Abstract
The daily ingestion of alcohol by pregnant mammals exposes the fetal brain to varying levels of alcohol through the placental circulation. Here we focus on the lingering impact on cortical function of 6.5% alcohol administered in a liquid diet to pregnant rats throughout gestation, followed by 3 alcohol-free months before brain function was analyzed in the offspring. Both spontaneous activity of the neurons in the barrel cortex and the level of response to test stimuli applied to the whiskers remained reduced by >75% after alcohol exposure. Whisker pairing, a type of cortical plasticity induced by trimming all but two whiskers in adult rats, occurred in <1 d in controls, but required 14 d to reach significance after alcohol exposure. These long-term neuronal deficits are present in all layers of cortex and affect neurons with both fast and slow action potentials. Plasticity is first seen in the total sample of neurons at 14 d; however, by 7 d, neurons in layer II/III already show plasticity, with no change in layer IV neurons, and a reverse shift occurs toward the inactive whisker in layer V neurons. Analysis of NMDA receptor subunits shows a persistent, ∼30–50% reduction of NR1, NR2A, and NR2B subunits at postnatal day 90 in the barrel field cortex. Exposing the prenatal alcohol-exposed rats to enriched rearing conditions significantly improves all measured cortical functions but does not restore normal values. The results predict that combinations of interventions will be necessary to completely restore cortical function after exposure of the fetal brain to alcohol.
- synaptic plasticity
- FAS
- somatosensory cortex
- NMDA receptors
- barrel cortex
- cortical reorganization
- rat neocortex
The first postnatal month is an important period for the development of normal, dendritic spines, synapses, and local circuits in rat sensory cortex (Miller, 1988). Most cortical neurons are silent in rat cortex around postnatal day 7 (P7), with only ∼3% of the neurons showing stable spontaneous activity at that age (Armstrong-James, 1975). Organized spontaneous activity in the form of burst-pause discharge and spindling first appear between the first and second week after birth. In our experience the end result of normal development in somatic sensory (barrel field) cortex of rats leads to (1) spontaneously active neurons under urethane anesthesia with ∼1 spike/sec, (2) a short latency (<10 msec) response of ∼1 spike/stimulus produced by controlled stimulation of the principal whisker, (3) a robust longer latency response from the principal whisker and 4–10 surrounding whiskers, and (4) inducible, experience-dependent, cortical plasticity that occurs within hours after whisker trimming (Diamond et al., 1993, 1994; Armstrong-James et al., 1994; Rema et al., 1998a). The shortest latency responses (6–10 msec) are generated most frequently in layer IV neurons, including both “fast” spike and “slow” or “regular” spiking neurons.
Given the immature state of rat cortex at the time of birth, any residual postnatal effects of prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) on the development of normal functional properties in cortex must be exerted through immediate and irreversible brain damage and/or through effects on mechanisms that are needed for continued postnatal development. If there is no alcohol exposure after birth, but there are persistent deficits in cortical function in the adult PAE animal, the assumption has to be that alcohol-free rearing under typical laboratory conditions is not sufficient to induce or rekindle the impaired processes. A large literature documents residual deficits in brain structure and function that persist after various periods of PAE (Jones, 1988), with each deficit dependent on the time and duration of exposure, the concentration of alcohol, and the period of alcohol-free survival before the brain is analyzed (West et al., 1986).
To date, there has not been a detailed report on the impact of PAE on receptive field properties and activity-dependent single-neuron response plasticity in the neocortex. We initiated studies on the effect of PAE on the primary somatic sensory cortex of the rat after several reports that alcohol blocks NMDA receptor function in adult brain slices (Dildy and Leslie, 1989; Lovinger et al., 1989, 1990; Yuen et al., 1991; Morrisett and Swartzwelder, 1993). Chronic ingestion of ethanol produces an upregulation of NMDAR1 subunit expression in the adult (Trevisan et al., 1994), but in contrast, alcohol decreases NMDA receptor binding levels for many weeks after prenatal alcohol exposure (Morrisett et al., 1989; Savage et al., 1992).
One of the most relevant questions for studies of cognitive disabilities is how the normal mammalian brain develops the capacity for lifelong learning and memory. One current hypothesis is that an animal's initial interactions with the extrauterine environment may “set” or “fix” the threshold for plasticity throughout life. Prenatal alcohol produces changes that could interfere with the establishment of effective thresholds for plasticity. A substantial literature exists showing that early rearing experience has a powerful influence on the development of anatomical and functional features of the brain, such that enriched environmental stimulation increases total brain weight and cortical thickness (Krech et al., 1960; Diamond, 1986). More detailed analysis at a cellular level evolved from these experiments and cellular improvements have been documented that include increased cortical synapse-to-neuron ratio and increased frequency of dendritic branching, among others (for review, see Greenough, 1986; Beaulieu and Colonnier, 1987).
On the basis of these results, we used the enriched rearing conditions as an intervention to reverse fetal ethanol effects. The deficits need to be measured with sensitive quantitative methods that can detect subtle improvements in cortical function. We provide evidence that cortical function is impaired by specific criteria into adulthood after PAE and that enriched rearing conditions do improve cortical function, yet without restoring excitability and rates of synaptic modification to control levels.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
All procedures used in these experiments were approved by the Institutional Animal Use Committee and followed guidelines set by National Institutes of Health and the Society for Neuroscience.
Animals
We used Long–Evan rats for this study. Data are included from 37 experimental (seven PAE litters) and 28 control (seven litters) animals. All animals were 3 months old when the brain analyses were performed.
