Abstract
We have used aspiration and electrolytic lesions to investigate the contributions of cerebellar cortex to the acquisition and expression of conditioned eyelid responses. We show that lesions of the anterior lobe of rabbit cerebellar cortex disrupt the timing of previously learned conditioned eyelid responses. These short-latency responses were used as an indication that the cerebellar cortex was sufficiently damaged and that the underlying pathways necessary for the expression of responses were sufficiently intact to support responses. Rabbits were subsequently trained for 15 daily sessions using a new conditioned stimulus. Whereas rabbits in which lesions had no significant effect on response timing showed rapid acquisition of appropriately timed eyelid responses to the new conditioned stimulus, animals with lesions that disrupt timing showed no significant increases in either amplitude or probability of responses. Histological analysis suggests that damage to the anterior lobe of the cerebellar cortex is necessary and sufficient to abolish timing and prevent acquisition. These data indicate that the cerebellar cortex is necessary for the acquisition of conditioned eyelid responses and are consistent with the hypotheses that (1) eyelid conditioning results in plasticity in both the anterior lobe of the cerebellar cortex and in the anterior interpositus nucleus and (2) induction of plasticity in the interpositus requires intact input from the cerebellar cortex.
- Pavlovian eyelid conditioning
- classical eyelid conditioning
- nictitating membrane
- LTP
- LTD
- motor learning
- extinction
- timing
- deep cerebellar nucleus
Since the publication of Marr's (1969) seminal theory, the role of the cerebellar cortex in motor learning has been intensely debated. Although evidence indicates that Pavlovian eyelid conditioning is one of several examples of motor learning mediated by the cerebellum (Ritchie, 1976; Robinson, 1976;Gilbert and Thach, 1977; Thach, 1980; Thompson, 1986; Lisberger, 1988;Robinson et al., 1993; Thompson and Krupa, 1994), the relative contributions of cerebellar cortex and cerebellar nuclei are a source of controversy (McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; Yeo et al., 1985a;Lisberger and Sejnowski, 1992; Ito, 1993; Mauk, 1997). Here we address the necessity of the cerebellar cortex for the acquisition of conditioned eyelid responses. Previous studies indicate that presentation of the conditioned stimulus (CS) is conveyed to the cerebellum via mossy fiber afferents (Steinmetz et al., 1986, 1987,1989), the reinforcing properties of the unconditioned stimulus (US) require activation of climbing fiber afferents (McCormick et al., 1985;Mauk et al., 1986; Yeo et al., 1986), and activation of the appropriate neurons in the anterior interpositus nucleus drives the expression of conditioned responses (McCormick and Thompson, 1984a,b; Chapman et al., 1988). There are two cerebellar pathways by which CS-activated mossy fibers could come to activate interpositus neurons during training—the excitatory mossy fiber synapses directly onto the output neurons and the more complex pathway through the cerebellar cortex (Fig.1A). Evidence suggests that both pathways can undergo plasticity during eyelid conditioning (McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; Krupa et al., 1993; Perrett et al., 1993; Hesslow and Ivarsson, 1994; Tracy et al., 1998) and during adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) (Miles and Lisberger, 1981; Ito, 1993; Lisberger and Raymond, 1996; Raymond et al., 1996). In essence, our experiments ask whether the direct pathway through the cerebellar nucleus can support eyelid conditioning without the cerebellar cortex (Fig. 1B).
Interpreting the effects of cerebellar cortex lesions on the acquisition of conditioned eyelid responses requires assurance that a lesion has damaged appropriate regions of cortex but has spared underlying pathways necessary for response expression. For example, in previous studies reporting normal or only partially compromised acquisition, it is not clear that relevant regions of the cerebellar cortex were damaged (McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; Lavond et al., 1987; Lavond and Steinmetz, 1989; Yeo and Hardiman, 1992; Harvey et al., 1993; Clark and Lavond, 1994; Gruart and Yeo, 1995). Similarly, in studies showing abolished eyelid response acquisition, the functional integrity of the interpositus nucleus was unclear (Yeo et al., 1984,1985a). Here, we attempt to avoid these shortcomings by using the effects of cerebellar cortex removal on the expression of conditioned responses as an assay for the effectiveness and specificity of the lesions. We have shown previously that either blockade of cerebellar cortex output (Garcia and Mauk, 1998b) or lesions of the cerebellar cortex (Perrett et al., 1993; Perrett and Mauk, 1995) abolish the timing of eyelid responses, yielding responses with short and relatively fixed latencies to onset. Because evidence indicates that the short-latency responses are learned and are abolished by lesions of the interpositus nucleus (Perrett and Mauk, 1995; Garcia and Mauk, 1998a,b), the short-latency responses can be used as a functional readout that a lesion has damaged critical regions of cerebellar cortex without damaging the interpositus nucleus or other pathways necessary for response expression.
