Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
ARTICLE, Behavioral/Systems

Dissociation in Conditioned Dopamine Release in the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell in Response to Cocaine Cues and during Cocaine-Seeking Behavior in Rats

Rutsuko Ito, Jeffrey W. Dalley, Simon R. Howes, Trevor W. Robbins and Barry J. Everitt
Journal of Neuroscience 1 October 2000, 20 (19) 7489-7495; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07489.2000
Rutsuko Ito
1Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jeffrey W. Dalley
1Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Simon R. Howes
1Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Trevor W. Robbins
1Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Barry J. Everitt
1Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The dopaminergic innervation of the nucleus accumbens is generally agreed to mediate the primary reinforcing and locomotor effects of psychostimulants, but there is less consensus on conditioned dopamine (DA) release during drug-seeking behavior. We investigated the neurochemical correlates of drug-seeking behavior under the control of a drug-associated cue [a light conditioned stimulus (CS+)] and to noncontingent presentations of the CS+ in the core and shell subregions of the nucleus accumbens. Rats self-administered cocaine under a continuous reinforcement schedule in which a response on one of two identical levers led to an intravenous cocaine infusion (0.25 mg/infusion) and a 20 sec light CS+. Response requirements for cocaine and the CS+ were then progressively increased until stable responding was established under a second-order schedule of reinforcement. During microdialysis, rats were presented noncontingently with a set of 10 sec CS+ and neutral tone stimuli (CS−) before and after a 90 min period during which they responded for cocaine under a second-order schedule. Results showed the following: (1) nucleus accumbens DA increased in both the core and shell during intravenous cocaine self-administration; (2) noncontingent presentations of a cocaine-associated CS+ led to increased DA release selectively in the nucleus accumbens core; and (3) extracellular DA levels were unaltered in both core and shell during a protracted period of drug-seeking behavior under the control of the same cocaine-associated cue. These results indicate that the mesolimbic dopamine system is activated after exposure to drug-associated stimuli under specific conditions.

  • cocaine
  • dopamine
  • nucleus accumbens
  • second-order schedule
  • conditioned reinforcement
  • microdialysis

Exposure to environmental stimuli associated with the effects of stimulant drugs can evoke intense craving and cause relapse in detoxified and abstinent addicts (O'Brien et al., 1990, 1998; Ehrman et al., 1992; Tiffany and Carter, 1998). Such stimuli, through pavlovian conditioning, acquire conditioned reinforcing and incentive motivational properties and are thereby able to generate and maintain drug-seeking behavior (Davis and Smith, 1979; de Wit and Stewart, 1981; Stewart et al., 1984; Phillips and Fibiger, 1990). The mesolimbic dopamine (DA) system innervating the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) is implicated in the primary reinforcing and locomotor effects of psychostimulants (Roberts et al., 1980; Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988; Hurd et al., 1989; Robledo et al., 1992), as well as in potentiating the conditioned reinforcing properties of reward-associated stimuli (Taylor and Robbins, 1984, 1986). In vivo monitoring studies (Fontana et al., 1993; Gratton and Wise, 1994; Kiyatkin and Stein, 1996; Di Ciano et al., 1998) have reported conditioned DA release in the NAcc in response to exposure to environmental or discrete drug-associated stimuli in rats, but there have also been failures to observe such conditioned neurochemical effects, despite evidence of behavioral activation (Barr et al., 1983;Brown and Fibiger, 1992) or concurrent operant drug-seeking behavior (Neisewander et al., 1996; Bradberry et al., 2000). Such apparent discrepancies may have arisen because of differences or limitations in the techniques and protocols used or because of a failure to take into account the anatomical and functional heterogeneity of the NAcc when placing dialysis probes or electrodes.

The elucidation of neurochemical responses associated specifically with craving or drug-seeking has also been limited by self-administration protocols that fail to distinguish clearly between drug-seeking and drug-taking behavior. Self-administration procedures using low fixed ratio (FR) or progressive ratios of reinforcement cannot clearly dissociate cue-elicited neurochemical responses from drug-induced responses, because the period of responding before any drug infusion is necessarily very brief. In contrast, the use of a second-order schedule of drug reinforcement (Goldberg, 1973; Whitelaw et al., 1996; Arroyo et al., 1998) helps to overcome this problem. Thus, in a typical second-order schedule, a drug-paired light conditioned stimulus (CS+) is presented contingent on a fixed ratio of lever pressing, and a high rate of such responding can be maintained for prolonged periods of time until the drug itself becomes available, upon completion of a specific number of FR units of responses, after a fixed interval (FI) has elapsed, or both. This allows the investigation of drug-seeking behavior that is both under the control of drug-associated conditioned stimuli and also unconfounded by the reinforcing and response rate-altering effects of the self-administered drug (Goldberg and Tang, 1977; Arroyo et al., 1998).

