Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
ARTICLE, Cellular/Molecular

Sustained Activation of Hippocampal Lp-Type Voltage-Gated Calcium Channels by Tetanic Stimulation

Jessica M. Schjött and Mark R. Plummer
Journal of Neuroscience 1 July 2000, 20 (13) 4786-4797; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-13-04786.2000
Jessica M. Schjött
1Rutgers University, Department of Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Nelson Laboratories, Piscataway, New Jersey 08854-8082
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mark R. Plummer
1Rutgers University, Department of Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Nelson Laboratories, Piscataway, New Jersey 08854-8082
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The molecular heterogeneity of voltage-gated calcium channels is mirrored by extensive biophysical diversity. Subtype-selective antagonists have been used to place different kinds of calcium channels in functional categories. Dihydropyridine (DHP) antagonists have been used, for example, to implicate L-type calcium channels in the induction of NMDA receptor-independent forms of synaptic plasticity. DHPs, however, do not discriminate between the recently identified Lp and Ls subtypes of L-type calcium channel. The different properties of the two kinds of L-type channels suggest that they may have different functional roles. Ls channels are comparable with cardiac L-type channels, whereas Lp channels show low-threshold voltage-dependent potentiation. To clarify the potential roles of Lp and Ls channels in the induction of synaptic plasticity, we studied the responses of these channels to trains of action potentials. The frequency and duration of the trains were chosen to mimic the stimuli used to induce changes in synaptic strength. Cell-attached single-channel recordings from cultured hippocampal neurons revealed that both Lp and Ls channels responded to these trains, but only Lp channels showed persistent activation that outlasted the train. The magnitude of Lp channel activity increased with increasing action potential frequency and train duration. Stimuli that reproduced the postsynaptic response to action potential trains were also examined, and Lp channels were found to show much greater responses than were Ls channels. These results suggest that the Lp channel may play a critical role in the induction of long-lasting changes in synaptic strength.

  • calcium channel
  • hippocampus
  • potentiation
  • long-term potentiation
  • synaptic plasticity
  • dihydropyridine
  • L-type

Calcium entry into neurons is an essential trigger for many forms of synaptic plasticity (for review, see Malenka and Nicoll, 1993). The flux of extracellular calcium into cells is accomplished via ligand- or voltage-gated calcium-permeable ion channels such as the NMDA receptor channel or voltage-gated calcium channels. The involvement of these ion channels in the induction of synaptic plasticity has been demonstrated with selective antagonists. AP-5, a specific blocker of NMDA receptors, prevents induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons (Collingridge et al., 1983; Harris et al., 1984), and the calcium channel antagonists nifedipine and nimodipine can prevent induction of NMDA receptor-independent LTP in CA1 (Grover and Teyler, 1990), CA3 (Kapur et al., 1998), and amygdala neurons (Weisskopf et al., 1999). Nifedipine and nimodipine, dihydropyridine (DHP) compounds, are selective antagonists of L-type voltage-gated calcium channels (for review, see Triggle, 1999), thus implicating this channel type in the regulation of NMDA receptor-independent forms of synaptic plasticity (Johnston et al., 1992).

We (Kavalali and Plummer, 1994, 1996; Kavalali et al., 1997a) and others (Forti and Pietrobon, 1993; Hivert et al., 1999) have suggested that there are multiple functional kinds of L-type channels that differ in their conductance and kinetic properties. The primary criterion used to distinguish between types of L-type channels is the presence of repolarization reopenings, openings that occur subsequent to a depolarization and return to the holding potential.

The subtype of L-type channel that shows reopenings has been referred to as either the Lp channel (Kavalali and Plummer, 1994) or the “anomalous” L-type channel (Forti and Pietrobon, 1993) on the basis of different interpretations of the channel's voltage dependence. Lp channels were named according to the voltage-dependent potentiation of their activity seen after conditioning depolarization. Anomalous L-type channels were named for their “bell”-shaped activation profile. At present, it is not known whether this distinction in nomenclature reflects yet further subdivisions in L-type channels that show repolarization reopenings or whether Lp and anomalous L-type channels are the same.

Regardless of the details of classification, it is clear that the activity of Lp channels is increased after depolarization. Moreover, relatively low-voltage stimuli can produce this potentiation (Kavalali and Plummer, 1996), leading to speculation that this channel type is involved in the induction of NMDA receptor-independent forms of synaptic plasticity. The goal of this study was to gain insight into this possibility by determining whether the Lp channel can be activated by the same kinds of stimuli known to elicit changes in synaptic strength. We tested both presynaptic and postsynaptic equivalents of action potential train stimuli by synthesizing appropriate waveforms and examining the responses produced by these stimuli in cell-attached single-channel recordings of Lp channels. We found that Lp channels could be activated by these stimuli and were preferentially activated by them when compared with the responses of the more cardiac-like L-type calcium channel (Ls). Thus we provide support for the notion that Lp channels may be critically involved in the induction of NMDA receptor-independent forms of synaptic plasticity.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Cell culture. Hippocampal cultures were grown as described previously (Levine et al., 1995). Briefly, hippocampi were obtained from embryonic day 18 Sprague Dawley rats and placed into cold PBS. Cells were triturated in 2 ml of MEM with added glucose and 7.5% fetal bovine serum and plated on poly-d-lysine-coated Petri dishes at a final density of 106cells/35 mm dish. Cultures were maintained in serum-free medium (SFM) at 37°C in a 95% air/5% CO2 humidified incubator. SFM consisted of a 1:1 (v/v) mixture of Ham's F-12 and Eagle's minimum essential medium and was supplemented with insulin (25 μg/ml), transferrin (100 μg/ml), putrescine (60 μm), progesterone (20 nm), selenium (30 nm), glucose (6 mg/ml), and penicillin–streptomycin (0.5 U/ml and 0.5 mg/ml, respectively).

Recordings and experimental treatments. Voltage-clamp recordings were obtained from pyramidal-type cells after 3–21 din vitro using standard techniques (Hamill et al., 1981). Cell-attached single-channel recordings were made with barium as the charge carrier (in mm, 20 BaCl2, 10 TEA, 90 choline chloride, and 10 HEPES-TEAOH, pH 7.5), and a depolarizing bath solution was used to bring the intracellular potential to ∼0 mV (in mm, 140 K-gluconate, 10 HEPES-KOH, and 5 EGTA, pH 7.5). Unless otherwise specified, 1 μm L-type channel agonist FPL 64176 (Zheng et al., 1991) was added to the bath solution to clarify the number and the types of calcium channels in the patch. Recordings were made with an Axopatch 200 amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Data were sampled with an INDEC 15125 analog-to-digital converter (INDEC Systems, Capitola, CA) at 5 kHz and filtered at 1 kHz. Voltage pulses were delivered at 5 sec intervals. Linear leak and capacitive currents were subtracted digitally. All recording parameters were controlled by programs written in Borland C++ using INDEC-supplied driver libraries.

Data analysis. Open times and open probabilities were obtained from sweeps idealized with a half-amplitude crossing criterion and cubic spline interpolation (Colquhoun and Sigworth, 1983). Overall open probability (po) in response to a particular stimulus was calculated by evaluating all applicable sweeps during the entire recording, including null sweeps. The total open time during the analyzed portion of the sweep was divided by the analysis time period. Incomplete channel openings, such as tail current openings, were included in the analysis. For patches that contained two of the same kind of channel, open probability was measured for both channels and divided by two. This assumes that the two channels have similar but independent gating characteristics and give values appropriate for a single channel. At least 20, typically 50–80, sweeps were used for each pocalculation.