We show that when cerebellar cortex lesions meet these criteria, animals are subsequently unable to acquire conditioned eyelid responses using a new conditioned stimulus. The ability of the previously trained CS to elicit responses excludes the possibility that the inability to acquire responses is really a deficit in response expression. In concert with previous work, these findings support the hypothesis that eyelid conditioning involves plasticity in both the cerebellar cortex and nuclei and that the cerebellar cortex is required both for acquisition and extinction of responses as well as for the induction of plasticity in the interpositus nucleus.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals. Data are presented from 14 male New Zealand albino rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), weighing 2.5–3.0 kg each at the start of the experiments. The animals were individually housed and fed food and water ad libitum. Treatment of the animals and surgical procedures were in accordance with an approved animal welfare protocol.
Surgery. All animals were first surgically prepared with a head bolt cemented to the skull. Of these, 2 animals were also prepared with electrodes implanted in the anterior lobe of the cerebellar cortex, and the remaining 12 animals later received aspiration lesions after initial training.
Surgical procedures for aspiration lesions were identical to those described previously (Perrett and Mauk, 1995). For the electrolytic lesion animals, the following procedures were used. A large craniotomy was drilled to accommodate the electrode assembly or a cannula and covered with bone wax. The electrode assembly consisted of an array of four stainless steel electrodes equally spaced in a linear strip ∼3 mm in length. The distal 1 mm of each electrode was exposed by scraping off the insulation with a scalpel blade. The head of the rabbit was positioned with lambda 1.5 mm ventral to bregma. The array was placed in a parasagittal orientation. The anteriormost electrode was placed at stereotaxic coordinates corresponding to the lateral anterior lobe (5.7 mm anterior, 4.9 mm left lateral, and 14.0 mm ventral to lambda). After placement, the electrode assembly and head bolt were secured to the skull with dental acrylic, and the skin was sutured. Two stainless steel stimulating electrodes were chronically implanted in the periorbital muscles rostral and caudal to the eye. Antibiotics, intravenous fluids, and analgesics were administered after surgery as needed, and animals were allowed ∼1 week to recover.
Conditioning procedures. The standard training session involved a Pavlovian conditioning delay protocol with a 500 msec interstimulus interval. Each training session consisted of 12 nine-trial blocks. Each block was comprised of eight paired presentations of the CS and US and one presentation of the CS only. The CS (a 1 kHz, 85 dB tone or a vibrator secured to the animal's back) was presented for 550 msec during CS-alone trials and coterminated with a 50 msec train of constant-current pulses (200 Hz; 1 msec pulse width; 2–3 mA) delivered to the periorbital electrodes during paired trials. The intensity of the US was adjusted individually for each animal to produce a robust unconditioned response without overt signs of discomfort. Trials were separated by a fixed 30 sec intertrial interval.
Animals were conditioned to an asymptotic rate of responding with at least 10 standard training sessions. Lesions were then made either by passing 2.0 mA of direct current through one or more of the electrodes for ∼1 min or by surgical aspiration as described previously.
In these animals the effect of lesions on timing was assessed with two postlesion training sessions. Animals with lesions then received 15 d of postlesion acquisition training, each consisting of 12 10-trial blocks. Each block was of eight paired presentations of the novel CS with the US and one presentation each of both the pre- and postlesion CS alone. The tone and vibrator were counterbalanced with regard to pre- and postlesion training. Finally, animals that failed to acquire responses after the lesion received 2 d of training the contralateral eyelid to the postlesion CS.
