Abstract
The developmental refinement of excitatory synapses is often influenced by neuronal activity, and underlying synaptic mechanisms have been suggested. In contrast, few studies have asked whether inhibitory synapses are reorganized during development and whether this is accompanied by use-dependent changes of inhibitory synaptic strength. The topographic inhibitory projection from the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB) to the lateral superior olive (LSO) undergoes synapse elimination during development (Sanes and Takács, 1993). To determine whether there is an associated period of synaptic plasticity, whole-cell recordings were obtained from developing LSO neurons of gerbils in a brain slice preparation. In current-clamp recordings, low-frequency stimulation of the MNTB led to a decline in IPSP amplitude by 43%. In voltage-clamp recordings, hyperpolarized LSO neurons also exhibited a long-lasting depression of MNTB-evoked inhibitory synaptic currents (34%) after low-frequency stimulation. When LSO neurons were depolarized, low-frequency stimulation of the MNTB produced a significantly larger inhibitory synaptic depression (59%). This synaptic plasticity declined dramatically by postnatal days 17–19. Similar to well studied forms of excitatory synaptic plasticity, inhibitory depression depended on postsynaptic calcium. We propose that such activity-dependent synaptic depression may support the developmental rearrangement of inhibitory terminals as they compete with neighboring excitatory and/or inhibitory inputs.
The refinement of excitatory projections has been well described in systems as diverse as the neuromuscular junction and the visual cortex. Furthermore, the formation and elimination of excitatory connections depend, in part, on synaptic transmission. Thus, when neural activity is blocked or altered in the developing visual pathway, excitatory terminals may innervate inappropriate targets (Constantine-Paton et al., 1990; Shatz, 1990). The mechanism that mediates excitatory synaptic refinement is not yet known, although there is strong evidence that NMDA receptors are involved in the activity-dependent stabilization of glutamatergic connections in the CNS (Constantine-Paton and Cline, 1998). At the vertebrate neuromuscular junction, excitatory synapse elimination may rely on a calcium-dependent depression mechanism (Lo and Poo, 1994;Cash et al., 1996; Colman et al., 1997). Despite these tremendous advances in our understanding of excitatory synaptic plasticity in developing systems, there is little information about the activity-dependent refinement of central inhibitory connections. In the present study, we demonstrate use-dependent depression at a set of inhibitory synapses that are known to be refined via a process of synapse elimination (Sanes and Takács, 1993).
One reason that motor neuron and retinal ganglion cell afferents have been so well studied is that they form pure excitatory projections. To test selectively the role of inhibitory transmission during development, it seemed important to identify a similar kind of projection. The lateral superior olivary nucleus (LSO) is a central auditory structure that encodes interaural level differences, an acoustic cue that is used for sound localization. LSO neurons perform this computation by integrating excitatory inputs driven by the ipsilateral ear with inhibitory inputs driven by the contralateral ear (see Fig. 1A). Inhibitory afferents from the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB) form a tonotopic projection in LSO that is aligned with the ipsilateral excitatory projection (Sanes and Rubel, 1988). During postnatal development, individual MNTB arbors become ∼30% more restricted along the frequency axis (Sanes and Siverls, 1991). Furthermore, if MNTB neurons are functionally denervated by removal of the contralateral ear, then the arbors remain in their expanded state, suggesting the involvement of an activity-dependent mechanism (Sanes and Takács, 1993). Recently, it has been found that inhibitory terminals on another binaural nucleus, called the medial superior olivary nucleus (MSO), also undergo a period of synaptic pruning (Kapfer et al., 1999). In this case, inhibitory synapses are initially spread out along MSO dendrites but are restricted to the soma during development, a process that also appears to require activity. Taken together, these data suggest that MNTB synapses may compete with inhibitory and/or excitatory terminals for postsynaptic space during early postnatal development. Therefore, we asked whether inhibitory afferent activity influences the strength of their inhibitory synapses within the LSO. Specifically, we were interested in exploring whether activation of MNTB afferents at low levels could decrease inhibitory transmission in the LSO, because low-frequency stimulation of excitatory afferents has been shown to produce a decline in excitatory synaptic strength (Dudek and Bear, 1992).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
MNTB–LSO slice preparation. Gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) aged postnatal day 7 (P7)–P12 or P17–P19 were used to generate 300 μm transverse brain slices through the LSO and medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (Sanes, 1993). The artificial CSF (ACSF) contained (in mm): 125 NaCl, 4 KCl, 1.2 KH2PO4, 1.3 MgSO4, 26 NaHCO3, 15 glucose, 2.4 CaCl2, and 0.4l-ascorbic acid, pH = 7.3 when bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. ACSF was continuously superfused in the recording chamber at 5 ml/min at room temperature (23–24°C).