Procedure for feeding alcohol to experimental animals using the BioServ liquid diet
The method used to expose the fetal brain to alcohol throughout gestation has been used in a number of previous studies (Lieber and DeCarli, 1989; Miller, 1989; Lee et al., 1994; Spuhler-Phillips et al., 1997) because it mimics the consumption of moderate levels of alcohol each day during pregnancy [the calculation has been made that 6.5% alcohol in a rat's diet is roughly equivalent to a woman drinking two six-packs of 12 oz. bottles of beer per day throughout pregnancy (Miller, 1986)]. Starting with a nutritionally complete formula from BioServe Corporation, pregnant Long–Evans rats were fed a diet with increasing concentrations of ethanol until at the end of the first week they were receiving 6.5% vol/vol alcohol in their daily diet, which provided ∼35% of their daily calories from the alcohol. The animals were weighed and fed every day, and consumption of diet was recorded. Experimental and control diets continued for 20 d after conception. The blood alcohol concentration taken at 4 hr after the daily diet was offered (diet was given during the dark period of the 12 hr light/dark cycle) averaged 133 mg/dl.
Pair-fed controls. Because alcohol exposure at this level is expected to reduce food intake somewhat, a pair-fed group was run in parallel to control for nutritional deficiencies. After mating, a sperm-positive, pair-fed female rat was carefully weight-matched to its alcohol-receiving counterpart. The pair-fed animal was one that was found to be sperm positive 1 d after the alcohol-treated dam with which it is paired, so that its duration of gestation is always 1 d behind. This allows the food consumption of the alcohol animal to be determined on a given day so that a comparable amount can be given to the pair-fed animal on the corresponding and equivalent day of pregnancy for both animals. Pair-fed animals were treated in every way like their alcohol-treated counterparts except that they did not receive any alcohol. Chow-fed animals are our standard condition, and we have a large database for comparison.
Litters. As the pregnant rats approached parturition, they were monitored carefully several times a day. When the litter was delivered, the number and condition of pups was determined (litter size ranged from 11 to 13). Birth weights were recorded. These data were used for determining whether there are differences in litter size and birth weight. The litters were then randomly culled to eight pups for uniformity in nursing and care.
Analysis of cortical responses to sensory stimuli and plasticity changes
The procedures used to induce plasticity and measure single-unit responses have been described in detail in Armstrong-James et al. (1994) and Rema et al. (1998a). The following is a brief synopsis of these methods.
Whisker pairing. “Whisker pairing” (WP) is a procedure used to measure the status of plasticity in adult rat somatosensory cortex. In experimental and control animals, whiskers on the left side of the face were left intact, whereas all but two whiskers on the right side of the face were clipped to the level of the fur for periods of 1–30 d. Such animals are defined as “whisker-paired animals for x days”; e.g., trim on day 0 and record 72 hr later would be a 3 d whisker-paired animal. For these studies two whiskers, D2 and D1, in the same row were left intact. The whiskers were cut daily up to 2 d before recording. At the start of the recording session all whiskers were trimmed to the same length (3–5 mm) for applying test stimuli. Typically experimental animals were caged in plastic rat cages with at least one normal littermate during the intervals between the initial whisker clipping and the start of the experimental recording session. They appear to use the intact, paired whiskers to palpate, explore, and “whisk” in the normal manner.
Electrophysiology. All animals were anesthetized with intraperitoneal injection of urethane, 1.5 gm/kg body weight (30% solution in water) for recording. The animals were placed in a stereotaxic apparatus, and their body temperature was maintained at 36–37°C using a rectal thermistor coupled to an electronically controlled heating pad. Barrel cortex (typically ∼0–4 mm caudal to Bregma and ∼4–7 mm lateral to midsagittal) was exposed on the left hemisphere.
Action potentials were recorded using carbon-fiber microelectrodes (Armstrong-James and Millar, 1979). A three-dimensional microdrive was used to control the x-y position of the penetrations and the z-axis depth from the cortical surface. Single units were isolated with a time-amplitude window discriminator (Bak Electronics), and each accepted action-potential waveform was compared with the original waveform template on a digital storage oscilloscope (Nicolet). Presumptive localization of the D2 barrel was made by finding neurons in layer IV that generated a response to stimulation of the D2 (principal) whisker at <10 msec latency. Confirmation that those neurons were in penetrations through the D2 barrel was performed histologically before they were included in this study.
Whiskers were stimulated by deflecting individual whiskers 300 μm upward for 3 msec duration with a computer-controlled piezoelectric “bimorph” stimulator positioned with a micromanipulator just beneath the whisker. Each block of trials consisted of 50 stimuli delivered at 1 Hz. For all the neurons analyzed, one block of stimulus trials was presented to the principal whisker D2 and to each of its immediate surround whiskers (D1, D3, C2, E2). For the sake of brevity, in many figures only the D-row whiskers D2, D1, and D3 are shown because they define the rate of cortical synaptic plasticity.
Data analysis. Poststimulus time histograms (PSTHs) at 1 msec bin resolution were generated on-line using a CED 1401 plus processor (Cambridge Electronic Design) and PC computer (Compaq), and all data on the timing of action potentials were stored on a hard disk for off-line analysis. The magnitude of responses evoked from each whisker was the mean ± SE of 50 trials delivered at 1 Hz. For all forms of PSTH analysis, the counts in each bin were adjusted for spontaneous activity by subtracting the spikes generated 50 msec before the stimulus. For determining the significance of responses, nonparametric statistical analysis of the data were performed using Wilcoxon matched-pair sign rank test (Wilcoxon) and Mann–WhitneyU (MWU) test.