Stimulus presentation and data acquisition were computer-controlled using custom software. Movement of the unrestrained eyelid was recorded by measuring the reflectance of an infrared light-emitting diode aimed at the eyelid. Voltage responses were determined to be linearly related to eyelid movement and were calibrated for each animal daily. Digitized responses (1 point per msec; 12 bit resolution) were analyzed using custom software to determine onset and peak latencies.
Histology. After training, the location of the lesion was determined for each animal using standard histological procedures. Animals were killed with an overdose of sodium pentobarbital and perfused intracardially with 1.0 l of 10% formalin. The brains were removed and stored in 10% formalin for several days. Brains were embedded in an albumin gelatin mixture. To assess the extent of aspiration or electrolytic lesions, we sectioned the cerebellum parasagittally using a freezing microtome (80 μm sections). This plane of sectioning best illustrated the degree to which tissue damage involved each lobule of cerebellar cortex.
Data analysis and statistical tests. Peak response amplitude and onset latency were calculated by custom software. Digitized sweeps corresponded to the 200 msec before and 2300 msec after the CS onset. After calibration, peak amplitude was measured relative to an average of the 200 msec baseline collected before CS onset. Onset latency was determined by calculating the point at which the response slope deviated by two SDs from the average slope of the baseline. To be counted as a conditioned response, onset latency had to follow CS onset, and the amplitude was required to reach 0.3 mm before US onset during paired trials. This criterion was relaxed for CS-alone trials in which movements were counted as conditioned responses if they reached a 0.3 mm amplitude at any time after CS onset. Trials in which there was >0.3 mm of movement during the baseline were excluded.
Statistical analysis included tests for the effects of the lesions on response timing for each individual animal and tests for group effects of the lesions on acquisition using amplitude and percent response as dependent measures. Animals were separated into “lesion” or “control” groups on the bases of separate one-tailed ttests performed on peak and onset latencies from the last 12 conditioned responses before the lesion and the first 12 conditioned responses after the lesion (p < 0.01 criterion). A two-way, mixed ANOVA was performed on the postlesion response rate to compare groups with regard to acquisition with the novel CS. Response rate data from the lesion group during the 15 postlesion training days were further assessed using a one-way, repeated measures ANOVA. A minimum confidence level ofp < 0.05 was used for these group analyses.
RESULTS
The ability to evaluate the effects of the lesions on a previously trained response is the key feature of the experimental design. Before aspiration or electrolytic lesions of the cerebellar cortex, animals were trained to make robust eyelid responses to either a tone or a tactile vibratory CS applied to the back. After lesions, animals were grouped according to the effects of the lesions on response timing. Animals were assigned to the experimental group only if lesions decreased both peak and onset latencies. Five animals reported in this study demonstrated significant effects (p < 0.001) on both onset and peak latency as determined by separatet tests. Cortex lesions had similar effects on response timing whether the original CS was a 1 kHz tone or the vibratory stimulus. Several animals with cerebellar cortex lesions demonstrated clear effects on the latency to onset but not the latency to response peak. This pattern of results, which appears to result from partial lesions of the anterior lobe, will be reported in a separate study.
To assess postlesion acquisition, animals were then trained to a new CS. Those that were initially trained to the tone received 15 d of training using the vibratory CS, and animals trained to the vibrator before the lesion were trained using the tone CS. We find that animals in which cortex lesions disrupted response timing were unable to acquire either short-latency or timed responses when trained with the new CS, although presentation of the prelesion CS continued to elicit robust short-latency responses (Fig. 2). The animals sometimes displayed small, short-latency responses to the new CS, presumably because of the slight auditory component of the vibratory stimulus. Although these animals show no increases in responding to the new CS, they rapidly acquired well timed responses when the eye contralateral to the lesion was trained. In contrast, when response timing was not affected by the cerebellar cortex lesion, animals rapidly acquired well timed conditioned responses to the novel CS.
The CS-alone trials (original CS) intermixed during the 15 d of postlesion training also permitted evaluation of extinction in the control and lesion animals (Fig. 3). Although extinction in the control animals was modest, presumably owing to the small number of trials presented, the decline in responding was significant. In contrast, in both the amplitude and percent response measures the lesion animals showed no apparent decreases in short-latency responding to the original CS.