Whole-cell electrophysiology and pharmacological manipulations. Initially, whole-cell current-clamp recordings were performed in LSO to examine the influence of prolonged MNTB stimulation (see Fig. 1B; n = 10). Recording electrodes were fabricated from borosilicate glass microcapillaries (1.5 mm outer diameter), and their resistance ranged from 5 to 10 MΩ. Access resistance was balanced throughout the recordings and generally ranged between 15 and 40 MΩ. The internal patch solution contained (in mm): 127.5 potassium gluconate, 0.6 EGTA, 10 HEPES, 2 MgCl2, 5 KCl, 2 ATP, and 0.3 GTP, pH = 7.2.
In the majority of experiments, whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were obtained from LSO neurons at a holding potential of −55 to −90 mV (see Fig. 2; n = 10) or 0 mV (see Fig. 3;n = 21), while electrical stimuli were delivered directly to the MNTB (see Fig. 1A). The wide range of hyperpolarizing holding potentials was chosen to record IPSCs at an operational resting potential level or, if the ISPCs were small, at more hyperpolarized levels to increase the driving force and thus enhance their baseline amplitude. The internal patch solution contained cesium gluconate (127.5 mm) to block most potassium channels, and QX-314 (5 mm) was added to block voltage-dependent sodium channels. The rest of the constituents (in mm) were the same as those in current clamp (pH = 7.2). To decrease postsynaptic free calcium, BAPTA (20 mm) was added to the recording pipette solution in some experiments. This solution was made separately, and the pH was adjusted to 7.2. A Warner Instruments PC-501A was used for recordings, and ∼70% of the access resistance was compensated.
After a whole-cell voltage-clamp recording was obtained (Kotak et al., 1998), a modified ACSF with kynurenic acid (5 mm; pH adjusted to 7.3) was superfused for 6 min before adjusting the holding potential either to −55 to −90 mV or to 0 mV for the duration of the recording. Under these conditions, MNTB-evoked maximum IPSCs were recorded as inward or outward currents, respectively (Kotak et al., 1998). The voltage pulse (200 μsec) that evoked the maximum amplitude IPSC was used for the remainder of the experiment. During a 15 min control period, MNTB-evoked IPSCs were acquired every minute for the first 5 min and then at 10 and 15 min. MNTB was then activated with low-frequency stimulation (LFS; 1 Hz for 15 min). This protocol was chosen because it has been shown previously to induce long-term depression (LTD) of excitatory synapses in the hippocampus (Dudek and Bear, 1992). In addition to hyperpolarized potentials, a holding potential of 0 mV was selected because a similar pairing of depolarization and 1 Hz stimulation was used to study age-dependent plasticity of excitatory connections in barrel cortex (Crair and Malenka, 1995) and robust IPSCs free of EPSCs were recorded previously (Kotak et al., 1998). A small number of P7–P12 neurons (n = 6) exhibited a significant and rapid reduction of MNTB-evoked IPSCs during the initial 15 min control period, and these neurons were excluded from the analysis because we believed this behavior indicated a sudden decline in viability.
Immediately after LFS, MNTB-evoked IPSCs were recorded every minute for the first 5 min and then every 5 min for the next hour. The total duration of the experiments was therefore typically 90 min. In some cases, recording was extended for an additional hour. The IPSC amplitudes and their slopes were analyzed off-line using custom software (Sanes, 1993). In some neurons, to see the effect of the absence of LFS and the interim afferent activity during the 90 min data acquisition, five IPSCs were recorded before and 90 min after holding the cells at VHOLD = −80 mV (n = 6) or VHOLD = 0 mV.