Locating the recording sites. At the end of every experiment a DC current of 2 μA for 5–10 sec (electrode tip positive) was passed to produce an easily identifiable lesion roughly 50 μm in diameter to mark the recording site. In most cases the lesions were made at two or three depths along the penetration to determine the electrode path along the column. If penetrations were 100 μm apart the alternate penetrations were marked with a lesion, and unmarked penetrations were determined by geometric interpolation. At the end of the recording session the animals were perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde, and the brains were cryoprotected in 20% sucrose. The cortex was separated from the rest of the brain and flattened. Cytochrome oxidase staining was performed on tangential sections of the flattened cortex to locate the position of the electrode penetrations. Only neurons located within the D2 barrel column were included in the data for this study.
Enriched environment rearing
Our “enriched” rearing environment for a rat consisted of a large (36 × 36 × 30 inch) cage made of one-half inch wire mesh with a pan underneath to catch droppings and urine. Ladders to three platforms with food available on the top level made the environment highly three-dimensional. The bottom of the large cage was covered with toys of various sizes, shapes, and chewability. The rat pups were sexed, and different cages were used for each sex. Equal numbers of PAE (typically six) and control animals (typically six) were placed into the enriched environment chamber from P21 to P90 to permit social as well as spatial challenges. The animals were introduced into the environment each day at the beginning of a 12 hr dark cycle, during which time the room was illuminated with a dim red light bulb just bright enough to allow videotaping of behavior to determine that the animals were using the environment and not, for example, huddling in the corner. During the 12 hr light cycle, animals were housed two to a cage in standard plastic cages. Sets of toys were changed in the chambers before each day to provide maximum novelty. An easily accessible dish of food was placed on the floor of the chamber until the animals were 1 month old. Daily weighing and observation confirmed that by P30 the animals were able to feed from an overhead hopper placed at the top of the chamber.
Analysis of glutamate receptors
Immunocytochemistry. Immunocytochemical analysis was performed on four brains after the physiological analysis using the methods described in Rema and Ebner (1996), with slight modifications. Rats were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital and then perfused transcardially with PBS for 1–2 min to clear the blood, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde for 20–30 min to fix the tissues. Brains were removed and cryoprotected sequentially with 10, 20, and 30% sucrose. Coronal sections, 30 μm thick, were cut on a sliding microtome. The sections were washed four times (15 min for each wash) with Tris-buffered saline (TBS), then incubated in 3% normal serum for 2 hr at room temperature for blocking the nonspecific reaction. Primary antibody to NR1 (AB59) that recognizes all NR1 splice variants was added to the sections in blocking solution and incubated at 4°C for 48–72 hr. The sections were washed in TBS (four washes, each for 10 min) and then incubated with biotinylated secondary antibody for 1–2 hr at room temperature. After another four washes in TBS, the sections were incubated in ABC reagent (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) for 90 min at room temperature and washed again. The sections were finally rinsed in 25 mm Tris buffer and reacted for chromogenic detection of peroxidase, with diaminobenzidine and H2O2 as substrates.
Western blot analysis. Western blot analyses of proteins from membrane-enriched particulate fractions of somatic sensory cortices of control and PAE rats were performed with antibodies to NR1 (AB59) (Rema and Ebner, 1996), NR2A, and NR2B (PharMingen, San Diego, CA) using the method described in Rema and Ebner (1996). Iodinated secondary antibodies were used to detect bands and to aid in quantitation. The bands from the immunoblot were quantified using a phosphorimager and the Imagequant system. The density of bands was converted to percentage values where the counts of the band from control was set at 100%. The values of bands from the PAE animals were percentage of control value.
RESULTS
Effect of PAE on cortical neuron responsiveness
Recording from single neurons in adult cortex after PAE is a silent process compared with controls. The audiomonitor silence reflects a ∼70% reduction in the mean spontaneous activity of all cells recorded in PAE cortex compared with controls (control = 0.94 ± 0.12 vs PAE = 0.28 ± 0.05) even after 90 d of recovery (Fig. 1B). This overall reduction in spontaneous activity is produced by a lower percentage of spontaneously active cells in PAE cortex (control = 76% vs PAE = 38%) as well as a lower discharge rate of the cells that do show spontaneous activity (control = 1.26 ± 0.06 Hz vs PAE = 0.27 ± 0.05 Hz). As the electrode is advanced through PAE cortex, very few neurons can be isolated by their spontaneous discharge under urethane anesthesia. Whisker pairing elevated the level of spontaneous discharge by 14 d in PAE animals with a significant increase (p = 0.0001 MWU) compared with PAE animals with no whisker pairing. At this time the layer V neurons showed a higher level of spontaneous activity than neurons in the other cortical layers (layer V = 1.10 ± 0.3 Hz, layer IV = 0.22 ± 0.05 Hz, layer II/III = 0.19 ± 0.06 Hz). Responsive neurons could be identified in most penetrations by advancing the electrode in 100 μm increments and manually stimulating the whiskers to evoke responses from neurons near the electrode. Examples of single D2 barrel neuron PSTHs generated by stimulation of the principal whisker (D2) and one in-row surround whisker (D1) before any whisker trimming in PAE and control animals are shown in Figure 1C.
Even evoked responses are reduced by a significant 80% after PAE, and the PSTHs illustrate that the effect is exerted almost exclusively by absence of the longer latency components of the response after stimulation of either the principal D2 or the in-row surround D1 and D3 whiskers (Fig. 1C).