For analysis of open time, values were plotted on square root–log coordinates, and mean open times were estimated from the maximum likelihood fitting (Sigworth and Sine, 1987). The number of open time components used for fits was based on a previous study (Kavalali et al., 1997a). Data from all experiments were summed and treated as a single distribution. The summed distributions were compared statistically with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Unless otherwise stated, statistical comparisons were made with the Student'st test. Data analysis was accomplished with programs written in Microsoft Visual Basic.

Lp and Ls channels were identified according to criteria described previously (Kavalali and Plummer, 1994). In brief, Lp activity was identified by the presence of reopenings during repolarization to −70 mV after a voltage pulse to −10 mV. When studied in the presence of FPL 64176, Lp channels could be further distinguished from Ls channels on the basis of unitary current amplitude and relatively shorter open times. Two kinds of recordings were selected for detailed analysis. The first was from patches that contained one to two of the same kind of channel, determined by the presence of superimposed openings of a single type. The second type of recording was from multichannel patches that contained both Lp and Ls channels (typically one to three Ls and one to two Lp channels). These patches were used exclusively for studies of Lp reopenings because only Lp channels are active after repolarization (Kavalali and Plummer, 1994). Patches containing non-DHP-sensitive channels (e.g., T-, N-, and P-type channels that typically showed more openings when tested from a holding potential of −90 mV compared with −40 mV) were not used.

RESULTS

As we have shown previously (Kavalali and Plummer, 1994, 1996;Kavalali et al., 1997a), cell-attached single-channel recordings from hippocampal neurons reveal two general classes of voltage-gated calcium channel whose openings are prolonged by exposure to the L-type calcium channel agonist FPL 64176 (Fig.1A,B). One type of channel, Ls, shows openings that are restricted to the test depolarization or are strict “tail currents” in which the abrupt change in driving force from the test potential to the holding potential elicits a large current through a channel that is already open (Fig. 1D). The second type of channel, Lp, opens during the test potential but can also reopen several times after the transmembrane voltage has been returned to the holding potential (Fig.1C). A key distinction between Ls tail currents and Lp reopenings at −70 mV is that tail currents are relatively brief and are not interrupted by numerous brief closures. As described in detail in previous work (Kavalali and Plummer, 1994), the two channels also show characteristic differences in unitary conductance and mean open time in the presence of agonist (Fig. 1C,D).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Lp and Ls channels show characteristic gating patterns in the presence or absence of agonist. A, Ten consecutive sweeps of a cell-attached recording from a hippocampal neuron in tissue culture. The patch contained multiple calcium channels; in the absence of agonist, openings were brief and poorly resolved. In this particular patch, repolarization reopenings (horizontalbars) were observed in some of the sweeps. B, Ten consecutivesweeps from the same patch illustrated inA after addition of the L-type channel agonist FPL 64176 (1 μm). Test pulse and repolarization reopening durations were both prolonged. C, Cell-attached recordings in the presence of agonist from a patch that contained two Lp channels. Reopenings are shown in sweeps1(top), 3, and 5.D, Cell-attached recordings in the presence of agonist from a patch that contained two Ls channels. Tail current openings are shown in sweeps1 and 3. The dashedlines in both Cand D indicate the amplitude of Ls channels under similar recording conditions. Note that the amplitudes of Lp openings were slightly smaller than those of Ls channels, as expected from the slightly smaller conductance of this channel type (Kavalali and Plummer, 1994). Also note the characteristic difference in agonist-induced open time between Lp and Ls channels.

The goal of this study was to gain insight into the functional roles of Lp and Ls channels by comparing their responses to physiologically relevant stimuli such as trains of action potentials in addition to the more standard square pulse voltage protocols. To accomplish this, synthetic waveforms were created in which action potentials were delivered at three different frequencies: 25, 50, and 100 Hz (Fig.2). The action potential used (Fig. 2,left) was based on those recorded from cultured hippocampal neurons with the whole-cell current-clamp technique. The holding potential was set to −70 mV, and the interspike potential (the voltage between individual action potentials) was set to −60 mV. Again, these values for the transmembrane voltage were chosen according to their general approximation of responses of hippocampal neurons to step current injection. The 100 Hz action potential train was tested because of its common usage as “tetanic” stimulation in the induction of long-term forms of synaptic plasticity (for review, see Bliss and Collingridge, 1993). The lower frequency trains were used for comparison with the higher frequency ones.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Stimulus templates used to study the responses of Lp and Ls calcium channels to trains of action potentials. The computer-generated waveforms were used to reproduce the general characteristics of trains of action potentials firing at defined frequencies. Left,Trace showing the model for an individual action potential. Its shape was based on whole-cell current-clamp recordings from cultured hippocampal neurons.Middle, Right, Tracesshowing action potential bursts of the indicated firing frequency. Each of the bursts was constructed using a series of the individual action potentials. The holding voltage was −70 mV, the interspike voltage was −60 mV, and each action potential peaked at +47 mV. Note the difference in the timescale between the left and the remaining three traces. AP, Action potential.

In the first part of this study, Lp and Ls activity was assessed during the repolarization portion of the stimulus (the period at the holding potential subsequent to the action potential train—see Fig.3A, dashed line). This approach allowed us to examine relatively long-lasting effects of action potential trains. We were not able to quantify channel activity during the action potential trains because the leak and capacitive currents could not be subtracted accurately. It is clear in the traces, however, that action potential trains elicited openings of both Ls and Lp channels (Fig. 3A,C). Repolarization activity was quantified as the open probability (po) by dividing the percent time that the channel was open by the total duration of the repolarization period. Thus both tail current openings and reopenings contributed to the measured po.

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Differential responses of Lp and Ls channels to action potential trains. A, Exampletraces of a cell-attached recording from a multichannel patch that contained two Lp channels are shown. Only activity during the repolarization period (dashedline) was analyzed. In response to a 320 msec, 100 Hz train, the Lp channel exhibited numerous reopenings. Bottomtrace, An ensemble average was constructed from 42 sweeps. B, A 320 msec square pulse to −30 mV elicited repolarization activity comparable with that produced by the 100 Hz stimulus. Bottomtrace, An ensemble average was constructed from 49 sweeps. The same patch shown inA was used. C, Exampletraces of a cell-attached recording containing two Ls channels are shown. The 100 Hz stimulus elicited an occasional tail current. Bottomtrace, An ensemble average was constructed from 40 sweeps. D, A 320 msec square pulse to −30 mV elicited essentially no repolarization activity. Bottomtrace, An ensemble average was constructed from 51 sweeps. The same patch shown inC was used. In this and all subsequent figures,averagecurrents have been divided by the number of channels in the patch.

When tested with action potential train stimuli, Lp and Ls channels showed different responses (Fig. 3). A 100 Hz train of action potentials, for example, caused Lp channels to show numerous reopenings after the cessation of the action potential train (Fig. 3A). Ls channels, however, in response to the same stimulus, showed only occasional tail current openings (Fig. 3C). This difference in channel kinetics was more evident in ensemble average currents that revealed a rapid decay of Ls current during repolarization compared with a slow decay of Lp current. To understand how Lp and Ls channel gatings were being affected by action potential trains, we compared, in the same recordings, responses to trains with responses to square pulse depolarizations to different test potentials (Fig. 3B,D). We found that square pulse depolarizations to relatively modest test potentials (less than or equal to −30 mV) elicited reopenings and tails currents that were similar to the maximal response produced by the action potential trains. In addition to comparable levels of activity, there were no obvious differences in the pattern of openings or the latencies to the first opening. Therefore, action potential train stimuli did not appear to produce qualitatively different types of responses than did square pulse stimuli.