The NAcc, although a primary site mediating the reinforcing effects of psychomotor stimulants, is also a heterogenous structure with at least two anatomically and functionally distinct subregions: a medial and ventral “shell” region and a more lateral “core” region. (Zahm and Heimer, 1990; Heimer et al., 1991; Jones et al., 1996; David et al., 1998). It has been suggested that the DA innervation of the shell region is especially responsive to primary rewards, such as food (Tanda et al., 1998; Bassareo and Di Chiara, 1999) and drugs of abuse (Di Chiara et al., 1993; Pontieri et al., 1995; Carlezon and Wise, 1996; Tanda et al., 1997). In contrast, the NAcc core region has been implicated in response-reinforcement (instrumental) learning (Kelley et al., 1997) and in subserving behavioral responses to motivationally significant conditioned stimuli (Bassareo and Di Chiara, 1999; Di Chiara, 1998; Parkinson et al., 1999, 2000). Therefore, in the present study, we have used a second-order schedule of cocaine reinforcement (Arroyo et al., 1998) combined with in vivo microdialysis in both the shell and core subregions of the NAcc to investigate the neurochemical correlates of a protracted period of drug-seeking behavior under the control of a cocaine-associated discrete light stimulus, as well as responses to the same drug cue presented noncontingently.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals. Male Lister hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK), weighing between 290 and 360 gm at the beginning of the experiment, were housed in pairs and then individually after surgery, under a reversed 12 hr light/dark cycle (lights off 9:00 A.M.). Water was available ad libitum, and food was made available immediately after a training session, each animal receiving 20 gm Purina lab chow per day, sufficient to maintain preoperative body weight and growth. All experimental sessions were performed during the dark phase, between 9:00 A.M. and 6:00 P.M., and in accordance with the United Kingdom 1986 Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act Project License 90/1324.

Intracerebral cannulation surgery. Animals were anesthetized with Avertin [10 gm of 99% 2,2,2-tribromoethanol (Sigma-Aldrich, Dorset, UK) in 5 mg of tertiary amyl alcohol and 4.5 ml of PBS (Dulbecco “A”; Unipath Ltd, Basingstoke, UK) in 40 ml of absolute alcohol; 1 ml/100 gm body weight, i.p.]. A unilateral guide cannula (BAS Technicol, Congleton, UK) was then lowered and positioned above the nucleus accumbens shell [anteroposterior (AP), +1.2 mm; lateral (L), ±0.8 mm; ventral (V), −1.75 mm; incisor bar, −3.3 mm] or core (AP, +1.2 mm; L, ±1.6 mm; V, −1.75 mm) subregion and secured to the skull using dental cement, anchored by four stainless steel screws (BAS Technicol). A removable stainless steel stylet, cut flush with the tip of the cannula, was placed inside the cannula to maintain its patency throughout the training period.

Intravenous catheterization. After stereotaxic surgery, rats were allowed a recovery period for at least 5 d with food available ad libitum. They were then anesthetized with Avertin and implanted with chronic intravenous jugular catheters as described previously (Caine et al., 1992). The catheter was inserted into the right jugular vein, secured in place by a suture and Superglue, and was passed subcutaneously over the right shoulder to exit dorsally between the scapulae. Antibiotic treatment [flushing daily with 0.1 ml of Timentin 3.2 g: 200 mg of potassium clavulanate with 3 gm of ticarcillin (Beecham Research, Welwyn, UK); 66 mg/1 ml 0.9% sterile saline (Animal Care Ltd., Dunnington, UK)] was given for 5 d after surgery and was reinstated 3 weeks afterward for 5 d to minimize postoperative primary and secondary infection. Thereafter, before each self-administration session, the animals were flushed with 0.1 ml of sterile 0.9% saline and, at the end of the session, with 0.1 ml of hepranized saline (30 U/ml 0.9% sterile saline; CP Pharmaceuticals Ltd., Wrexham, UK) to maintain catheter patency.