The magnitude of the PAE deficits are clearly demonstrated in the mean response levels of all of the neurons studied from PAE and control animals. The data for Figure 2 are derived from several animals from different litters after test stimuli were applied to the principal whisker and four surround whiskers. In both PAE and control animals the principal whisker always produces a response with the highest magnitude and the shortest latency (Fig.2).
The striking reduction in response to stimulation after PAE is seen at all latencies, except the shortest poststimulus epoch of 3–10 msec. Typically, only the principal whisker (in these studies, the D2 whisker) generates the shortest latency responses. Evidence is discussed below showing that the shortest latency response of cortical neurons reflects activity mainly in thalamocortical synapses, and this component of the cortical response remains intact in PAE animals when at the same time the longer latency responses are severely depressed. The response to the D-row surround whiskers, D1 and D3, nearly “bottomed out” in every epoch represented, because the surround whiskers produce little or no short latency activity in the cortical response.
Effect of PAE on cortical plasticity
Trimming all but the D2 and D1 whiskers induces a predictable modification of responses in the D2 barrel neurons to test stimuli delivered to D-row and other whiskers: a type of response modification we call whisker-pairing plasticity. In control animals, within 3 d the responses to the principal D2 whisker remain unaltered, but responses to the intact surround D1 whisker significantly increase, whereas those to the cut D3 whisker remain unaltered or are slightly reduced in magnitude (compare Figs. 2 and3A).
Magnitude of response to the principal D2 whisker increases significantly (peaks) by 7 d after trimming in control animals and then returns to near initial levels after 14 and 30 d. In contrast, the low level of cortical responses to the D2 whisker in non-whisker-paired PAE animals decreases even further after 3 d of whisker pairing (Figs. 2, 3, 4) and slowly recovers at 7–14 d to show significant increase by 30 d (p = 0.012) (Figs. 3, 4). Overall in PAE animals the responses to the D1, D2, and D3 whiskers remain severely reduced from 3 to 30 d after whisker pairing (note that 30 d whisker-paired animals are 4 months old at the time of recording) (Fig. 3). D2 responses of PEA animals never attain more than ∼50% of control levels. In the PAE animals the paired D1 whisker responses fail to show the expected increase after 3 and even 7 d of whisker pairing, but finally, by 14 d of whisker pairing the D1 whisker musters a greater response than the D3 (cut) whisker, so that the D-paired/D-cut surround whisker ratio is changed significantly (p = 0.0001, Wilcoxon), as shown in Figures3C and 4B. Responses to the cut D-3 whisker stay depressed for the entire 30 d period of whisker pairing (Fig. 4C). Subtracting the average spikes generated by the cut D3 whisker from those produced by the intact D1 whisker highlights the negative effect of alcohol over time on this type of cortical plasticity (Fig. 4D).
Effects of PAE on latency epochs of response
Examination of the latency epochs in the overall response magnitudes generated by the D-row whiskers in D2 barrel cortex neurons showed another dimension of how the challenge of whisker pairing alters cortical neuron responses over time (Fig. 3, shading on bar graph). After 3 d of whisker pairing the response to the D2 whisker decreased by 25% in the alcohol-exposed brains (compare Figs.3, 4). This lower response magnitude was the result of a lower response in the 3–10 and the 20–50 msec epochs (Fig. 3A) compared with the non-whisker-paired condition (Fig. 2). In 3 d controls, the expected 100% response increase to the intact D1 whisker reflected an increase in all latency components over the response generated by the cut D3 surround whisker. This activity-dependent plasticity did not occur in the PAE cortex after 3 d (Fig. 3A). There was even a trend for the response to the cut D3 whisker to be greater than the response to the active, intact whisker (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon). This profile of response was not grossly different after 7 d of whisker pairing (Fig. 3B). It took ∼14 d of the activity imbalance produced by whisker trimming for the alcohol-exposed neurons to first show plasticity, to the level of >2:1 ratio in the D1 (paired)/D3 (cut) response, the level seen within 3 d of whisker pairing in normal animals (i.e., surround whisker plasticity only occurred after 14 d). The significant increase in D2 response, which occurs by 7 d of WP in normal animals, took 30 d in PAE animals. The spikes in the increased response to the D1 and D2 whiskers all occur in the long (10–100 msec) latency epochs (Fig. 3, A vs C). The responses that the alcohol-exposed brains were able to produce after 14 d were maintained for 30 d, the duration of these experiments (Fig.3D).
Effect of PAE on fast- and slow-spiking neurons
The effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on cortical activity could be exerted on excitatory neurons with spiny dendrites or on inhibitory neurons with few if any spines on their dendrites that use GABA as a neurotransmitter. The data did not directly answer this question, but the total population of neurons can be sorted by their spike duration, which has been shown consistently to have a high correlation: fast spikes <0.75 msec are characteristic of GABAergic neurons, whereas longer (regular or slow) spikes are typical of excitatory neurons [see discussion by Rema et al. (1998a)]. Figure5 shows that in both the fast- and slow-spiking neuron populations in PAE animals there is (1) the generalized decrement in response, and (2) significant shift in the D1/D3 ratio occurs after 14 d. However, the degree of shift (i.e., the D1 (paired)/D3 (cut) ratio) is greater for the slow-spiking neurons at 14 d (D1 = 11.3 ± 4.3 vs D3 = 2.5 ± 1.5) than it is for the fast-spiking neurons (D1 = 7.1 ± 1.8 vs D3 2.7 ± 1.0) (Fig. 5, 14 Day WP). Thus, the slow-spiking cells show greater upregulation of the paired D1 whisker response, with little change in the cut D3 whisker response.
