component was abolished. This was true both in cells with initially hyperpolarizing and initially depolarizing responses (Fig. 10). In the presence of TTX, F2 response components at the three highest contrasts decreased to 0.28 ± 0.49 times the initial level and were indistinguishable from noise (0% contrast response) (Fig. 10). After ∼5 min of wash, spiking returned, and both the resting potential (−57 ± 12 mV; n = 13) and the response to the peripheral grating returned to initial levels. After TTX washed out, F2 response components at the three highest contrasts returned to 1.4 ± 1.1 times the initial level and were clearly above the noise (Fig. 10).
We considered whether TTX abolished the response to a peripheral grating by blocking spikes in the recorded ganglion cell or spikes in retinal interneurons. To test this, we included lidocaineN-ethyl bromide (QX-314; 25–50 mm) in the electrode to block spikes from inside the recorded ganglion cell. The response to a peripheral grating persisted but was then abolished by TTX (Fig. 10B). Thus, lateral relay of the nonlinear response from the periphery requires action potentials in retinal interneurons.
TTX did not abolish but rather increased the response to a central contrast-reversing grating (n = 2) (Fig.11A). Because TTX abolished the response to a peripheral grating and enhanced the response to a central grating by an equal amount, the response to simultaneous stimulation by a full-field grating was primarily unaffected (Fig. 11B). The F2response component increased to 1.4 ± 1.4 times the initial level in the presence of TTX and remained at 1.6 ± 1.5 times the initial level during the wash. The F1 response component to these stimuli was small because the grating was fine, but it did also increase to 1.3 ± 1.2 times the initial level in the presence of TTX and remained at 1.4 ± 1.2 times the initial level during the wash (n = 9).
Nonlinear response sums linearly at the ganglion cell
The F2 response component to a contrast-reversing grating in the receptive field center was much stronger than the F2 response component to the same grating in the periphery (Fig.12A). However, the response to a full-field grating was less than the sum of the response amplitudes to the gratings presented in concentric rings (Fig.12A). This was because the responses were slightly out-of-phase. When responses were summed (taking into account phase, as well as amplitude), the result equaled the response to a full-field grating. This result, shown for a particular cell in Figure12B, was true for most cells and can be seen in the average response of the population (Fig. 12C). Thus, spatial summation of the ganglion cell's nonlinear responses is linear.
DISCUSSION
We can now address the questions raised in the introductory remarks concerning the ganglion cell's nonlinear receptive field: how the subunit rectifies, how its signal travels millimeters across the retina, and how signals from multiple subunits combine at the ganglion cell.
Model for the nonlinear circuit
Figure 13 suggests a working model for the nonlinear receptive field. When a grating reverses contrast, cones under a dimming bar depolarize, and cones under a brightening bar hyperpolarize. Consequently, an OFF bipolar cell under the dimming bar releases more transmitter, and an OFF bipolar cell under the brightening bar releases less transmitter. A wide-field amacrine cell costratifying with the OFF bipolar synaptic terminals is depolarized by the first bipolar cell but not equivalently hyperpolarized by the second bipolar cell. This nonlinearity is assumed to arise at the bipolar–amacrine synapse (see below). The nonlinearity is then transmitted via the spiking amacrine cell to the ganglion cell and/or its presynaptic bipolar cell. The spiking amacrine cell probably releases GABA (Vaney, 1990) and would thus initially hyperpolarize the ganglion cell at each contrast reversal. A similar mechanism could explain the initially depolarizing response to a peripheral grating if a local, inhibitory amacrine synapse were interposed between the spiking amacrine cell and the ganglion cell. The model would work equally well for ON ganglion cells driven by ON bipolar and amacrine cells.
This model implies that the fine subunits comprising the nonlinear receptive field correspond to the bipolar cell receptive field (Victor and Shapley, 1979b). The subunits resolve a grating at least 10-fold finer than the ganglion cell dendritic field (Figs. 4, 5). Each subunit would be ∼50 μm in diameter, approximately the size of a bipolar cell receptive field center (Nelson and Kolb, 1983; Cohen and Sterling, 1992; Sterling, 1998). Also, the subunit's extent, like that of the bipolar cell center, is approximately constant with eccentricity (Figs.4, 5) (Derrington et al., 1979). Finally, the same bipolar cell that drives the nonlinear subunit apparently also drives the classical center/surround. This is supported by Figure 7, which shows that the same component responses can predict both center/surround and nonlinear receptive field responses.
The subunit's underlying nonlinearity might well arise at the synaptic output of a specific category of cone bipolar cell (Figs. 8, 9). An OFF bipolar cell of this category would strongly increase its transmitter release to light offset (sluggishly or briskly) and weakly decrease transmitter release to light onset. This asymmetry is equivalent to “half-wave rectification.” When a ganglion cell sums two such responses out-of-phase, it gives a characteristic frequency-doubled response (Fig. 7). The cat b1 bipolar cell, presynaptic to the Y cell, provides an example of this behavior. Its release rate is low during steady light (∼1 vesicle per synapse/sec), so light onset can evoke a large increment in transmitter release, but because of the low sustained rate, light offset cannot cause a comparable decrement (Freed, 1993; M. Freed, unpublished observations). Alternatively, the proposed mechanism of half-wave rectification via low basal release could apply to elements downstream from the bipolar cell.