To assess the effect of a potent [Ca2+]i chelator, a separate group of recordings were performed with 20 mmBAPTA (Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR) in the internal recording solution at VHOLD ≤ −55 mV (n = 7) or VHOLD = 0 mV (n = 12).
RESULTS
Current-clamp recordings were obtained from LSO neurons without any pharmacological manipulations, while electrical stimuli were delivered directly to the MNTB (Fig.1A). LFS (1 Hz for 15 min) of the MNTB produced long-lasting depression of the evoked IPSPs. At 60 min after LFS, the IPSP amplitude declined by 43 ± 7% (n = 10; mean ± SEM) when compared with the initial IPSP amplitude (Fig. 1B). The membrane potential of some neurons changed by up to 6 mV after LFS. Therefore, to determine whether depolarization itself could generate a similar IPSP reduction, we raised membrane potential by 10–15 mV (n = 5). Before LFS, the IPSP amplitude increased by 3.2 ± 0.3 mV during membrane depolarization. After LFS, the increase in IPSP amplitude was 1.1 ± 0.3 mV (mean ± SEM). Thus, in both control and depressed conditions, the increased IPSP amplitude during membrane depolarization was approximately proportional to the IPSP amplitude at rest.
To better assess long-lasting depression of MNTB-evoked inhibitory transmission, we performed the remainder of the experiments under voltage-clamp conditions, while ionotropic glutamate receptors were blocked pharmacologically. At a holding potential of −55 to −90 mV, when MNTB was directly stimulated, IPSCs were recorded as inward currents (Fig. 2A). At these hyperpolarized potentials, LFS (1 Hz for 15 min) of the MNTB produced long-lasting depression of the evoked IPSCs (Fig.2B). At 60 min after LFS, the IPSC amplitude declined by 34% when compared with the initial IPSC amplitude (Fig.2B). To determine whether synaptic depression was caused by the LFS and in absence of any afferent activation, recordings were obtained for an equivalent amount of time (90 min) without any stimulation of the MNTB (Fig. 2B,asterisks). The LFS treatment induced a significant depression when compared with these control recordings.
In a separate set of recordings, we determined whether inhibitory synaptic depression was influenced by postsynaptic membrane potential. LSO neurons were held at 0 mV, and electrical stimuli were once again delivered to the MNTB while IPSCs were recorded as outward currents. As shown for one P9 neuron in Figure3A, MNTB-evoked IPSCs were ∼110 pA during the first 15 min of recording but decreased to ∼50 pA after LFS. All 21 neurons recorded from P7 to P12 slices displayed a depression whose magnitude was significantly greater than that recorded at hyperpolarized potentials (see Fig. 6). As shown in Figure 3B, the IPSC amplitude decreased by over 55% at 50–60 min after LFS. There was also a complementary decline in IPSC slope (before LFS, 7.4 ± 0.7 pA/msec; after LFS, 3.5 ± 0.4 pA/msec), indicating that the peak IPSC amplitude and its time to peak had both decreased concomitantly. In five neurons, the recording continued for 2 hr after LFS, and the synaptic depression persisted during this period. When recordings were obtained for 90 min in the absence of LFS to the MNTB (but with continued acquisition of IPSCs), a 21% decrease in IPSC amplitude was observed (Fig. 3B). In addition, there was a small (∼15%), but significant, decline in IPSC amplitude in the absence of any interim MNTB activity (see Figs.3B, 6). The IPSC amplitude in LFS-treated neurons was significantly reduced compared with both control groups (see Fig. 6). The effect was not caused by the particular glutamatergic antagonist used (kynurenic acid) because depression was also observed in the presence of 20 μm CNQX and 40 μm AP-5 (62% depression at 60 min after LFS;n = 2).