Statistical analyses of both amplitude and percent-responding data indicate that the lesions prevented acquisition of responses to the new CS (i.e., Fig. 2). Separate two-way, mixed ANOVAs were performed for both amplitude and percent-responding data. For the new CS data there were main effects of lesion versus control [percent dataF(1,12) = 118.69;p < 0.001; amplitude dataF(1,12) = 233.87; p < 0.001]. There were also significant main effects for days [percent data F(14,168) = 7.85;p < 0.001; amplitude dataF(14,168) = 3.12; p < 0.025]. The interaction effect of lesion by day was significant only for the percent-responding data [percent dataF(14,168) = 4.50; p < 0.005; amplitude data F(14,168) = 1.96; NS]. As an additional post hoc test, one-way repeated measures ANOVAs were calculated separately for data from lesion animals and from control animals. These analyses show no significant change in responding in the lesion group over the 15 d of postlesion training [percent dataF(14,56) = 1.69; NS; amplitude dataF(14,56) = 1.62; NS]. In contrast, similar analysis of the control data showed significant acquisition over 15 d of training [percent dataF(14,112) = 8.79; p < 0.001; amplitude data F(14,112) = 3.38; p < 0.01].
Identical analyses of the original CS data indicate that the lesions of cerebellar cortex prevented extinction of responding to the CS trained before the lesion (i.e., Fig. 3). Separate two-way, mixed ANOVAs (percent and amplitude data) revealed a main effect of the lesion only for the amplitude data [percent dataF(1,12) = 0.04; NS; amplitude dataF(1,12) = 21.61; p < 0.001]. This indicates that the lesions had a significant effect on the amplitude of previously trained responses but not on their likelihood. There were main effects for days [percent dataF(14,168) = 4.03; p < 0.005; amplitude data F(14,168) = 4.19; p < 0.005]. Importantly, for both response measures there was a significant lesion-by-day interaction effect [percent data F(14,168) = 2.73;p < 0.025; amplitude dataF(14,168) = 2.56; p < 0.025]. As additional tests, one-way repeated measures ANOVAs were calculated separately for data from lesion and from control animals. For both response measures the results indicate a significant extinction in control animals [percent dataF(14,112) = 5.65; p < 0.001; amplitude data F(14,112) = 4.86; p < 0.001]. Similar analyses for the lesion animal data showed that there was no significant change in responding to the original CS over the 15 postlesion days [percent dataF(14,56) = 0.60; NS; amplitude dataF(14,56) = 0.41; NS].
Histological reconstruction of the lesions revealed a consistent relationship between damage to cerebellar lobules HIV/V and the effects observed on both timing and acquisition. Large lesions that included most of HVI but did not involve HIV/V neither affected timing nor prevented acquisition. Figure 4depicts the smallest effective and the largest ineffective lesions. All of the effective lesions and none of the ineffective lesions included the lateral aspect of HIV/V. Damage to HVI had no effect on response timing or acquisition. One animal with a small electrolytic lesion illustrated especially clearly the role of the anterior lobe (Fig.5). In this animal, visible damage was limited to the anterior lobe, with no histologically detectable damage to lobule HVI. This animal showed a robust effect on response timing and a complete inability to acquire responses to the new CS. Together these data demonstrate that the anterior lobe of the cerebellar cortex is required for the acquisition and expression of timed responses and plays a critical role in the training-induced plasticity underlying the acquisition of short-latency responses as well.
DISCUSSION
We have demonstrated that lesions of the cerebellar cortex abolish the learned timing of conditioned eyelid responses and prevent acquisition of responses to a new CS. Although extinction in control animals was modest because of the paucity of CS-alone trials, the present data also replicate previous findings that lesions of the cerebellar cortex prevent extinction of previously learned responses. In contrast, large lesions that do not affect the anterior lobe, including those with extensive damage of lobule HVI, neither affect response timing, prevent extinction, nor prevent acquisition. The data also show that these lesion effects are not restricted to auditory stimuli. These data demonstrate that the cerebellar cortex is necessary for the acquisition and extinction of conditioned eyelid responses and suggest that the cerebellar cortex is necessary for the induction of plasticity that mediates short-latency responses.