Apparatus. Six operant chambers (24 × 20 × 22 cm; Med Associates, St. Albans, UK) contained within a sound-attenuating box with a ventilating fan were used in the experiment. Each chamber contained a side wall with two 4-cm-wide retractable levers, positioned equidistantly, 10 cm apart and 5 cm from the grid floor. Placed 3 cm above each lever was a round disk (2 cm in diameter) that could be illuminated by a 2.5 W, 24 V light bulb, to serve as a stimulus light. The whole chamber was illuminated by a red 1.8 W, 17 V house light positioned at the top corner of the chamber. The chamber was also equipped with a tone generator (RS Components, Northants, UK) located centrally above the two levers. Intravenous infusions of cocaine were delivered by a software-operated infusion pump (Semat Technical Ltd., St. Albans, UK) placed outside the sound-attenuating box, through a counterbalanced single-channel or a dual-channel liquid swivel and an extra length of plastic tubing enclosed in a metal spring, connecting the swivel to the external guide cannula of the catheter mounted on the animal's back or to the external head mount.

Each session could be initiated manually by three rapid presses on one of the two levers, thereby designating the active or drug lever, as opposed to the second, inactive lever on which responding had no programmed consequence. The active and inactive levers were counterbalanced between rats. The beginning of the session was also marked by illumination of the house light. Subsequent depression of the active lever resulted in the retraction of both levers, extinction of the house light and simultaneous illumination of the drug stimulus light for 20 sec, and the activation of the infusion pump for 4 sec, delivering 0.1 ml of intravenous infusion of cocaine solution (0.25 mg/infusion). On completion of the 20 sec time-out period, the levers were re-extended, the house light was illuminated, and the stimulus light was extinguished. Additional active lever presses resulted in the same sequence of events leading to cocaine infusions.

The apparatus was controlled by an Acorn Archimedes microcomputer (Acorn Computers Ltd., Cambridge, UK) running a program written in the BASIC control language Arachnid.

Drugs. Cocaine hydrochloride (McFarlan-Smith, Edinburgh, UK) was dissolved in sterile 0.9% saline. The dose of cocaine was calculated as the salt.

Self-administration training. Animals were first trained during 2 hr sessions of cocaine self-administration under a continuous reinforcement schedule (fixed ratio 1). Once stable rates of self-administration had been established (10 d), a second-order FRx(FRy:S) schedule of cocaine reinforcement was introduced. Under this schedule, rats were required to makey responses to obtain a single presentation of a 2 sec light CS+, whereas completion of x of these response units resulted in the delivery of cocaine, the illumination of the light CS+ for 20 sec, the retraction of both levers, and extinction of the house light during a 20 sec time-out period. In the initial stage of training, x was set at 5 and y was 1. The value for x was then increased to 10 and remained at this value throughout the training. The value for y was progressively increased from 1 to 10 until stable responding was established at FR10(FR10:S). At this stage, a 2 hr delay period before each daily session was gradually introduced over 10 d for the rats to become accustomed to the baseline collection period during the dialysis experiment. Furthermore, for 3 d before the test day, rats were pre-exposed to noncontingent presentations of a neutral tone stimulus (CS−) to avoid possible effects of novelty on the neurochemical response.

In vivo microdialysis. A 2 mm microdialysis probe (BAS Technicol) was lowered into the nucleus accumbens via the guide cannula ∼18 hr before the start of the experiment. On the test day, the probe was continuously perfused with artificial CSF (aCSF) (in mm: 147 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.3 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 0.2 NaH2PO4, and 1.3 Na2HPO4) at a rate of 2 μl/min. After a 60 min equilibration period, six 10 min baseline samples were collected in plastic vials containing 4 μl of aCSF (Fig. 1). For the next three 10 min samples, the rats received five noncontingent 10 sec light CS presentations at 1 min intervals starting at 50 sec into the 10 min sample. The same pattern of presentation was subsequently repeated with the tone (clicker) for 30 min. A 90 min self-administration session under a second-order fixed interval schedule FI20 min(FR10:S) was then commenced. No priming injections were ever given. In this newly introduced schedule, animals received a cocaine infusion on the completion of the first FR10 responses made after a fixed interval of 20 min had elapsed. The animals could thus self-administer a maximum of four cocaine infusions within 90 min. Using this fixed interval/fixed ratio schedule allowed at least two samples to be obtained during the initial drug-free first interval of drug-seeking behavior. At the end of the session, the levers were retracted, and the house light was extinguished. Nine additional samples were taken for baseline levels to be re-established, and then CS+ and CS− presentations were given in reverse order and an additional six samples were collected. Sampling continued for another 30 min. On completion of testing, animals were returned to their home cages.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