Effect of PAE on individual neurons that constitute the sample
The mean response of PAE and control groups to test stimuli applied to the intact D1 and the cut D3 whiskers masks the variability of individual neurons that contribute to the averaged response. Figure6 shows the response of every D2 barrel neuron in our sample to the D-row surround whiskers D1 and D3 before (normal) and after 7 d (control and PAE) whisker pairing. In normal animals, without the challenge of whisker pairing, roughly equal numbers of the D2 barrel neurons respond better to each of the surround D-row whiskers D1 and D3 (Fig. 6, D1>D3=49%). Because the neurons were from several different penetrations, animals, and litters, the response may be somewhat biased by the position of the electrode in the D2 barrel, although all penetrations have been confirmed histologically to be in the D2 barrel column (Armstrong-James et al., 1994). As examples, the unfilled arrow in Figure 6 points to a neuron that responded best to the D1 whisker, whereas thefilled arrow points to a neuron in which the D3 whisker provides the stronger input in a normal animal. Recasting the responses to the D1 and D3 whiskers separately by magnitude of response (Sort by Mag.) helps visualize the symmetry of the surround whisker influence on D2 barrel neurons before whisker pairing, although the opposing bars are no longer from the same neuron. Seven days of whisker pairing in control animals skewed the distribution toward the intact D1 whisker so that 81% of the D2 barrel neurons respond at a higher level to the intact D1 whisker, which confirms the expected shift to the active intact whisker. The triple arrow points to three neurons that still have a better response to the cut D3 whisker after whisker trimming in a control animal. Thus, after 1 week not every neuron was driven more intensely by the greater activity in the intact surround whisker, but the trend toward intact D1 whisker dominance is highly significant (p = 0.0001, Wilcoxon) (Fig. 6).
Prenatal alcohol exposure, even after 3 months of recovery, left the neurons not only low in response magnitude after 7 d of whisker pairing, but unexpectedly strengthened the inputs to quite a number of neurons to the inactive, trimmed whisker. For this reason, although the overall values remain roughly half (D1 > D3 = in 52% of cells) of the cortical neurons responding better to one or the other surround whisker, many neurons respond much better to the cut D3 whisker. It is noteworthy that in a certain sense this represents a type of plasticity that is still occurring, the unusual feature being that the shift is moving toward the low activity input. This phenomenon was obscured by averaging. However, when the responses were lined up by magnitude they formed a very symmetrical distribution, denying a shift in the population toward either the intact D1 or the cut D3 surround whisker after 7 d of whisker pairing. The failure of shifting at 7 d but its presence at 14 d supports the conclusion that the expected shift toward the active surround whisker (plasticity) is abnormally slow in PAE animals.
Effect of PAE on neurons in different layers of cortex
Another question raised by averaging responses from all neurons sampled is whether there is specificity of effect on neurons in different layers. One rationale for examining layer responses would be that because of the preservation of the short latency responses to the principal D2 whisker, the layer IV neuron responses that receive a high density of the thalamocortical inputs may develop normally, and the failures could involve neurons in other layers that depend more heavily on intracortical inputs for their activation. Because layer IV neurons in the cortical barrels project almost exclusively to more superficial neurons in layer II/III (Kim and Ebner, 1999), neuron by neuron responses to whisker pairing in layer II/III neurons were separated and compared with responses of neurons in layer IV and layer V of PAE and control animals. Figure 7 shows that the changes in response to whisker pairing occur differently in different cortical layers. In PAE animals, layer II/III neurons showed that after 7 d there was already a significant (p < 0.015, Wilcoxon) shift toward the intact surround D-row whisker (D1). Layer IV neurons showed strong biases; 47% of neurons toward the paired D1 whisker and 37% toward the cut D3 whisker and the remaining neurons showed equal responses to both, but the mean response profile for layer IV neurons remains symmetrical, and hence without the expected bias toward the paired D1 whisker. Quite unexpectedly, the layer V neurons showed a better response to the cut D3 (53%) than to the paired D1 whisker (37%) (Fig. 7, bottom panels). Thus, after 7 d of whisker pairing in PAE animals, neurons in different layers were affected in strikingly different ways from that of controls: superficial layer neurons showed the expected plasticity, thalamic input-dominated layer IV neurons showed great variability in response to one or another whisker but no net plasticity yet, and the output neurons in layer V responded best to the inactive inputs. By 14 d there was significant shift toward the intact whisker by neurons in all layers.
Effect of enriched environment rearing on PAE cortex
The rate of whisker pairing plasticity can be influenced in normal animals by the levels of sensory activity present after the whiskers are trimmed, so a logical question is whether the environmental challenge of enriched rearing conditions from weaning to 3 months would reverse some or all of the deficits seen after prenatal alcohol exposure. In control animals the main effect of enriched rearing conditions was to accelerate the increase in response to the D2 whisker to peak levels as early as 3 d instead of 7 d of whisker pairing (Fig. 8). In the alcohol-exposed group, enriched rearing increased the response magnitude to each individual whisker and accelerated the rate of whisker pairing plasticity so that there was an overall shift toward the intact D1 whisker at 7 d (p < 0.002, Wilcoxon) (Fig. 8), although equivalent neurons in PAE animals in a standard environment would show the plasticity changes only after 14 d. The effects of enrichment indicate that the low cortical activity levels caused by the alcohol did not eliminate plasticity but rather created conditions under which much greater activity was required to reach the threshold for modification, and this was partially achieved by increased environmental challenge.