Quantification of Lp channel activity showed that action potential trains of increasing frequency produced greater levels of repolarization activity at −70 mV (Fig.4A). The 100 Hz train, for example, caused an eightfold greater response than did the 25 Hz train (po = 0.03 ± 0.008 vs 0.0036 ± 0.002; p < 0.01). As expected, square pulses with increasingly positive test potentials also produced progressively greater responses. Increasing the test potential from −40 to −10 mV caused a 24-fold increase in reopenings (po = 0.005 ± 0.003 vs 0.12 ± 0.016; p < 0.001). Quantification of Ls channel responses showed that the amount of activity elicited during the repolarization period was very low. Furthermore, none of the Ls channel responses to the different stimuli was significantly different (p > 0.05 for all comparisons). Nonetheless, trends similar to that seen for Lp channels were observed (Fig.4B). Larger Ls channel responses were obtained with greater action potential frequencies or an increased magnitude of depolarization. Finally, comparison of Lp versus Ls channels revealed significant differences for all stimuli tested (p < 0.01) except for the smallest square pulse (−40 mV; p > 0.1) and the lowest-frequency action potential train (25 Hz; p > 0.1).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Action potential trains elicited larger repolarization activity of Lp channels than of Ls channels.A, Quantitative analysis of Lp channel reopenings in response to three different frequencies of action potential trains and four different square pulse depolarizations. Square pulses to −10 mV elicited the greatest level of Lp channel reopenings. Reopeningpo decreased with decreasing depolarization. Of the action potential trains, the 100 Hz stimulus produced the greatest response, with reopening podecreasing with decreasing frequency. The number of recordings is indicated above each bar inparentheses. Responses statistically different from the response to the 100 Hz train are indicated by an * (p < 0.05). Responses statistically different from the response to −10 mV are indicated by a † (p < 0.01; one exception,p < 0.05 for −20 mV). B, Repolarization activity of Ls channels in response to the same stimuli used in A. For comparison, the scale of they-axis is the same as that in A. None of the stimuli that produced Ls channel responses showed a significantly different magnitude from that of the 100 Hz stimulus or the −10 mV voltage pulse. The magnitude of the Lp channel responses was significantly greater than that of the Ls channels for the −30 mV, −20 mV, −10 mV, 100 Hz, and 50 Hz stimuli (p < 0.01).

Additional insight into the mechanism of Lp channel gating was obtained by comparing responses to the action potential trains and square voltage pulses. A significant difference between these two types of stimuli is that action potential trains briefly make the membrane potential very positive, even though the average depolarization remains modest. To determine whether the briefly positive membrane potential had an effect on Lp channel gating, we calculated the average depolarization produced by each of the action potential trains and compared the response obtained with the action potential train with the response obtained with a square pulse of comparable average depolarization. For the 25, 50, and 100 Hz trains, the average depolarizations were approximately −55, −51, and −41 mV, respectively. Each of these values was determined by integrating the area under the action potential burst portion of the waveform. Preliminary experiments showed that voltage pulses to −50 mV elicited essentially no response from Lp channels, and experiments with this level of depolarization were not pursued further. In fact, −40 mV voltage pulses (10 mV more positive than the calculated average depolarization) were necessary to produce responses that were comparable with those elicited by the 25 and 50 Hz trains. The 100 Hz train also elicited a larger response than would be expected from its calculated depolarization of −41 mV. When compared with the average response seen to the −40 mV pulse (po = 0.005 ± 0.003), the average response to the 100 Hz train was significantly greater (po = 0.03 ± 0.008;p < 0.01) and was actually more similar to that of the −30 mV pulse (po = 0.025 ± 0.005; p > 0.6). These data suggest that action potential trains evoke larger responses of Lp channels than would be expected from the average depolarization of the stimulus. Thus, even the brief depolarization of the action potential may be sufficient to enhance Lp repolarization reopenings. An important caveat, however, is that the pipette voltage is not likely to follow the command voltage exactly. Therefore, the increased reopening activity may also result from a larger actual depolarization than that predicted from the stimulus waveform.

As described in Materials and Methods, use of an L-type channel agonist was essential for most of these studies. Therefore, it was important to determine whether the results obtained from these recordings would generalize to agonist-free conditions. This issue was addressed by making recordings in the absence of agonist and testing the same set of stimuli described previously. Only Lp channels were studied because the activity of these channels could be isolated by measuring the brief reopenings still observed without agonist (Fig. 1). However, the brevity of the openings made determination of channel number difficult and precluded accurate measurement of absolutepo. Therefore, the data were normalized.

In general, Lp channels studied under agonist-free conditions showed the same pattern of responses as those obtained in the presence of agonist. Channel reopenings could be observed during the repolarization period (Fig. 5A), and action potential trains of increasing frequency produced responses with higherpo (Fig. 5B). The same was true for square pulses of increasing amplitude. As seen with recordings done in the presence of agonist, square pulses to voltages more depolarized than −30 mV elicited responses greater than that produced by the 100 Hz train. Comparison of the responses in agonist (Fig. 4) and without agonist (Fig. 5) shows very similar profiles with unchanged frequency and voltage dependence. The response to the 25 Hz train was significantly less than that to the 100 Hz train (p < 0.05), and the response to the −40 mV pulse was significantly lower than the response to the −20 mV pulse (p < 0.01). This argues for the idea that the agonist did not change the general response characteristics of Lp channels although it had a clear effect on their magnitude.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

The pattern of Lp channel responses to action potential and square pulse stimuli is not changed in the absence of agonist. An analysis of repolarization responses obtained from Lp channels in the absence of FPL 64176 is shown. A, Example traces from a cell-attached multichannel recording showing that Lp channel reopenings can persist for several hundred milliseconds in the absence of agonist. B. Average responses normalized according to the activity elicited by the −10 mV square pulse. Note that the relative magnitudes of individual responses are essentially identical to those obtained in the presence of agonist. A response statistically different from the response to the 100 Hz train is indicated by an * (p < 0.05). Responses statistically different from the response to −20 mV are indicated by a † (p < 0.01; one exception,p < 0.05 for −30 mV).

This conclusion was supported further by experiments in which response properties before and after agonist exposure were studied in single recordings. The most complete experiment of this type is shown in Figure 6, in which all the stimuli tested were applied in a single recording both before and after addition of agonist. The pattern of responses from this single recording was the same as that observed previously. As expected, the magnitude of the reopening probability was much lower before addition of agonist (Fig.6A) than after (Fig. 6B). Most important, however, was the near identity of the response profile before and after addition of agonist (Fig. 6C,D). Thus, it is reasonable to expect that addition of agonist does not alter the fundamental way in which Lp channels respond to action potential trains.

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

Addition of agonist during a recording changes the magnitude but not the pattern of responses to stimuli.A, Example traces of a cell-attached multichannel recording from a patch that contained Ls channels and three to four Lp channels. Responses to a −30 mV square pulse are shown, with reopenings present in all three traces.B, Example traces obtained from the same patch shown in A after addition of 1 μmFPL 64176. Reopenings were prolonged by the addition of agonist.C, Quantitative analysis of the agonist-free portion of the recording. As shown with the group data in Figures 4 and 5, the square pulse to −10 mV elicited the greatest response, with action potential trains producing smaller responses comparable with smaller depolarizations. D, Analysis of the recording after addition of agonist. The magnitude of the responses increased, but the pattern remained the same.