A schematic diagram of the sampling protocol on the dialysis test day. BASAL, Baseline sample collection; SA, self-administration period;CS+, CS+ presentations; CS−, CS− presentations. The whole session lasted 390 min.

HPLC procedure. DA was determined in dialysate samples by HPLC and electrochemical detection. Separation was achieved by using a Hypersil analytical column (100 × 4.6 mm inner diameter, 3 μm) and a mobile phase consisting of 8.82 gm/l trisodium citrate, 2.03 gm/l NaH2PO4, 500 mg/l Na-1-octane sulfonic acid, 22.5% methanol, 25 mg/l EDTA, and 1 ml/l triethylamine, pH 2.7 adjusted using orthophosphoric acid. DA was detected by oxidation using a Coulochem II detector (ESA 5014) equipped with a guard cell (+300 mV) and a dual electrode analytical cell (E1, −150 mV; E2, +150 mV). Chromatographic data were acquired and processed using Gyncosoft V4.4. The detection limit of DA in aqueous standards was ∼2 fmol on column.

Histological assessment of microdialysis probe placement.Within a week after the completion of the testing, rats were deeply anesthetized with Euthatal (sodium pentobarbitone, 200 mg/ml) and perfused with 0.9% PBS followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were then removed, stored in PFA, and transferred to a 20% sucrose cryoprotectant solution the day before sectioning. Coronal sections (60 μm) of the brain were cut and stained with cresyl violet for verification of probe placement.

Data analysis. All analyses were conducted using GBStat (V3.0; Dynamic Microsystems Inc.). Neurochemical data were analyzed using two-way repeated measures ANOVA, with region (two levels: core and shell) as the between subjects factor and time bin as the repeated within-subjects factor (three levels: basal plus CS1, −50 to 60 min; FI, 70–240 min; CS2 plus basal, 250–330 min). In addition, the 20 min drug-seeking period was analyzed using a two-way ANOVA with region and time as factors. The preceding point (60 min) was included in this analysis. Fisher's least significant difference post hoc test was used for multiple comparisons. Behavioral data were analyzed using two- or three-way repeated measures of ANOVA with region as the between subjects factor and interval and lever as the repeated measures.

RESULTS

Histological assessment of dialysis probe locations

Figure 2 shows a schematic representation of the locations of the 2 mm dialysis probe membrane within the NAcc core (n = 8) and shell (n = 6) subregions. Data from four animals in which probes were placed outside their intended target were excluded from the study.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Schematic representation of the locations of the 2 mm dialysis probe membrane within the nucleus accumbens core (gray; n = 8) and shell (black; n = 6) compartments (distances are in millimeters from the bregma) (adapted from Paxinos and Watson, 1986).

Behavioral data

Figure 3 shows the mean rate of responding on the drug-paired active lever during the four intervals of cocaine self-administration under a FI20(FR10:S) schedule of reinforcement in rats in the NAcc core and shell groups. Every 10th lever press was followed by the presentation of a 2 sec light stimulus (CS+) associated previously with cocaine self-administration during training, and the completion of the first 10 lever responses after an interval of 20 min resulted in an intravenous cocaine infusion (0.25 mg/infusion) that was also paired with a 20 sec CS+ presentation. All animals received the maximum number (four) of infusions within the 90 min session. As shown in Figure 4, the rate of responding on the active lever showed significant increases with each successive interval in both groups of rats (interval effect,F(3,36) = 14.35, p = 0.0001). Although overall responding in the shell group was higher than in the core group of animals, ANOVA showed this was not significant (F(1,12) = 1.82, p = 0.2). Responding on the inactive lever was significantly lower in both groups of rats as shown in Figure 4 (lever effect,F(1,12) = 169, p = 0.0001).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Cumulative response record during the period of responding under a second-order FI20 min(FR10:S) schedule, both before and after the self-administration of cocaine Thearrow represents the delivery of a cocaine infusion (0.75 mg/infusion), which was paired with a 20 sec CS+ presentation.