If inhibitory synaptic depression is a developmental mechanism that is specifically involved in the refinement of inhibitory MNTB synapses, then we might expect to see a decline in plasticity after anatomical refinement has been primarily completed (Sanes and Siverls, 1991). Therefore LSO neurons were recorded at VHOLD = 0 mV in P17–P19 slices to test the age dependence of synaptic depression. As shown in Figure 4, although LFS produced a decline in IPSC amplitude at P17–P19, this decrease was significantly less when compared with the depression in IPSCs in the P7–P12 group otherwise treated under similar conditions (see Fig. 6).
We next asked whether postsynaptic calcium was necessary to elicit inhibitory synaptic depression. A calcium chelator (BAPTA, 20 mm) was added to the pipette solution and allowed to equilibrate for 5 min before the experiment commenced. We performed the BAPTA manipulation experiments at both hyperpolarized and depolarized holding potentials because depression was recorded in either condition. As shown in Figures 5 and6, this manipulation completely eliminated inhibitory synaptic depression after LFS in P7–P12 neurons. In fact, we observed potentiation of MNTB-evoked IPSCs after LFS in 6 of the 12 neurons recorded at VHOLD = 0 mV.
Finally, we considered the possibility that two factors could contribute to synaptic depression: a general change in the postsynaptic excitability or a depolarizing shift inEIPSC. Alteration of postsynaptic membrane properties could not have produced the depression because the minor change in holding current (IHOLD) during the period of recording was nearly identical for both LFS-treated and control neurons. In fact, the IHOLD decrease was slightly greater for control neurons (control, −48 ± 10 pA; LFS, −35 ± 7 pA; t = 1.019; p = 0.32). Nonetheless, to determine whetherIHOLD contributed to depression, IPSCs were reanalyzed after the holding current was adjusted to its pre-LFS value. This manipulation did not change IPSC amplitude significantly (before LFS, 128 ± 10 pA; after LFS, 56 ± 8 pA; after LFS with adjustment of IHOLD , 74 pA ± 11 pA; t = 2.0; p = 0.16;n = 9). The EIPSC also depolarized slightly during the course of the experiments (before LFS,EIPSC = −40 ± 1.5 mV; after LFS, EIPSC = −33 ± 2.6 mV;t = 2.0; p = 0.02; n = 11). However, we found that there was a significant decline in the inhibitory conductance after LFS, suggesting that the driving force (EHOLD −EIPSC) played only a minor role in depression (before LFS, 4.7 ± 0.8 pS; after LFS, 2.3 ± 0.2 pS; t = 2.08; p = 0.05;n = 11). In fact, inhibitory depression of >50% was observed in three LSO neurons without any shift inEIPSC.
DISCUSSION
Our study attempts to bridge previous data showing that inhibitory synapses are rearranged in the developing central auditory system (Sanes and Takács, 1993) with a candidate mechanism, long-lasting depression of synaptic transmission. There are several lines of evidences that synaptic depression contributes to the developmental reduction of polyneuronal innervation at the nerve–muscle junction. Electrical stimulation of one set of motor axons can speed up the process of synapse elimination and produce a weakening of unstimulated terminals (O'Brien et al., 1978; Ridge and Betz, 1984; Balice-Gordon and Lichtman, 1994; Lo and Poo, 1994). This mechanism appears to be dependent on a postsynaptic rise in calcium (Connold et al., 1986; Cash et al., 1996). LTD of excitatory synapses is also well characterized in the CNS (Linden and Connor, 1995), and an age-dependent decline of excitatory LTD has been demonstrated (Battistin and Cherubini, 1994; Dudek and Friedlander, 1996; Feldman et al., 1998). For example excitatory LTD is present in layer IV of the visual cortex in juvenile cats and guinea pigs but is virtually absent in adult animals (Dudek and Friedlander, 1996).
In the LSO, several experimental findings suggest that spontaneous inhibitory activity influences the maturation of neuronal morphology and physiology within the LSO, including the expression of NMDA receptors (Sanes et al., 1992; Kotak and Sanes, 1996; Sanes and Hafidi, 1996). The present results are broadly consistent with the remodeling of inhibitory synaptic pathways during development (Sanes and Siverls, 1991; Sanes and Takács, 1993). Inhibitory depression recorded under either current- (Fig. 1) or voltage-clamp (Figs. 2-6) conditions, and at different holding potentials, supports the hypothesis that this phenomenon may be an integral element of inhibitory synaptogenesis. In other forms of inhibitory plasticity that decrease GABAergic transmission, glutamate may mediate depolarization-induced suppression of inhibition via a metabotropic signaling at the interneuron–Purkinje cell synapse (Glitsch et al., 1996) or at the hippocampal CA1 neurons (Morishita et al., 1998). In our experiments, inhibitory depression was maximized by a postsynaptic depolarization and high intracellular calcium.