The accelerated shift seen in PAE animals with enriched rearing experience was not reflected in all layers. Different layers responded differently to the enriched rearing (Fig.9). Enigmatically, the layer II/III neurons appeared to transiently “lose ground,” with a trend for the cut whisker to become a stronger drive on cortical neurons under enriched conditions. Layer IV was greatly affected by enriched rearing, such that the absence of 7 d plasticity after standard caged rearing was converted into a robust and significant shift (p = 0.0001, Wilcoxon) toward the active surround whisker after 7 d of whisker pairing under enriched rearing conditions. Layer V neurons do not show any significant shift to the intact whisker (p > 0.1, Wilcoxon) after 7 d of whisker pairing, even under enriched rearing conditions.
Qualitative observation of videotapes taken as single-frame time-lapse exposures at 5 min intervals during one period of enriched experience each week suggested that the PAE animals were moving around and using the environment indistinguishably from control animals. The data did not allow us to determine whether they were learning from sensory experience and adapting to the environment differently from controls.
Status of NMDA receptors in PAE cortex
The findings above and the deficiency in longer latency, NMDA-dependent components of the evoked response led us to examine the status of NMDA receptors in the barrel field cortex of alcohol-exposed animals after they reached 3 months of age. Because the NMDAR1 subunit is the obligatory protein for a functional NMDA receptor calcium channel, immunocytochemistry was used to look for evidence of change. Levels of NMDAR1 immunoreactivity were visibly decreased in all layers of the somatic sensory cortex (Fig.10A). The staining in the neuropil of the supragranular layers that was densest in the control cases was markedly reduced after alcohol exposure, and the intense immunoreactivity of layer V neurons and their apical dendrites was reduced, as was the number of densely labeled neurons. Western blot analysis confirmed quantitatively that the levels of NMDAR1, NMDAR2A, and NMDAR2B proteins were reduced by ∼50, 40, and 30%, respectively (Fig. 10B,C). One of the AMPA receptor subunits, GluR1, was examined and found to be elevated rather than depressed (data not shown).
DISCUSSION
The central finding of these experiments was that the normal functioning and the capacity to reorganize in the somatic sensory cortex are severely impaired by prenatal alcohol exposure for at least 3 months after birth under standard laboratory rearing conditions. The deficits were characterized by low levels of all types of spontaneous and long latency cortical activity. The preserved, 3–10 msec short latency responses were typically produced only by stimulation of the principal whisker. In normal animals these short latency responses are almost completely dependent on AMPA-type glutamate receptors, and the longer latency (10–100 msec) responses depend mostly on the NMDA type of glutamate receptor (Armstrong-James et al., 1993; Rema et al., 1998a), so the suppression of long latency responses in PAE rats may be related to the reduction in NMDA receptors.
Effect of PAE on cortical plasticity
Theoretically there is a carefully modulated balance between excitation and inhibition in adult cortex that maintains the excitability needed for processing new sensory inputs and for plasticity and learning (Xing and Gerstein, 1996; Shadlen and Newsome, 1998). The low levels of spontaneous and driven cortical neuron excitability in the PAE animals could be produced by a decrease in excitation, by excessive inhibition, or both. Several reports suggest that there are direct negative impacts of PAE on inhibitory processes. For example, the inhibition expected after paired-pulse stimulation at short interstimulus intervals in hippocampal CA1 neurons is minimal or absent after PAE (Hablitz, 1986). At comparably short (20 msec) interstimulus periods, inhibition has been shown to be maximal in barrel field cortex (Simons, 1985). The reduced inhibition after PAE appears to be attributable in part to impairment of GABAA1 receptor subunit-mediated inhibition (Allan et al., 1998). The linkage between PAE and deficits in inhibition, and whether the effect of prenatal alcohol is on only certain subtypes of GABAergic neurons, such as those that contain high levels of certain calcium-binding proteins, is not yet clear. Parvalbumin-containing GABAergic neurons, for example, are reduced by 45% in the cingulate gyrus of alcohol-exposed rats (Moore et al., 1998), but it is not yet known whether this is true for parietal cortex as well.
Indirect effects of PAE on activity levels, and hence on plasticity, could arise through insufficient activity to induce activity-dependent aspects of cortical development that occur postnatally. Low activity just after birth could add a “sensory deprivation” type of plasticity deficit (Huang and Ebner, 1995) on top of the direct, residual alcohol effects, and the combination of the two insults could prolong or consolidate the alcohol-induced deficits in cortical function (Ebner et al., 1998). Similar plasticity deficits in the hippocampus may share this etiology (Swartzwelder et al., 1988).
Effect of PAE on neurons in different layers of cortex
Previous studies have shown that whisker-pairing plasticity occurs at different rates in different cortical layers. Within 24 hr significant changes occur in layer II/III, before any plasticity is detectable in layer IV (Diamond et al., 1994). This rate of plasticity is accelerated by high levels of activity, so that enriched environment rearing conditions for only 15 hr after whisker pairing is enough to produce significant synaptic modification in layer IV neurons (Rema et al., 1998b). Early sensory deprivation, on the other hand, slows equivalent cortical plasticity to 3–7 d (Huang and Ebner, 1995). In the present study the effect of PAE on the rate of plasticity was even greater. Layer IV and layer V neurons in PAE animals did not shift to the intact surround whisker after 7 d of whisker pairing under standard cage conditions, but could be induced to do so by enriched rearing conditions. However, the enriched rearing did not restore plasticity in any layer after 3 d of whisker pairing, so restoration of the rate of synaptic change to normal levels was not complete.