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Responses on active and inactive levers before (Interval 1) and after (Intervals 2–4) the self-administration of cocaine. Error bars represent the mean ± SEM total of active and inactive lever presses made during each of the four intervals in the shell (black; n = 6) and core (white; n = 8) groups of animals. Every 10th lever press on the active lever resulted in the presentation of a 2 sec light CS+, whereas responding on the inactive lever had no programmed consequence.

Neurochemical data

Basal concentrations of DA

Basal levels of DA in the NAcc core and shell, taken as the mean ± SEM fmol/10 min of the first six values obtained before the presentation of the CS+, were 11.81 ± 0.075 and 11.85 ± 0.12, respectively, and thus were not significantly different between the two regions (F(5,60) = 0.39). Mean changes in the extracellular DA levels in the NAcc core and shell subregions expressed as a percentage of baseline are shown in Figure5, and absolute values are shown in Figure 6.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

Mean changes in extracellular DA levels in the core and shell subregions in the NAcc (expressed as percentage of baseline ± SEM). A significant elevation in DA levels (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01) compared with baseline levels was observed during the first CS+ presentations selectively in the core subregion. No significant changes in extracellular DA were seen in either region during the first 20 min of the self-administration session, which provided a measure of drug-seeking behavior unaffected by cocaine itself (gray shading). The ensuing cocaine infusions caused significant increases in DA levels in both core and shell subregions (arrows represent time points of cocaine infusions).

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

Absolute extracellular DA levels in the core (filled circles) and shell (white circles) subregions during the first set of CS+ presentations and during responding under the second-order schedule both before and after the self-administration of cocaine. The bars indicate the period of CS+ and CS− presentations; 10 sec CS+/CS− were presented five times within a 10 min sample. There was a significant increase in DA levels in the core subregion in response to the CS+ presentations (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01). The drug-seeking period, indicated by gray shading, was not accompanied by a rise in DA levels in either region of the NAcc, whereas during the drug-taking period, significant increases in DA levels were observed in both subregions (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01).

Noncontingent CS+ and CS− presentations

As shown in Figures 5 and 6, the first set of noncontingent presentations of the cocaine-associated light CS+ resulted in a significant 150–200% increase in extracellular DA at time points 10 and 20 min (p < 0.01) selectively in the core subregion (time effect, F(11,132) = 2.363, p < 0.01; region × time interaction,F(11,132) = 2.12, p < 0.02). In contrast, the presentation of a non-cocaine-associated CS− did not elevate extracellular DA in either region.

CS+ presentation contingent on cocaine-seeking behavior

At 60 min, levers were extended into the operant chambers signaling the commencement of the self-administration session. The first 20 min of the session allowed the measure of the neurochemical correlates of cocaine-seeking behavior maintained by the CS+ contingent on lever pressing, unaffected by any pharmacological effects of cocaine. As shown in Figures 5 and 6, there were no significant changes in extracellular DA seen during this period of drug-free cocaine-seeking behavior in either of the NAcc subregions (region,F(1,12) < 0.02; time,F(2,24) < 0.73; region × time,F(2,24) < 0.42).

CS+ presentations contingent on cocaine-seeking behavior after the self-administration of cocaine

After the first cocaine infusion at 90 min, extracellular DA levels were significantly elevated above baseline levels, and this increase was sustained over subsequent cocaine infusions (maximum of four) in the range of 150–250% in the core and 200–310% in the shell (F(17,204) = 7.93,p = 0.0001). Fisher's post hoc analyses revealed that this increase from baseline reached significance at time points 130–160 min in the core (p < 0.01) and at time points 90–180 min in the shell (p < 0.01). There was no interaction between region and time with respect to the dopamine response during the self-administration session (F < 1). Within 50–60 min after the termination of the self-administration period, extracellular DA declined to levels slightly below the initial baseline (73–85% in core group; 93–97% in shell group), but these changes were not statistically different between the two subregions (F = 1.01).