Having established that Lp channels can be activated by trains of action potentials, we wanted to assess specifically tetanic stimuli known to elicit changes in synaptic strength. Therefore we switched from relatively short action potential trains to longer ones. These were 200 Hz for 500 msec and 100 Hz for 1 sec. These two stimuli are used to elicit NMDA receptor-independent LTP (Grover and Teyler, 1990) and NMDA receptor-dependent LTP, respectively. For comparison, we also examined Ls and Lp channel responses to 25 Hz for 1 sec and a square pulse to −30 mV for 1 sec.

Initial experiments focused on repolarization reopenings and tail currents. As seen with the trains of 320 msec duration, Lp channels showed substantially greater responses to the 500 msec and 1 sec trains than did Ls channels (Fig. 7). The 500 msec trains also provided a prolonged observation time during the repolarization period. With these stimuli, we noted that Lp channel reopenings could persist for as long as 700 msec after the end of the action potential train (Fig. 7A). Comparison of Lp channel responses to the 100 Hz, 1 sec train and the 200 Hz, 500 msec train showed that the two stimuli did not produce statistically different levels of activity, although both of these stimuli evoked substantially greater responses than did the 1 sec depolarization to −30 mV (p < 0.01). Ls channel tail currents showed a similar response profile with the greatest responses to the 100 and 200 Hz stimuli (Fig. 7B). The Lp channel reopeningpo was, however, much greater in magnitude than was that observed for Ls channel tail currents (p < 0.01 for all except −30 mV atp < 0.05).

Fig. 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 7.

Stimuli that mimic those used to induce synaptic plasticity elicit different responses in Lp and Ls channels.A, Example traces of a cell-attached multichannel recording from a patch that contained two Lp channels (top) and a patch containing four Ls channels (bottom). A 500 msec, 200 Hz stimulus elicited Lp channel reopenings and Ls channel tail currents. Activity during the action potential train was completely obscured by the stimulus artifact and was blanked out in the traces shown.Bottomtraces, For both Lp and Ls recordings, an ensemble average constructed from 105 and 38 sweeps, respectively. B, Summary Lp and Ls channel repolarization responses to four different stimuli: a 1 sec, −30 mV square pulse; a 500 msec, 200 Hz train of action potentials; a 1 sec, 100 Hz train of action potentials; and a 1 sec, 25 Hz train of action potentials. The three action potential train stimuli produced significantly larger Lp and Ls channel responses than did the square pulse (*p < 0.05; #p < 0.01). All Lp channel responses were also significantly greater than were the Ls responses to the same stimuli (§p < 0.05; ‡p < 0.01).

For three of the stimuli tested (square pulse to −30 mV and 100 and 25 Hz trains) we were able to assess the effect of stimulus duration on Lp channel responses (comparison of data shown in Figs. 4, 7). For the −30 mV voltage pulse, the difference in response magnitude between the short (320 msec) pulse and the long (1 sec) pulse was not significant (p > 0.2). Increased duration of the action potential trains did produce larger responses, however. The 1 sec, 25 Hz train elicited a 10-fold greater response than did the 320 msec train (p < 0.01). The 1 sec, 100 Hz train elicited a 2-fold greater response than did the 320 msec train (p < 0.05).

The trains of action potentials used in this part of the study resemble most closely the voltage change that an Lp or Ls channel would experience if it were located presynaptically. Action potential train stimuli may also be a useful model of voltage changes that a postsynaptic channel would experience as a result of back-propagating action potentials that invade a dendrite. The transfer characteristics of a synapse, however, are also a major determinant of a postsynaptic response. Recordings from CA1 neurons have shown that tetanic stimulation produces compound synaptic potentials and a transient burst of action potentials. To model a postsynaptic response, we simulated recordings made by others of the postsynaptic recording seen in response to a high-frequency train of action potentials (Grover and Teyler, 1990; Kapur et al., 1998). This response is comprised of an initial burst of action potentials followed by a maintained plateau potential. Our “burst–plateau” stimulus was made up of a 150 msec train of action potentials at 100 Hz followed by an 850 msec depolarization to −30 mV. As with the previous stimuli, the holding potential was −70 mV. In addition to approximating the voltage changes seen by postsynaptic Lp and Ls channels in response to tetanic stimulation, this stimulus had the additional advantage of allowing us to observe channel activity during the stimulus itself, rather than only looking at responses during the repolarization period. Thus we quantified the responses during the −30 mV plateau portion of the stimulus. To assess the contribution of the initial burst of action potentials to the overall channel response, we also used a stimulus that was a simple 1 sec square pulse to −30 mV.

Lp channels responded strongly to the burst–plateau stimulus, whereas Ls channels did not (Fig.8A,C). Lp openings began during the burst of action potentials and continued throughout the plateau portion of the stimulus. This was seen clearly both in individual sweeps as well as in an ensemble average current (Fig.8A, top average current). Ls channels also began opening during the train of action potentials, but opening frequency declined rapidly during the plateau at −30 mV (Fig. 8C). This was most apparent in the ensemble average current in which an initial inward current was present that declined to a negligible amount after the action potential burst had ended even though the voltage remained at −30 mV (Fig.8C, top average current). Comparison of the Lp and Ls average currents showed that although the two began with similar peak levels, the slow decay of Lp current would provide greater total calcium influx than would the rapidly decaying Ls current.

Fig. 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 8.

Stimuli that mimic postsynaptic responses to high-frequency stimulation strongly activate Lp channels.A, Example traces and two ensembleaveragecurrents of a cell-attached recording from a patch that contained two Lp channels. The stimulus used was a brief burst of action potentials riding on top of a maintained plateau potential. Because of the small number of channels in the patch, partial leak subtraction of activity during the action potential burst was possible. Lp channel activity began during the action potential burst, was maintained throughout the plateau potential, and continued during the repolarization period. This was evident in the individual traces as well as in theaveragecurrents. The top(Tailcurrentincluded) and bottom (Tailcurrentexcluded) averagecurrentswere constructed from 240 sweeps. The topaveragecurrent was a nonconditional average that contained all sweeps. The bottomaveragecurrent had all tail currents removed. Sweeps that consisted solely of tail currents were counted as nulls, and sweeps that contained both tail currents and reopenings had the tail currents removed. Exclusion of the tail currents from the plateau and repolarization averagecurrents did not have a significant impact on the time course of the Lp channel current. B, Exampletraces from the same patch shown in A but in response to square pulse depolarization to −30 mV. Lp channels showed slow activation and maintained current throughout the depolarization that continued into the repolarization period. Thetop and bottomaveragecurrents were constructed as described inA from 214 sweeps. C, Cell-attached recording from a patch containing Ls channels. The channel was activated strongly during the action potential burst but declined rapidly during the plateau portion of the stimulus and ceased after repolarization. The top and bottomaveragecurrents were constructed as described in A from 253 sweeps. Exclusion of the tail currents eliminated the bulk of Ls channel current during the plateau depolarization. D, Same patch shown in Cbut traces shown were responses to a square pulse depolarization to −30 mV. Activity was low throughout the stimulus. The top and bottomaveragecurrents were constructed from 222 sweeps.

To determine the contribution of the initial burst of action potentials to the production of the overall responses, we compared activity elicited by burst–plateau stimuli with that evoked by square pulses to the plateau voltage. In the absence of the initial burst of action potentials, Lp channel activity increased gradually (Fig.8B), although it eventually reached the same magnitude that was seen with burst–plateau stimulation (Fig.8A). Thus the burst of action potentials was responsible for the initial increase in Lp channel current.