Another noteworthy feature of the laminar analysis was the reverse bias of the layer II/III neurons after 3 d of whisker pairing and for layer V neurons after 7 d of whisker pairing in the PAE animals. This type of “anti-Hebbian” reversal of plasticity such that the synaptic strengths related to the inactive inputs are increased has been reported under several conditions in previous studies. The clearest example occurred when muscimol, a GABA agonist, was minipumped onto kitten visual cortex during monocular experience, and the cortical neurons changed their responses such that the closed eye inputs became stronger than the open eye inputs (Reiter and Stryker, 1988). Our interpretation of this effect is consistent with theirs: postsynaptic neuron activity is crucially important for activity-dependent strengthening of active synapses.
A similar reversal has been reported after acetylcholine (ACh) depletion, where the cut whisker inputs became stronger than the intact surround whisker inputs in the whisker-pairing paradigm (Baskerville et al., 1997; Sachdev et al., 1998). The low spontaneous activity seen after PAE could create a condition analogous to the muscimol application or ACh depletion, causing the inactive whisker to achieve greater strength, but we also show that the depressed responsiveness can be overcome. The results confirm that the superficial layers are most sensitive to changes in levels of activity caused by whisker pairing even in PAE animals, but they produce “anti-Hebbian” plasticity after 3 d of whisker pairing attributable to the developmental impairments. By 7 d, layer II/III neurons shifted to show the expected Hebbian modifications. Similar changes occur in deep layer neurons but at a slower rate; i.e., at 7 d more neurons responded to the cut (inactive) whisker and by 14 d they shifted to the active input. Layer IV neurons took 14 d to show a shift to the active whisker under standard conditions but were speeded up by enriched rearing. A global anti-Hebbian shift toward the inactive whiskers has never been witnessed in normal animals, before the expected activity-driven change, but the possibility exists that it occurs in some layers as a normal prelude to the bias toward the active whisker, but for a very transient (and easily overlooked) period. To our knowledge, there is currently no evidence showing the functional effect of anti-Hebbian plasticity on cortical information processing or a molecular explanation of its pathogenesis.
Abnormal neuron migration is a prominent feature of PAE in laboratory animals and humans; i.e., some neuronal cell bodies end up in the “wrong” layer (Miller et al., 1990; Miller, 1997) or on the cortical surface, or they are delayed in arriving in their appropriate layer (Clarren et al., 1978; Miller, 1986). The extent of abnormal cell migration depends on the time and amount of alcohol exposure. In light of the demonstrated importance of NMDA receptors on cortical neuron migration (Komuro and Rakic, 1993), migration abnormalities could be one early consequence of the reduction in NMDA receptors demonstrated here. The point to note is that when the “responses of 50 neurons in layer II/III” are described in this paper, it is not known what percentage of those cells have mismigrated into those laminae and how that would influence the results. We assume that the number of mismigrated neurons in our sample is small enough that they would not change the mean values for a given lamina but might increase the variance.
Effect of enriched environment rearing on PAE cortex
Enriched rearing does increase the responsiveness and plasticity of cortical neurons significantly after PAE, although not up to control levels. This partial improvement in cortical cell responsiveness indicates that the enhanced sensory and social challenge cannot completely restore cortical plasticity after PAE. Similar enhancement of performance in PAE animals has been reported after enriched rearing, such as improvements in motor skills, compared with animals reared alone in a cage (Hannigan et al., 1993). The environmental enrichment increased the density of dendritic spines on the apical and basal dendrites of control animals, but it failed to increase spine density in the PAE animals (Berman et al., 1996). This result appears at odds with the results of Miller et al. (1990) in which a single subclass of neuron, corticospinal neurons, showed a 32% increase in spine density in PAE animals with simple cage rearing, although many spines took on odd shapes and in some cases the cell bodies had migrated to the wrong layer in cortex. The possibility exists that the enriched environment would reduce the exuberant number of spines in these PAE neurons and move them toward or below normal densities, but such experiments have not been reported. The failure of an enriched environment to completely restore plasticity suggests that additional mechanisms that are less dependent or independent of neural activity are affected by prenatal alcohol exposure.
Other demonstrated regulators of whisker-pairing plasticity in normal animals are the modulatory neurotransmitters acetylcholine and norepinephrine (Lu et al., 1996; Sachdev et al., 1998). After cholinergic depletion there is only a slight reduction in spontaneous activity and in the response of cortical cells to whisker stimulation, but a profound failure of whisker-pairing plasticity that persists for as long as the animals were tested (Baskerville et al., 1997; Sachdev et al., 1998). A reduction of AChE-positive axons in the cortex has been reported in PAE animals (Miller and Rieck, 1993), but the functional impact of PAE on cholinergic mechanisms has not been studied in detail. In addition to direct actions of PAE on cholinergic neurons and the receptors in cortex, indirect actions could be exerted by reducing cholinergic enhancement of calcium entry through NMDA receptors (Auerbach and Segal, 1992). Other modulatory neurotransmitters have shown transient decreases after PAE. Dopamine and D1 receptor binding sites were reduced in frontal cortex and striatum after PAE, whereas at the same time the same features of D2 receptors remained unaffected (Druse et al., 1990).
Status of NMDA receptors in PAE cortex
NMDA-type glutamate receptor levels increase after birth (Insel et al., 1990), and this late developing feature of cortex and hippocampus predicts that the greatest impact of alcohol on NMDA mechanisms would be produced by exposure that occurs during the last part of gestation.Savage et al. (1992) indeed showed that alcohol exposure throughout gestation reduces the NMDA-sensitive glutamate binding sites in the postnatal hippocampus. The change in receptor binding was not present if the exposure was restricted to the first half of gestation, but it was equal in magnitude to exposure throughout gestation if the alcohol was given only during the last one-half to last one-third of pregnancy. These results indicate that the vulnerability of the developing brain to some of the more subtle effects of alcohol exposure on plasticity mechanisms is high in the perinatal period. Furthermore, the results predict that the pathogenesis of decreased neuronal function after PAE is in part channeled through the demonstrated decrease in calcium entry through NMDA receptors (Dildy and Leslie, 1989; Hoffman et al., 1989;Lee et al., 1994; Spuhler-Phillips et al., 1997).