In the final phase of the experiment, presentations of the CS+ and CS− were reversed in order. The increase in extracellular DA (150%) in the core subregion during the passive presentation of the CS+ (Fig.5) was again significantly greater than baseline at time points 290 and 300 min (p < 0.05), whereas those in the shell did not change on presentation of the CS+ (p > 1.0). As before, presentations of the CS− were not accompanied by any change in extracellular DA.

DISCUSSION

Noncontingent presentations of a cocaine-associated cue were associated with significantly increased DA efflux in the core, but not the shell, subregion of the nucleus accumbens. In contrast, presentations of cocaine-associated cues contingent on responding during drug-seeking behavior were not accompanied by any changes in DA efflux. However, after self-administration of cocaine, extracellular DA levels were significantly increased in both the NAcc core and shell subregions.

Unconditioned effects of cocaine

The finding of increased extracellular DA in both the core and shell during cocaine self-administration is in contrast to the findings suggesting preferential responsiveness of the DA innervation of the NAcc shell to psychomotor stimulants and other drugs of abuse (Pierce and Kalivas, 1995; Pontieri et al., 1995; Heidbreder and Feldon, 1998;Cadoni and Di Chiara, 1999). One explanation for these different results may lie in the route and method of drug administration. Thus, in the present study cocaine was self-administered, whereas in the earlier studies the drugs were given noncontingently by the experimenter. However, there are several demonstrations that contingent and noncontingent administration of cocaine differentially affect extracellular DA levels in the nucleus accumbens (Hemby et al., 1997) and amygdala (Wilson et al., 1994), with greater increases occurring after contingent versus noncontingent drug. Cocaine-induced alterations in the activity of NAcc neurons also depend on the passive or active nature of drug administration (Carelli and Deadwyler, 1994). Thus, cocaine self-administration in the present experiment may have allowed increases in extracellular DA in the core more readily to be measured.

Drug-seeking behavior

The failure to observe a DA response during active drug-seeking maintained by conditioned stimuli in the present experiment is consistent with the observation of Neisewander et al. (1996) that response-contingent presentations of a CS+ associated previously with cocaine self-administration in rats responding in extinction failed to induce an increase in DA overflow in the NAcc (probe placements being predominantly in the core subregion). Moreover, drug-seeking triggered by a visual cue in monkeys was also not associated with changes in extracellular DA in the ventral striatum (Bradberry et al., 2000). These data lead to the speculation that the absence of increases in DA levels in the NAcc concomitant with response-contingent presentations of a CS+ could be attributable to the automated, or habitual, nature of responding under the second-order schedule, the control of which has devolved to a neural system not encompassing the NAcc (Robbins and Everitt, 1999). Not only has drug-seeking behavior been suggested to become compulsive and habitual in the transition from drug abuse to addiction (O'Brien and McLellan, 1996; Robbins and Everitt, 1999), but electrophysiological studies have shown that, after extensive over-training, the response of midbrain DA neurons to conditioned stimuli is gradually attenuated (Ljungberg et al., 1992; Redgrave et al., 1999).

These results are therefore inconsistent with the hypothesis that mesolimbic DA is invariably activated during drug-seeking behavior (de Wit and Stewart, 1981; Robinson and Berridge, 1993). Although it has been shown that DA receptor agonists and antagonists can increase or decrease, respectively, drug-seeking responses (Ettenberg et al., 1996;Shaham and Stewart, 1996; DeVries et al., 1999; Pilla et al., 1999) or reinstate such responses after extinction (Wise et al., 1990; Self et al., 1996; McFarland and Ettenberg, 1997), it remains possible that these effects occur relatively early in the acquisition of drug-seeking or drug-taking behavior or are mediated at sites other than the NAcc, such as the amygdala (Hitchcott and Phillips, 1998; Everitt et al., 1999). Moreover, in these studies of reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior, the subjects' responding has been subjected both to a sometimes long period of extinction and also drug withdrawal. Neither condition was present in these studies, and it remains possible that dopaminergic mechanisms in cue-dependent drug-seeking might become more prominent after extinction and withdrawal.