Unlike Lp channels, the initial burst of action potentials was almost entirely responsible for the Ls channel responses to burst–plateau stimuli. When the square pulse stimulus was used, the Ls channel average current was small (Fig. 8D), especially when compared with the Lp channel average current (Fig.8B). The initial increase in current seen with burst–plateau stimulation was entirely absent.

One consequence of using the L-type channel agonist to identify Lp and Ls channels is the prolongation of channel open times. Although the comparison of agonist-containing and agonist-free recordings (Fig. 5) demonstrated that the overall pattern of channel activation did not depend on the presence of agonist, the shape of the ensemble average currents would be altered by the presence of long-duration tail currents, particularly for Ls channels. To assess the contribution of prolonged tail currents, the ensemble average responses of Ls and Lp channels were constructed separately with and without the addition of tail currents (Fig. 8A–D, bottom traces). The average currents with and without tail openings were only slightly different for Lp channels. This is consistent with the general gating behavior of Lp channels because activity during the repolarization period arises predominantly from reopenings and not tail currents. The Ls channel average currents, however, were more affected by the removal of tail currents. The response to the initial burst of action potentials was much smaller. This again was a straightforward prediction from Ls channel responses in which tail currents are exclusively responsible for repolarization period activity and, in this case, plateau period activity.

To compare Lp and Ls channel responses to the burst–plateau stimuli quantitatively, we measured open probability at the plateau voltage of −30 mV and compared this with channel open probability seen in response to the square pulse to −30 mV (Fig.9A). This analysis substantiated two points. First, Lp channels responded much more robustly during the entire stimulus than did Ls channels (p < 0.01 for both types of stimuli). Second, a −30 mV depolarization was more effective at activating Lp channels when it was preceded by a short burst of action potentials at 100 Hz (p < 0.05). Ls channels, however, responded similarly to a voltage of −30 mV regardless of whether it was preceded by the initial burst of action potentials (p > 0.2). If anything, there was a slight tendency for Ls channels to respond less well to a −30 mV depolarization when it was preceded by action potentials (Fig. 9A).

Fig. 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 9.

Action potential bursts enhance Lp channel activity during plateau depolarizations. A, Quantification of open probability during the plateau portion of the stimuli shown in Figure 8, A and B, is presented. Lp channel open probability during the plateau portion of the stimulus was significantly greater when preceded by a burst of action potentials. Ls channel open probability was not different between the two conditions. Lp channel powas significantly greater than Ls channel pofor both conditions (p < 0.05). B, The number of Lp channel openings during the plateau depolarization was somewhat larger, but was not significantly greater, for stimuli with the action potential burst. For an accurate comparison, an equal amount of time was analyzed for the two types of stimuli. C, Analysis of the null sweep probability for Lp channels did not show a significant difference for the stimuli with and without the action potential burst. D, E, Comparison of the open-state dwell time duration for Lp channels during the plateau depolarization is shown. The distributions for Lp opening resulting from stimuli with (D) and without (E) the initial burst of action potentials were significantly different (Kolmogorov– Smirnov two-sample test, p < 0.01).

To gain a better understanding of the mechanism by which the burst of action potentials increased the Lp channel response to the −30 mV plateau voltage, we quantified the number of openings and the percentage of null sweeps (pnull) for Lp channels (Fig.9B,C). There was not a significant difference in these two parameters between the two stimulus conditions (p > 0.1). There was, however, a tendency for stimuli with action potentials to elicit more Lp channel openings during the plateau voltage and to produce fewer null sweeps than produced by stimuli without action potentials. Thus the effect of the action potential burst on Lp channel open probability could not be definitively attributed to an increase in the number of openings or a change in availability.

In the absence of a clear effect of the initial action potential burst on channel-opening frequency or availability, we also examined the possibility that the increase in current seen with burst–plateau stimuli could be attributed to an increase in channel open time (Fig.9D). With action potential bursts, the extracted mean open times at −30 mV for Lp channels were 0.8, 7.2, and 19.4 msec with relative proportions of 0.08, 0.38, and 0.53, respectively. With the −30 mV step pulse alone, the open times were 0.9, 9.9, and 22.9 msec with relative proportions of 0.33, 0.36, and 0.3, respectively. Thus the inclusion of the 150 msec train of action potentials before the −30 mV depolarization causes a significant shift in the observed frequency of long openings versus short ones (Kolmogorov–Smirnov two-sample test, p < 0.01). The mean open times themselves, however, were not different. Taken together, these data suggest that the 150 msec burst of action potentials substantially enhanced the entire response to the 850 msec plateau voltage by causing a shift to a longer-opening state coupled with a potential increase in the number of openings.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we have examined the effects of action potential burst stimuli on the activity of the Lp and Ls subtypes of L-type calcium channels. The rationale was to study physiologically relevant stimuli of the type traditionally used to elicit changes in synaptic strength. Action potential trains of different frequency and duration were tested for their ability to elicit openings and reopenings of the two channel types. All of the stimuli evoked openings of both channel types during the action potential train itself. Detailed measurement of activity during repolarization revealed that trains of action potentials elicited bouts of Lp channel reopenings that increased with increasing frequency. Ls channel tail currents were also seen, but at a dramatically lower level than was seen with Lp channel reopenings. Trains of longer duration but with the same frequency also evoked greater Lp channel reopenings and increased the frequency with which Ls channel tail currents were seen. None of the stimuli tested, however, induced reopening of Ls channels as identified by their characteristic long opening in the presence of agonist. Therefore, trains of action potentials effectively activated both Ls and Lp channels but produced more sustained Lp channel activity, whereas Ls channel activity ended after the action potential train ended. Thus, Lp channel activity remained elevated when the driving force on calcium ions was the highest.

Trains of individual action potentials of the type described above can be related easily to presynaptic activity during tetanic stimulation. There is, however, good reason to expect that Lp and Ls channels are more likely to be localized to proximal dendrites and somata than to axonal terminals (Jones et al., 1989; Westenbroek et al., 1990; Hell et al., 1993; Luebke et al., 1993; Magee and Johnston, 1995; Kavalali et al., 1997b; Kapur et al., 1998). Lp and Ls channels could conceivably experience action potential trains as a result of active back-propagation into a dendrite (for review, see Stuart et al., 1997), especially under conditions that reduce decrement during the train (Colbert et al., 1997; Tsubokawa and Ross, 1997). Trains of somatically induced action potentials have been shown to produce dramatic increases in intracellular calcium (Jaffe et al., 1992), which are consistent with the properties of Lp channels described here.

A second way in which we examined voltage changes experienced by postsynaptic Lp channels was by simulating postsynaptic responses to a burst of presynaptic action potentials. Stimuli modeled after published postsynaptic recordings of neuronal activity during stimulus trains consisted of an initial burst of action potentials followed by a plateau voltage that was sustained for the duration of the action potential firing. This stimulus elicited strong activation of Lp channels but relatively modest activity in Ls channels. Particularly evident was the difference in open probability during the plateau portion of the stimulus. Ls channels were virtually silent, whereas Lp channels showed openings that continued for the duration of the stimulus. In addition, Lp channels also showed reopenings after cessation of the stimulus. Therefore, the overall activation of Lp channels was substantially greater than that of Ls channels for a stimulus designed to simulate a postsynaptic response to a high-frequency train of presynaptic action potentials.