Signals from the periphery of the nonlinear receptive field almost certainly reach the ganglion cell via a spiking interneuron. These signals travel at ∼0.34 m/sec, consistent with a spiking mechanism (Fischer et al., 1975; Derrington et al., 1979). Furthermore, the nonlinear response to a peripheral grating was abolished by tetrodotoxin (Fig. 10). These signals could be transmitted by an amacrine cell with multiple axons that extend for millimeters across the retina (Vaney et al., 1988; Dacey, 1989; Famiglietti, 1992a-c;Bloomfield, 1996; Freed et al., 1996; Stafford and Dacey, 1997). The guinea pig retina contains such amacrine cells with axons that extend up to 3 mm (Kao et al., 1999).
It is notable that the nonlinear receptive field, which extends for millimeters, is summed linearly at the ganglion cell (Fig. 12). This linear summation was not tested previously over such a wide region, but it is consistent with the original model of the nonlinear subunits (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Victor and Shapley, 1979b). It implies that the subunits operate independently and therefore may not interact synaptically.
Does the nonlinear receptive field extend beyond the classical surround?
In cat Y cells, the nonlinear receptive field was initially described as extending beyond the classical surround (Fischer et al., 1975; Derrington et al., 1979). However, it now appears that the surround extends further than previously thought, ∼2 mm retinal distance from the receptive field center (Troy et al., 1993). Thus, in the cat Y cell, the nonlinear receptive field, and the classical surround are primarily coextensive. In the present experiment, the nonlinear receptive field in brisk and sluggish guinea pig ganglion cells was also coextensive with the classical surround (Fig.7B).
Most ganglion cell types express a nonlinear receptive field
The nonlinear receptive field seems to be expressed by most ganglion cell types in all mammalian species. In guinea pig, all cells we have studied so far (∼7 wide-field types) express a nonlinear receptive field (Sterling et al., 1999). In cat retina, Y and W cells express a nonlinear receptive field (Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Troy et al., 1989; Rowe and Cox, 1993; Pu et al., 1994; Troy et al., 1995), and even X cells, generally considered to be linear, express nonlinear responses from the periphery (Barlow et al., 1977; Hamasaki and Maguire, 1985). Furthermore, nonlinear receptive fields are expressed by ganglion cell types in rabbit (Caldwell and Daw, 1978; Watanabe and Tasaki, 1980), mouse (Stone and Pinto, 1993), and monkey (Kruger et al., 1975; Kaplan and Shapley, 1982). In monkey retina, there may be certain cell types that are completely linear and do not, under any condition, express a nonlinear receptive field (Kaplan and Shapley, 1982; Benardete et al., 1992). However, at least one class of neurons in the magnocellular layer of the lateral geniculate nucleus has a local nonlinear receptive field (Kaplan and Shapley, 1982; Benardete et al., 1992), and a larger percentage may show a peripheral nonlinear receptive field (Kruger, 1977).
Function of the nonlinear receptive field for vision
Although nonlinear responses were observed long ago, they were described as mere “effects” (the “McIlwain,” “periphery,” or “shift” effect), and only later were these related to the Y cell nonlinear subunit (McIlwain, 1964, 1966; Fischer et al., 1975;Derrington et al., 1979). Yet we are impressed that these responses are not oddities but reflect powerful circuits for computing contrast magnitude over a wide region. The ganglion cell might use this information to tune its linear receptive field. For example, when the nonlinear receptive field is stimulated continuously with a fine, drifting grating, the gain of the linear center is sharply reduced (Werblin, 1972; Caldwell and Daw, 1978; Enroth-Cugell and Jakiela, 1980). Alternatively, when the peripheral nonlinear receptive field is stimulated, the linear center of certain cells may be enhanced (McIlwain, 1964). This gain control might serve psychophysical “masking” whereby the ability to detect a small spot is modulated by surrounding stimuli (Derrington, 1984; He and Loop, 1990; Fuhr and Kuyk, 1998). However, the nonlinear receptive field may have other functions. For example, the nonlinear receptive field is still expressed by geniculate neurons, so it must be relayed to cortex where it might carry a message complementary to that of the linear receptive field (So and Shapley, 1979; cf. Spitzer and Hochstein, 1987).
It is a matter of great current interest that a nonlinear mechanism in the cortex computes “second order” contrast boundaries. In a scene where average luminance stays constant over space, object boundaries are determined by changes in local contrast on a fine scale. Such contrast boundaries are invisible to a linear mechanism that computes only “first order” luminance boundaries on a coarse scale (Mareschal and Baker, 1998, 1999). Shown psychophysically, this nonlinear mechanism was composed of fine subunits and insensitive to orientation (McGraw et al., 1999). Most studies have assumed a cortical mechanism. However, the ganglion cell nonlinear receptive field might also contribute to this visual computation.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health, National Eye Institute Grants F32-EY06850 (J.B.D.), T32-EY07131 (L.H.), EY11138 (M.A.F.), and EY00828 (P.S.).
We thank Robert Smith and Yen-Hong Kao for technical advice, Madeleine Johnson for technical assistance, and Sharron Fina for help in preparing this manuscript.
Drs. Demb and Haarsma contributed equally to this work.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jonathan B. Demb, Department of Neuroscience, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, 123 Anatomy/Chemistry Building, Philadelphia, PA 19104-6058. E-mail:demb{at}retina.anatomy.upenn.edu.