Hippocampal NMDA receptor binding is significantly reduced in PAE animals at ∼6 weeks postnatal (Savage et al., 1991). NMDA-evoked excitability is also reduced in hippocampal slices from PAE animals (Morrisett et al., 1989). Spuhler-Phillips et al. (1997) showed that PAE reduced NMDA-stimulated increases in intracellular calcium. However, when they analyzed membrane fractions from the forebrain (everything in front of the midbrain) of rats exposed to prenatal ethanol in a procedure similar to the present studies using a monoclonal antibody to NR1, they found no significant difference in NR1 subunit protein compared with control group on P0 and P21 (Spuhler-Phillips et al., 1997). These results and those of Hughes et al. (1998) are different from the present results. One methodological difference was that a membrane-enriched particulate fraction was prepared from only somatic sensory cortex at P90 in these experiments, and there was a decrease in NR1, NR2A, and NR2B. For NR1 analysis a polyclonal antibody prepared in our lab was used (Rema and Ebner, 1996). Sample area (barrel field cortex vs forebrain) or age at the time of analysis (P90 vs P0 to P21) or the type of antibody (polyclonal vs monoclonal) could influence these differences.
We have not yet determined whether the accelerated plasticity of enriched rearing has any effect on upregulation of NMDAR1 expression, but the results highlight the importance of understanding the development of NMDAR subunit expression and assembly into functional receptors during normal development. Insights into this feature of normal development are complicated by the NMDAR1 subunit being translated in eight splice variant forms that have different properties and show different distributions within a cortical neuron (Ehlers et al., 1995; Zukin and Bennett, 1995). Alcohol could alter the ratio of splice variants. Furthermore, the subunit composition of functional NMDA receptor changes during development (Monyer et al., 1994; Sheng et al., 1994). Changes in this feature of NMDA receptor function would not be detected even if the total subunit protein were restored to control levels. Given the effect of PAE on NMDA receptor function, it seems likely that normalization of NMDA receptors in PAE animals may provide a promising strategy for restoring cortical plasticity and perhaps intellectual function. One direction we are pursuing to further enhance plasticity is to use a partial agonist to the glycine binding site of the NMDA receptor channel (Lovinger and Zieglgansberger, 1996) during the period of enriched activity to pharmacologically elevate the function of the NMDA receptors in conjunction with the environmental challenge.
Relevance to fetal alcohol syndrome in children
There has been little controversy since the first reports of Jones et al. (1973) that PAE affects brain function in children as well as in animal models. One unique advantage of model systems is the ability to control other aspects of the animal's life, so that the effects of alcohol are not confounded by concurrent malnutrition, other abused substances, neglect, and other sequelae of poverty as is most often the case in humans (Abel, 1995; Abel and Hannigan, 1995). Many of the structural changes found in animal studies also have been reported in human neuropathology, such as smaller than normal head and brain size (Mattson and Riley, 1995) and failure of cell migration and malformations (Clarren et al., 1978). The intellectual disabilities persist, with little improvement in children diagnosed with fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) and fetal alcohol effects (Streissguth et al., 1996).
Some data from children with fetal alcohol syndrome are comparable to the present single-unit analysis. Kaneko et al. (1996) using noninvasive resting EEG analysis in FAS children found greatly reduced power in the α frequency band (7.5–12 Hz) without a correlated increase in slow wave activity as was found in their comparison of children with Down syndrome. One problem with interpreting this result is that it does not distinguish deficits in early processing in sensory systems from predominant dysfunction in “associative/cognitive” areas less dominated by one sensory modality. Evoked or event-related potential studies would help resolve this issue. Measurement of visual evoked potentials in children exposed to prenatal alcohol and other drugs showed delayed maturation in components of the visual evoked potential (Scher et al., 1998). Similar changes in event-related potentials in the hippocampus were detected in rats after prenatal ethanol exposure (Kaneko et al., 1996). We think that interesting new insights could arise from correlating noninvasive EEG analysis in PAE children with comparable EEG/single-unit analysis from experimental preparations, so that cross-comparisons could be made before and after interventions. For the most useful comparison, the animal studies would need to be performed in awake animals to eliminate the effects of anesthesia.
All of these data were collected under urethane anesthesia, and there is no way of evaluating whether the impact of the anesthetic was greater on responses evoked in impaired PAE cortex than those evoked in control cortex. Because anesthesia may be a key variable, studies are being performed to reproduce the experiments using awake animals with chronically implanted electrodes to measure the effect of anesthesia, which has been shown to be a potent variable for sensory information processing in our model system (Chapin et al., 1981; Chapin and Lin, 1984; Simons et al., 1992; Friedberg et al., 1999).
Footnotes
This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health Grants NS13031 and NS25907 and a generous gift from Irwin and Annette Eskind. We thank Mark Maguire and Anita Sankaran for help with alcohol exposure of the rats and histology, and Dr. Michael Armstrong-James for critical comments on this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to V. Rema, P.O. Box 152, Peabody College, Institute for Developmental Neuroscience, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37203. E-mail:rema.velayudhan{at}vanderbilt.edu.