Inhibitory synaptic depression was strongest in P7–P12 LSO neurons (Figs. 1-3), near the onset of response to airborne sound (Finck et al., 1972; Harris and Dallos, 1984; Woolf and Ryan, 1985; Sanes and Siverls, 1991). By P17–P19, the magnitude of inhibitory depression had declined to less than one-half the magnitude observed in younger animals (Fig. 4). Thus, the age at which inhibitory depression was observed correlates rather precisely with the period of normal inhibitory synapse elimination (Sanes and Siverls, 1991). If use-dependent synaptic depression is to have an effect on inhibitory synaptogenesis, then it is important to determine whether neural activity exists during the period of inhibitory synapse elimination. We have shown previously that spontaneous activity is present in the developing gerbil auditory nervous system, but at a relatively low level (Kotak and Sanes, 1995). In vivo recordings confirm that inhibitory synaptic drive is present by postnatal day 13 (Sanes and Rubel, 1988). In addition, the rate of spontaneous inhibitory synaptic currents can be dramatically enhanced by serotonin (Fitzgerald and Sanes, 1999). Thus, inhibitory depression could be elicited by either spontaneous or driven inhibitory synaptic activity in vivo.
Inhibitory synaptic depression occurred after a relatively brief (1 Hz; 15 min) train of stimuli to the entire MNTB afferent population. As shown in Figure 2, the effect was observed when LSO neurons were held at a potential equal to, or more hyperpolarized than, their normal resting membrane potential (less than or equal to −55 mV). The depression was clearly related to synapse activity in that nonstimulated preparations displayed no depression after 90 min of recording (Fig. 2). Therefore, it is possible the inhibitory synapses compete with one another on the basis of their ability to evoke a postsynaptic response. When LSO neurons were depolarized to 0 mV, LFS produced a significantly greater depression of the evoked IPSCs (Fig.3). This result suggests that excitatory synaptic activity may actually facilitate the inhibitory depression mechanism.
Intracellular free calcium was a necessary component of inhibitory synaptic depression, evoked at either hyperpolarized or depolarized membrane potentials (Fig. 5). Addition of the calcium-chelating agent BAPTA to the pipette solution resulted in the complete absence of depression after LFS. In fact, there was some indication that very low postsynaptic calcium levels were associated with a potentiation of the evoked IPSC. A 20% increase in the IPSC amplitude was observed at 90 min when LFS was delivered in the presence of intracellular BAPTA (Fig.5).
Although calcium was involved in synaptic depression at 0 or less than or equal to −55 mV, it is likely that the mechanism of calcium entry or mobilization differs under these two conditions. For example, neonatal inhibitory synapses have been shown to evoke depolarizing postsynaptic potentials because the chloride extrusion mechanism is not yet mature (Payne et al., 1996; Plotkin et al., 1997; Backus et al., 1998; Ehrlich et al., 1999; Kakazu et al., 1999; Lu et al., 1999;Rivera et al., 1999; Williams et al., 1999). Depolarizing IPSPs only are observed until approximately P7 in the rat LSO (Kandler and Friauf, 1995). In this study, persistent activation of inhibitory synapses does not lead to an inversion of the chloride gradient because IPSPs remained hyperpolarizing even after LFS (Fig. 1). A direct increase of postsynaptic calcium by inhibitory activity (Connor et al., 1987;Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Ito and Cherubini, 1991; Obrietan and Van den Pol, 1995; Boehm et al., 1997; Lo et al., 1998; Kullman and Kandler, 1999) after P8 in the gerbil LSO thus appears an unlikely mechanism for calcium-mediated depression, although the blockade of depression by BAPTA suggests that intracellular free calcium is a necessary element (Fig. 5).