These findings are consistent with previous observations regarding cerebellar cortex and adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (Raymond and Lisberger, 1996). Cerebellar cortex lesions have been shown to affect only partially the expression of previously learned VOR gain adaptation but to prevent subsequent VOR adaptation (Robinson, 1976). The present data together with the VOR studies suggest a consistent picture in which cerebellar cortex is required for learned increases and decreases (acquisition and extinction) in cerebellar-dependent movements.
Despite this concordance, many previous eyelid conditioning studies contradict this view. McCormick and Thompson (1984a) reported that cerebellar cortex lesions do not affect conditioned response expression. Subsequent studies have reported that cerebellar cortex lesions do not affect expression or extinction and have only marginal effects on the acquisition of responses (McCormick and Thompson, 1984a;Woodruff-Pak et al., 1985, 1993; Lavond et al., 1987; Knowlton et al., 1988; Lavond and Steinmetz, 1989; Yeo and Hardiman, 1992; Harvey et al., 1993; Clark and Lavond, 1994; Logan et al., 1994). However, pharmacological block of cerebellar cortex output, which abolished conditioned response timing, indicates the effects of cerebellar cortex lesions independent of any knowledge of which regions of cortex are important (Garcia and Mauk, 1998b). Thus, a lesion in the region of cerebellar cortex involved in eyelid conditioning should have the same result. We have shown here and previously that lesions of the anterior lobe produce this effect on response timing (Perrett et al., 1993;Perrett and Mauk, 1995). Thus, previous findings that cerebellar cortex lesions did not affect response timing, acquisition, or extinction appear to have been false negatives. Indeed, the present results replicated these negative findings when the lesions did not affect response timing. Similarly, our data suggest that the abolition of conditioned responses by cerebellar cortex lesions reported in other previous studies (Yeo et al., 1984, 1985b; Yeo and Hardiman, 1992) must have been the result of unintended damage to underlying pathways necessary for response expression. Here, by using the status of previously trained responses as an index for the functional extent and specificity of a cerebellar cortex lesion, we can exclude confounds that may have led previously to false positives or negatives.
As with all lesions caution is required regarding secondary effects of these lesions. One such concern relates to the possible contributions of degeneration of CS and US pathways produced by the lesions. Here, degeneration of CS pathways can be excluded by the presence of conditioned responses elicited by the previously trained CS. Degeneration of the US pathway, the climbing fibers, cannot be so easily excluded. It remains possible therefore that the cerebellar nucleus may be capable of learning without the cerebellar cortex. The absence of learning with cerebellar cortex lesions could then result from degeneration of climbing fiber inputs to the nucleus. This issue may require reversible lesion studies to resolve. Another potential concern is abnormal activity of nucleus cells after the lesions, which might prevent learning. However, such activity should be apparent behaviorally, because activation of nucleus cells can elicit eyelid responses in untrained animals. In ongoing reversible lesion studies we sometimes observe tonic partial closure of the eyelid, possibly because of high levels of activity in nucleus cells (unpublished data). No such tonic eyelid closure was observed in the present studies, consistent with an absence of abnormal nucleus cell activity after the lesions.
Our data also seemingly contradict results from eyelid conditioning in mutant mice in which the Purkinje cells undergo degeneration. These Purkinje cell degeneration mice show poor, but not abolished, acquisition at a time when Purkinje cells are almost completely gone (Chen et al., 1996, 1999). Our results provide no explanation for these differences. It is possible that there are enough viable Purkinje cells remaining in these mice to support compromised learning, that there are species differences, or that the mutations cause compensatory changes that allow the cerebellar nuclei or other brain regions to operate abnormally (Steele et al., 1998). Although the present results do not support or deny these possibilities, they do provide assurances that the lesions damaged the anterior lobe enough to affect response timing without significant damage to pathways required for response expression.