Comparison of responses to burst–plateau stimuli and to step depolarizations to the plateau voltage revealed that the initial burst of action potentials significantly augmented the activity of Lp channels. The increase in open probability could be attributed to a shift from short- to longer-duration openings. A similar phenomenon has been reported for anomalous L-type channels in spinal motor neurons (Hivert et al., 1999). In those studies, depolarizations of increasing voltage or duration increased the probability of observing long with respect to short reopenings. Functionally, this increases the responsiveness of Lp channels to physiologically relevant stimuli in which a steady depolarization is preceded by an initial burst of action potentials.

The results of this study suggest that of the known DHP-sensitive calcium channels, the one more likely to participate in the induction of the NMDA receptor-independent LTP would be the Lp channel. This conclusion is based on a recent study showing that NMDA receptor-independent LTP is induced postsynaptically (Yeckel et al., 1999), meaning that our stimulus that simulates a postsynaptic response to tetanic stimulation provides the most meaningful comparison between Ls and Lp channels. For that stimulus, Lp channels showed much larger and long-lasting activity than did Ls channels, thus providing a far more substantial calcium influx. Our comparisons of agonist-containing and agonist-free conditions suggest that although the absolute levels of activity would likely be smaller under physiological conditions, the relative contributions of Lp and Ls channels should remain unchanged.

It has been shown that calcium influx through dihydropyridine-sensitive calcium channels can potentiate synaptic transmission in hippocampal neurons under a variety of conditions, including TEA-induced LTP (Huang and Malenka, 1993) and short-term potentiation induced by repeated postsynaptic depolarization (Kullmann et al., 1992). Although typically associated with the hippocampal mossy fiber-to-CA3 pyramidal neuron synapse, L-type channel-dependent LTP has also been found in the hippocampal CA3-to-CA1 pyramidal neuron synapse (Grover and Teyler, 1990) in which it is coexpressed with NMDA receptor-dependent LTP (Morgan and Teyler, 1999). An interesting feature of calcium channel-dependent LTP is its age dependence, particularly with regard to age-related learning disorders. It has been shown that systemic administration of nimodipine can reverse a performance decrement in aged animals on a conditioned eye-blink task (Deyo et al., 1989). This decrease in performance has been correlated with increased calcium current in hippocampal pyramidal neurons (Pitler and Landfield, 1990;Moyer and Disterhoft, 1994; Campbell et al., 1996) and has been specifically correlated with L-type calcium channel reopenings at hyperpolarized voltages (Thibault and Landfield, 1996). It has also been shown that the relative contribution of NMDA receptor-independent LTP increases with age in hippocampal CA1 neurons (Shankar et al., 1998) and that there is an age-dependent modulation of the induction of LTP and long-term depression (Coussens et al., 1997; Norris et al., 1998). If one assumes that these functions result from the activity of Lp channels, then a detailed understanding of this subtype becomes especially important to elucidating the basis of these age-related changes.

In addition to synaptic plasticity, there is considerable evidence of the involvement of L-type channels in electrophysiological phenomena such as motor neuron plateau potentials (for review, see Kiehn and Eken, 1998), afterhyperpolarizations (for review, see Sah, 1996), and opioid enhancement of intracellular calcium oscillations (Przewlocki et al., 1999). L-type channels have also been implicated in a variety of constitutive functions such as neuronal survival and differentiation (Shitaka et al., 1996; Brosenitsch et al., 1998) and regulation of gene transcription (for review, see Bito et al., 1997), especially with regard to cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) phosphorylation (Murphy et al., 1991; Impey et al., 1996; Liu and Graybiel, 1996, 1998;Cigola et al., 1998; Shieh et al., 1998; Tao et al., 1998; Rajadhyaksha et al., 1999). There is, at present, no definitive proof for differential involvement of one L-type channel subtype over the other in these functions. It has been suggested, however, that L-type calcium channels can serve as a “kinetic filter” responsible for discriminating synaptic from action potential-mediated calcium influx (Mermelstein et al., 2000), a finding that provides a mechanistic basis for previous work showing that synaptic activity is much more effective than action potential firing at causing rapid, calcium-dependent phosphorylation of CREB (Deisseroth et al., 1996). One critical feature of the L-type calcium current responsible for this phenomenon was low activation voltage—a key characteristic that distinguishes Lp channels from Ls channels. This difference is especially clear in Figure 8 in which a model postsynaptic response evokes far greater activity of Lp channels than of Ls channels. Thus it is conceivable that Lp channels may be used as a means for enhancing calcium entry in response to synaptic input.

The molecular nature of Lp channel gating is currently unknown. Reopenings have been observed under a variety of circumstances (Fisher et al., 1990; Forti and Pietrobon, 1993; Thibault et al., 1993;Kavalali and Plummer, 1994; Cloues et al., 1997) and attributed to factors such as recovery from inactivation (Slesinger and Lansman, 1991, 1996), entry and exit into a nonabsorbing closed state (Forti and Pietrobon, 1993; Hivert et al., 1999), and altered voltage dependence (Kavalali and Plummer, 1996). There is substantial evidence, however, that reopenings are not a uniform property of L-type channels and instead reflect activity of a functionally distinct subtype of channel (Kavalali et al., 1997a; Hivert et al., 1999). It is clear that there is sufficient diversity in L-type channel α subunits to support distinctive gating patterns (for review, see Randall and Benham, 1999). Several alternative splicing products have been identified (Snutch et al., 1991; Soldatov, 1994) that have been shown to vary in features such as sensitivity to dihydropyridine antagonists (Soldatov et al., 1995; Zuhlke et al., 1998). There is also diversity in the ancillary β and α2δ subunit gene products and alternative splice products (Kim et al., 1992). A final determination awaits successful cloning and expression of calcium channel activity that reproduces the characteristics of Lp channel reopenings.

Footnotes

  • This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant NS 34061. We are grateful to Drs. R. L. Davis and S. B. Auerbach for helpful discussions and comments on this manuscript.

    Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Mark R. Plummer, Rutgers University, Nelson Laboratories, 604 Allison Road, Piscataway, NJ 08854-8082. E-mail: mplummer{at}rci.rutgers.edu.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Bito H,
    2. Deisseroth K,
    3. Tsien RW
    (1997) Ca2+-dependent regulation in neuronal gene expression. Curr Opin Neurobiol 7:419–429.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bliss TV,
    2. Collingridge GL
    (1993) A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361:31–39.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Brosenitsch TA,
    2. Salgado-Commissariat D,
    3. Kunze DL,
    4. Katz DM
    (1998) A role for L-type calcium channels in developmental regulation of transmitter phenotype in primary sensory neurons. J Neurosci 18:1047–1055.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Campbell LW,
    2. Hao SY,
    3. Thibault O,
    4. Blalock EM,
    5. Landfield PW
    (1996) Aging changes in voltage-gated calcium currents in hippocampal CA1 neurons. J Neurosci 16:6286–6295.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Cigola E,
    2. Volpe BT,
    3. Lee JW,
    4. Franzen L,
    5. Baker H
    (1998) Tyrosine hydroxylase expression in primary cultures of olfactory bulb: role of L-type calcium channels. J Neurosci 18:7638–7649.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Cloues RK,
    2. Tavalin SJ,
    3. Marrion NV
    (1997) ∃-Adrenergic stimulation selectively inhibits long-lasting L-type calcium channel facilitation in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 17:6493–6503.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Colbert CM,
    2. Magee JC,
    3. Hoffman DA,
    4. Johnston D
    (1997) Slow recovery from inactivation of Na+ channels underlies the activity-dependent attenuation of dendritic action potentials in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 17:6512–6521.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Collingridge GL,
    2. Kehl SJ,
    3. McLennan H
    (1983) Excitatory amino acids in synaptic transmission in the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol (Lond) 334:33–46.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Colquhoun D,
    2. Sigworth FJ
    (1983) Fitting and statistical analysis of single-channel records. in Single-channel recording, eds Sakmann B, Neher E (Plenum, New York), pp 191–263.
  10. ↵
    1. Coussens CM,
    2. Kerr DS,
    3. Abraham WC
    (1997) Glucocorticoid receptor activation lowers the threshold for NMDA-receptor-dependent homosynaptic long-term depression in the hippocampus through activation of voltage-dependent calcium channels. J Neurophysiol 78:1–9.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Deisseroth K,
    2. Bito H,
    3. Tsien RW
    (1996) Signaling from synapse to nucleus: postsynaptic CREB phosphorylation during multiple forms of hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Neuron 16:89–101.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Deyo RA,
    2. Straube KT,
    3. Disterhoft JF
    (1989) Nimodipine facilitates associative learning in aging rabbits. Science 243:809–811.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Fisher RE,
    2. Gray R,
    3. Johnston D
    (1990) Properties and distribution of single voltage-gated calcium channels in adult hippocampal neurons. J Neurophysiol 64:91–104.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Forti L,
    2. Pietrobon D
    (1993) Functional diversity of L-type calcium channels in rat cerebellar neurons. Neuron 10:437–450.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Grover LM,
    2. Teyler TJ
    (1990) Two components of long-term potentiation induced by different patterns of afferent activation. Nature 347:477–479.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Hamill OP,
    2. Marty A,
    3. Neher E,
    4. Sakmann B,
    5. Sigworth FJ
    (1981) Improved patch-clamp techniques for high-resolution current recording from cells and cell-free membrane patches. Pflügers Arch 391:85–100.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Harris EW,
    2. Ganong AH,
    3. Cotman CW
    (1984) Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus involves activation of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors. Brain Res 323:132–137.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Hell JW,
    2. Westenbroek RE,
    3. Warner C,
    4. Ahlijanian MK,
    5. Prystay W,
    6. Gilbert MM,
    7. Snutch TP,
    8. Catterall WA
    (1993) Identification and differential subcellular localization of the neuronal class C and class D L-type calcium channel alpha 1 subunits. J Cell Biol 123:949–962.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Hivert B,
    2. Luvisetto S,
    3. Navangione A,
    4. Tottene A,
    5. Pietrobon D
    (1999) Anomalous L-type calcium channels of rat spinal motoneurons. J Gen Physiol 113:679–694.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Huang YY,
    2. Malenka RC
    (1993) Examination of TEA-induced synaptic enhancement in area CA1 of the hippocampus: the role of voltage-dependent Ca2+ channels in the induction of LTP. J Neurosci 13:568–576.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Impey S,
    2. Mark M,
    3. Villacres EC,
    4. Poser S,
    5. Chavkin C,
    6. Storm DR
    (1996) Induction of CRE-mediated gene expression by stimuli that generate long-lasting LTP in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Neuron 16:973–982.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Jaffe DB,
    2. Johnston D,
    3. Lasser-Ross N,
    4. Lisman JE,
    5. Miyakawa H,
    6. Ross WN
    (1992) The spread of Na+ spikes determines the pattern of dendritic Ca2+ entry into hippocampal neurons. Nature 357:244–246.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Johnston D,
    2. Williams S,
    3. Jaffe D,
    4. Gray R
    (1992) NMDA-receptor-independent long-term potentiation. Annu Rev Physiol 54:489–505.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Jones OT,
    2. Kunze DL,
    3. Angelides KJ
    (1989) Localization and mobility of omega-conotoxin-sensitive Ca2+ channels in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Science 244:1189–1193.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Kapur A,
    2. Yeckel MF,
    3. Gray R,
    4. Johnston D
    (1998) L-type calcium channels are required for one form of hippocampal mossy fiber LTP. J Neurophysiol 79:2181–2190.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Kavalali ET,
    2. Plummer MR
    (1994) Selective potentiation of a novel calcium channel in rat hippocampal neurones. J Physiol (Lond) 480:475–484.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Kavalali ET,
    2. Plummer MR
    (1996) Multiple voltage-dependent mechanisms potentiate calcium channel activity in hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 16:1072–1082.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Kavalali ET,
    2. Hwang KS,
    3. Plummer MR
    (1997a) cAMP-dependent enhancement of dihydropyridine-sensitive calcium channel availability in hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 17:5334–5348.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Kavalali ET,
    2. Zhuo M,
    3. Bito H,
    4. Tsien RW
    (1997b) Dendritic Ca2+ channels characterized by recordings from isolated hippocampal dendritic segments. Neuron 18:651–663.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Kiehn O,
    2. Eken T
    (1998) Functional role of plateau potentials in vertebrate motor neurons. Curr Opin Neurobiol 8:746–752.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Kim HL,
    2. Kim H,
    3. Lee P,
    4. King RG,
    5. Chin H
    (1992) Rat brain expresses an alternatively spliced form of the dihydropyridine-sensitive L-type calcium channel alpha 2 subunit. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:3251–3255.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Kullmann DM,
    2. Perkel DJ,
    3. Manabe T,
    4. Nicoll RA
    (1992) Ca2+ entry via postsynaptic voltage-sensitive Ca2+ channels can transiently potentiate excitatory synaptic transmission in the hippocampus. Neuron 9:1175–1183.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Levine ES,
    2. Dreyfus CF,
    3. Black IB,
    4. Plummer MR
    (1995) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor rapidly enhances synaptic transmission in hippocampal neurons via postsynaptic tyrosine kinase receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:8074–8077.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Liu FC,
    2. Graybiel AM
    (1996) Spatiotemporal dynamics of CREB phosphorylation: transient versus sustained phosphorylation in the developing striatum. Neuron 17:1133–1144.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Liu FC,
    2. Graybiel AM
    (1998) Region-dependent dynamics of cAMP response element-binding protein phosphorylation in the basal ganglia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:4708–4713.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Luebke JI,
    2. Dunlap K,
    3. Turner TJ
    (1993) Multiple calcium channel types control glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the hippocampus. Neuron 11:895–902.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Magee JC,
    2. Johnston D
    (1995) Synaptic activation of voltage-gated channels in the dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Science 268:301–304.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Malenka RC,