Although the inefficient space clamp of distal LSO neuron dendrites during hyperpolarizing holding potential commands at the soma must be kept in mind, it is difficult to explain how the LSO calcium concentration could be increased when the membrane potential is held at a hyperpolarized level. One other possibility is that the inhibitory synapses activate a metabotropic pathway. For example, we have reported previously that inhibitory synapses are predominantly GABAergic at P3–P5 and become primarily glycinergic by P16 (Kotak et al., 1998). Furthermore, functional GABAB receptors are present during the ages when GABA is released. It is also known that MNTB neurons contain BDNF and that LSO neurons express the cognate receptor TrkB during development (Hafidi et al., 1996; Hafidi, 1999). Although we have no firm candidate at this time, it seems plausible that inhibitory synapses may do more than simply open ligand-gated chloride channels.
Modulation of inhibitory synaptic strength has been reported in several systems. For example, there is short-term facilitation or depression of IPSPs in P20–P40 auditory cortex (Buonomano and Merzenich, 1998) and inhibitory long-term potentiation (LTP) and LTD in the hippocampus (Morishita and Sastry, 1991). In goldfish, inhibitory inputs show LTP (Oda et al., 1995, 1998). Although there are a few accounts of inhibitory synaptic plasticity in the developing nervous system (Komatsu, 1994), these are not known to be associated with a developmental rearrangement of inhibitory terminals. Furthermore, it is not clear how the timing of excitatory and inhibitory events might influence the strength of either afferent projection. The relative timing of excitatory synaptic events can influence whether excitatory LTD or LTP is observed. In the Xenopus retinotectal system, an EPSC that occurs ∼20 msec before a postsynaptic action potential is potentiated, whereas an EPSC that occurs ∼20 msec after the action potential is depressed (Zhang et al., 1998).
We therefore propose that inhibitory terminals become stabilized when they are active during low postsynaptic calcium. Low intracellular calcium levels could exist because of the lack of excitatory activity, because of the ability of inhibitory terminals to hyperpolarize the membrane potential during brief activation (Callaway et al., 1995; Lo et al., 1998), or in much older animals (>P20) because their intracellular calcium-buffering efficacy could be greater (Friauf, 1994; Lohmann and Friauf, 1996; Parks et al., 1996; Zettel et al., 1997; Caicedo et al., 1998; Henkel and Brunso-Bechtold, 1998;Iwasaki and Takahashi, 1998). If excitatory terminals are activated first, the subsequent transmission at inhibitory terminals may lead to inhibitory synapse withdrawal. Although the MNTB-evoked IPSC became depressed, we do not yet know whether this represents the selective depression of some inhibitory synapses or the widespread depression of them all.
From a functional standpoint, one of the most intriguing elements of LSO development is the manner in which excitatory and inhibitory afferents become precisely matched, both structurally and functionally, along the tonotopic axis (Sanes and Rubel, 1988; Sanes and Siverls, 1991). Individual MNTB arbors become ∼30% more restricted along the LSO frequency axis during the third postnatal week (Sanes and Siverls, 1991). On the basis of estimates from intracellular recordings, the refinement of MNTB arbors correlates with the loss of ∼40% of the inhibitory inputs to each LSO neuron (Sanes, 1993). Thus, it is possible that inhibitory and excitatory terminals compete for the same postsynaptic space. For example, when sound is located closer to the “inhibitory” ear, the MNTB terminals responding to a specific frequency are activated first, and it is these terminals that must suppress discharge in the LSO neuron. Inhibitory terminals that are activated at a longer latency, after the excitatory terminals have depolarized the LSO neuron, may be inappropriate (for example, tuned to the incorrect frequency), and these terminals should be eliminated. Therefore, it will be important to learn about the amount and timing of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission in the developing LSO and the functional changes that are brought about by each.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant DC 00540 (D.H.S.).
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Dan H. Sanes, Center for Neural Science, 4 Washington Place, New York University, New York, NY 10003. E-mail: sanes{at}cns.nyu.edu.