Combined with previous studies the present results support the assertions that (1) the cerebellar cortex is not necessary for the expression of previously learned conditioned responses if timing is ignored but that (2) the cerebellar cortex is necessary for the acquisition and extinction of conditioned eyelid responses. Although the effect of cerebellar cortex lesions on the timing of VOR has not been tested, these assertions are otherwise consistent with the effects of cerebellar cortex lesions on adaptation of the VOR. These assertions are also consistent with the contributions to eyelid responses suggested by stimulation and recording studies. Purkinje cell recordings from the region of ferret cerebellar cortex involved in eyelid responses display learned decreases in Purkinje cell activity during the expression of conditioned responses (Hesslow and Ivarsson, 1994). Moreover, microstimulation of these regions suppresses the expression of conditioned responses (Hesslow, 1994). These results indicate that Purkinje cell activity suppresses the ability to express eyelid responses and that the expression of conditioned responses requires the acquisition of decreases in Purkinje cell activity in response to the CS. Our results suggest that in the absence of these processes, plasticity in other regions either does not occur or is incapable of supporting expression of conditioned responses.
The effects of cerebellar cortex lesions on response timing, acquisition, and extinction have important implications about cerebellar sites and rules for plasticity. Because conditioned response timing is learned, the effects of cerebellar cortex lesions imply that there are at least two cerebellar sites of plasticity responsible for eyelid conditioning. The abolition of timing implies timing-related plasticity either in the cerebellar cortex itself or at sites afferent to the cerebellum. In the latter view timing information would be conveyed to the cerebellar cortex via timed (delayed) activation of mossy fiber inputs by the CS. However, because mossy fiber inputs also project to the interpositus nucleus, this view predicts that responses should be appropriately delayed despite removal of cerebellar cortex. The data therefore appear to support more strongly the view that the cerebellar cortex is the site at which temporal coding and timing-related plasticity occurs. Similarly, the presence of (improperly timed) responses after cerebellar cortex removal indicates learning-related plasticity in the cerebellar nucleus as well. Moreover, the inability to acquire responses without the cerebellar cortex suggests that the induction of plasticity in the nucleus requires intact input from Purkinje cells, as predicted by Miles and Lisberger (1981).
On the basis of these arguments, we have proposed a model to account for the acquisition, properly timed expression, and extinction of conditioned eyelid responses (Mauk and Donegan, 1997; Mauk et al., 1997). As shown schematically in Figure6, the initial lack of responding to the CS may be caused by two factors: the relatively weak excitatory connections between CS-activated mossy fibers and the nucleus cells and the relatively strong connections between the CS-activated granule cells and the Purkinje cells. When the CS is presented, the Purkinje cells maintain their strong ongoing activity owing to the relatively strong granule cell synapses. The corresponding inhibition of the nucleus cells would counteract any weak excitation from the CS-activated mossy fiber inputs. With repeated CS + US pairings, the CS-activated granule cell synapses would undergo long-term depression (LTD), leading to a learned pause in Purkinje activity during the CS. The appropriate timing of this pause might be due to different granule cells becoming active at different times during the CS (Buonomano and Mauk, 1994; Raymond et al., 1996; Mauk, 1997; Mauk et al., 1998).
We suggest that this learned pause in Purkinje cell activity during the CS may serve two purposes. First, it would disinhibit nucleus cells, making it possible to elicit a response. Second, it may signal the induction of a long-term potentiation (LTP)-like phenomenon at CS-activated mossy fiber synapses in the nucleus. As such, presentation of the CS to well trained animals would provide a strong excitatory input to the nucleus directly from the strengthened mossy fiber synapses and would provide a well timed pause in inhibition because of the plasticity in the cerebellar cortex. This hypothesis is consistent with a number of findings. It would explain the well timed responses seen in intact animals, the short-latency responses after cerebellar cortex lesions, and the inability to acquire new responses without the cerebellar cortex. We have further hypothesized that extinction may result from the induction of LTP at the synapses between CS-activated granule cells and Purkinje cells. This would restore the ongoing activity of the Purkinje cells during the CS, and the corresponding inhibition of nucleus cells would prevent responding. This hypothesis is consistent with the requirement of the cerebellar cortex for the extinction of conditioned responses (Perrett and Mauk, 1995).
Confidence in these hypotheses clearly requires further empirical test. For now, however, they are consistent with the general notion that changes in cerebellar-mediated responses, such as increasing or decreasing the gain of the VOR as well as acquisition or extinction of conditioned eyelid responses, require an intact cerebellar cortex.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Grants MH 46904 and MH 57051.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Michael D. Mauk, Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, University of Texas Medical School, P.O. Box 20708, Houston, TX 77225. E-mail:mmauk{at}nba19.med.uth.tmc.edu.