    2. Nicoll RA
    (1993) NMDA-receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity: multiple forms and mechanisms. Trends Neurosci 16:521–527.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Mermelstein PG,
    2. Bito H,
    3. Deisseroth K,
    4. Tsien RW
    (2000) Critical dependence of cAMP response element-binding protein phosphorylation on L-type calcium channels supports a selective response to EPSPs in preference to action potentials. J Neurosci 20:266–273.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Morgan SL,
    2. Teyler TJ
    (1999) VDCCs and NMDARs underlie two forms of LTP in CA1 hippocampus in vivo. J Neurophysiol 82:736–740.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Moyer JR Jr.,
    2. Disterhoft JF
    (1994) Nimodipine decreases calcium action potentials in rabbit hippocampal CA1 neurons in an age-dependent and concentration-dependent manner. Hippocampus 4:11–17.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Murphy TH,
    2. Worley PF,
    3. Baraban JM
    (1991) L-type voltage-sensitive calcium channels mediate synaptic activation of immediate early genes. Neuron 7:625–635.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Norris CM,
    2. Halpain S,
    3. Foster TC
    (1998) Reversal of age-related alterations in synaptic plasticity by blockade of L-type Ca2+ channels. J Neurosci 18:3171–3179.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Pitler TA,
    2. Landfield PW
    (1990) Aging-related prolongation of calcium spike duration in rat hippocampal slice neurons. Brain Res 508:1–6.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Przewlocki R,
    2. Parsons KL,
    3. Sweeney DD,
    4. Trotter C,
    5. Netzeband JG,
    6. Siggins GR,
    7. Gruol DL
    (1999) Opioid enhancement of calcium oscillations and burst events involving NMDA receptors and L-type calcium channels in cultured hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 19:9705–9715.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Rajadhyaksha A,
    2. Barczak A,
    3. Macias W,
    4. Leveque JC,
    5. Lewis SE,
    6. Konradi C
    (1999) L-type Ca(2+) channels are essential for glutamate-mediated CREB phosphorylation and c-fos gene expression in striatal neurons. J Neurosci 19:6348–6359.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    1. Randall A,
    2. Benham CD
    (1999) Recent advances in the molecular understanding of voltage-gated Ca2+ channels. Mol Cell Neurosci 14:255–272.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Sah P
    (1996) Ca(2+)-activated K+ currents in neurones: types, physiological roles and modulation. Trends Neurosci 19:150–154.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Shankar S,
    2. Teyler TJ,
    3. Robbins N
    (1998) Aging differentially alters forms of long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal area CA1. J Neurophysiol 79:334–341.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Shieh PB,
    2. Hu SC,
    3. Bobb K,
    4. Timmusk T,
    5. Ghosh A
    (1998) Identification of a signaling pathway involved in calcium regulation of BDNF expression. Neuron 20:727–740.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Shitaka Y,
    2. Matsuki N,
    3. Saito H,
    4. Katsuki H
    (1996) Basic fibroblast growth factor increases functional L-type Ca2+ channels in fetal rat hippocampal neurons: implications for neurite morphogenesis in vitro. J Neurosci 16:6476–6489.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    1. Sigworth FJ,
    2. Sine SM
    (1987) Data transformations for improved display and fitting of single-channel dwell time histograms. Biophys J 52:1047–1054.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Slesinger PA,
    2. Lansman JB
    (1991) Reopening of Ca2+ channels in mouse cerebellar neurons at resting membrane potentials during recovery from inactivation. Neuron 7:755–762.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Slesinger PA,
    2. Lansman JB
    (1996) Reopening of single L-type Ca2+ channels in mouse cerebellar granule cells: dependence on voltage and ion concentration. J Physiol (Lond) 491:335–345.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Snutch TP,
    2. Tomlinson WJ,
    3. Leonard JP,
    4. Gilbert MM
    (1991) Distinct calcium channels are generated by alternative splicing and are differentially expressed in the mammalian CNS. Neuron 7:45–57.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Soldatov NM
    (1994) Genomic structure of human L-type Ca2+ channel. Genomics 22:77–87.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Soldatov NM,
    2. Bouron A,
    3. Reuter H
    (1995) Different voltage-dependent inhibition by dihydropyridines of human Ca2+ channel splice variants. J Biol Chem 270:10540–10543.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Stuart G,
    2. Schiller J,
    3. Sakmann B
    (1997) Action potential initiation and propagation in rat neocortical pyramidal neurons. J Physiol (Lond) 505:617–632.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Tao X,
    2. Finkbeiner S,
    3. Arnold DB,
    4. Shaywitz AJ,
    5. Greenberg ME
    (1998) Ca2+ influx regulates BDNF transcription by a CREB family transcription factor-dependent mechanism. Neuron 20:709–726.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Thibault O,
    2. Landfield PW
    (1996) Increase in single L-type calcium channels in hippocampal neurons during aging. Science 272:1017–1020.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  61. ↵
    1. Thibault O,
    2. Porter NM,
    3. Landfield PW
    (1993) Low Ba2+ and Ca2+ induce a sustained high probability of repolarization openings of L-type Ca2+ channels in hippocampal neurons: physiological implications. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90:11792–11796.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Triggle DJ
    (1999) The pharmacology of ion channels: with particular reference to voltage-gated Ca2+ channels. Eur J Pharmacol 375:311–325.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Tsubokawa H,
    2. Ross WN
    (1997) Muscarinic modulation of spike backpropagation in the apical dendrites of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci 17:5782–5791.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Weisskopf MG,
    2. Bauer EP,
    3. LeDoux JE
    (1999) L-type voltage-gated calcium channels mediate NMDA-independent associative long-term potentiation at thalamic input synapses to the amygdala. J Neurosci 19:10512–10519.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Westenbroek RE,
    2. Ahlijanian MK,
    3. Catterall WA
    (1990) Clustering of L-type Ca2+ channels at the base of major dendrites in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Nature 347:281–284.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Yeckel MF,
    2. Kapur A,
    3. Johnston D
    (1999) Multiple forms of LTP in hippocampal CA3 neurons use a common postsynaptic mechanism. Nat Neurosci 2:625–633.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Zheng W,
    2. Rampe D,
    3. Triggle DJ
    (1991) Pharmacological, radioligand binding, and electrophysiological characteristics of FPL 64176, a novel nondihydropyridine Ca2+ channel activator, in cardiac and vascular preparations. Mol Pharmacol 40:734–741.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  68. ↵
    1. Zuhlke RD,
    2. Bouron A,
    3. Soldatov NM,
    4. Reuter H
    (1998) Ca2+ channel sensitivity towards the blocker isradipine is affected by alternative splicing of the human alpha1C subunit gene. FEBS Lett 427:220–224.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 20 (13)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 20, Issue 13
1 Jul 2000
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Sustained Activation of Hippocampal Lp-Type Voltage-Gated Calcium Channels by Tetanic Stimulation
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Sustained Activation of Hippocampal Lp-Type Voltage-Gated Calcium Channels by Tetanic Stimulation
Jessica M. Schjött, Mark R. Plummer
Journal of Neuroscience 1 July 2000, 20 (13) 4786-4797; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-13-04786.2000

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Sustained Activation of Hippocampal Lp-Type Voltage-Gated Calcium Channels by Tetanic Stimulation
Jessica M. Schjött, Mark R. Plummer
Journal of Neuroscience 1 July 2000, 20 (13) 4786-4797; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-13-04786.2000
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • calcium channel
  • hippocampus
  • potentiation
  • long-term potentiation
  • synaptic plasticity
  • dihydropyridine
  • L-type

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

ARTICLE

  • Intracranially Administered Anti-Αβ Antibodies Reduce β-Amyloid Deposition by Mechanisms Both Independent of and Associated with Microglial Activation
  • Neural Correlates of Competing Fear Behaviors Evoked by an Innately Aversive Stimulus
  • Distinct Developmental Modes and Lesion-Induced Reactions of Dendrites of Two Classes of Drosophila Sensory Neurons
Show more ARTICLE

Cellular/Molecular

  • Sex differences in histamine regulation of striatal dopamine
  • Intracellular Spermine Is a Key Player in GSG1L’s Regulation of Calcium-Permeable AMPAR Channel Conductance and Recovery from Desensitization
  • A Critical Role of Neuroligin 2 C-Terminus in OCD and Social Behavior
Show more Cellular/Molecular
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.