The present data also do not support the suggestion that the magnitude of the DA response in the NAcc is correlated with lever pressing rates, as has been reported under high ratio schedules of food reinforcement (Sokolowski et al., 1998; Salamone et al., 1999). Dopamine depletion from the NAcc induced by 6-hydroxydopamine (Robbins et al., 1983;Sokolowski and Salamone, 1998) or infusions of DA receptor antagonists into the NAcc (Cory-Slechta et al., 1997; Salamone et al., 1999) have been shown to depress instrumental lever pressing, particularly under schedules demanding high response rates. Yet despite the high numbers of responses made by rats under the FI20 min(FR10:S) second-order schedule used in the present study, no increase in extracellular DA in the NAcc was observed until cocaine had been self-administered. These different results may again reflect the degree of pretraining and the extent to which the instrumental behavior has devolved to habit rather than action–outcome control.

Effect of noncontingent CS+ presentations

In contrast to the absence of a measurable DA response in the NAcc during drug-seeking behavior, noncontingent presentations of a cocaine-associated CS+ evoked a selective increase in extracellular DA in the core, but not the shell, of the NAcc. This result extends the observation of Di Ciano et al. (1998) who found conditioned increases in DA oxidation currents in the NAcc in response to passive presentation of a flashing light stimulus that had been paired previously with either self-administered or noncontingent amphetamine administration. Similarly, presentations of a drug-associated CS+ alone before a self-administration session were also accompanied by an increased DA signal in the NAcc (Gratton and Wise, 1994; Kiyatkin and Stein, 1995, 1996).

The selective DA response to noncontingent presentations of the CS+ in the core subregion supports the observation of Bassareo and Di Chiara (1999), who also reported that a CS+ associated with a highly palatable and calorific food elicited strong appetitive behavioral responses and a concomitant rise in DA levels selectively in the core subregion of the NAcc. Moreover, the increased DA efflux in the NAcc core that followed “unexpected” presentations of the CS+ in the present study is consistent with electrophysiological data showing that midbrain DA neurons increased their activity specifically to the unexpected presentation of rewards or conditioned stimuli, whereas such responses were not evident for expected positively reinforcing stimuli, whether conditioned or not (Romo and Schultz, 1990; Schultz et al., 1994;Schultz, 1998).

Pavlovian conditioning, conditioned reinforcement, and drug-seeking behavior

The results of these experiments indicate important and subtle differences in the ways in which drug-associated stimuli influence drug-seeking behavior and the extent to which this involves activation of the mesolimbic DA system. Thus, although the pavlovian CS+ eliciting property of a cocaine-conditioned reinforcer is sufficient to increase DA selectively in the NAcc core, this effect is apparently unnecessary for the maintenance of drug-seeking behavior by the contingent presentation of the stimulus as a conditioned reninforcer. Therefore, this aspect of the maintenance of drug-seeking is independent of the mesolimbic DA system, whereas the primary reinforcing action of cocaine clearly is not. The observation that conditioned reinforcement (i.e., response-produced CS+ presentations) did not increase DA levels in the NAcc during drug-seeking is also consistent with our earlier results that DA depletion from the NAcc (Taylor and Robbins, 1986) or DA receptor blockade in the NAcc (Wolterink et al., 1993) does not decrease the effects of conditioned reinforcers per se but only their amplification by stimulant drugs.

The particular circumstances under which a drug-associated CS+ does affect drug-seeking behavior via activation of the mesolimbic DA system therefore remain uncertain. One circumstance may be the elicitation of drug-seeking or drug-taking behavior because, after extinction, such cues are known to reinstate drug self-administration (de Wit and Stewart, 1981; Stewart et al., 1984) and drug-seeking under a second-order schedule of reinforcement (Arroyo et al., 1998). Moreover,Weiss et al. (2000) have shown that DA is increased in the NAcc and amygdala by exposure to a cocaine cue that also reinstated responding after a prolonged period of extinction of cocaine self-administration. Another possibility, yet to be studied directly, is that unexpected presentation of drug-cues may facilitate, that is increase the rate of, drug-seeking behavior, much as has been demonstrated for the facilitative effect of a pavlovian CS+ on responding for an ingestive reward (Lovibond, 1983). Indeed, this influence of a CS+ on a seeking response for food is abolished by lesions of the NAcc core, but not shell (Hall et al., 1999), and by the DA receptor antagonist pimozide (Dickinson et al., 2000), whereas intra-accumbens infusions ofd-amphetamine potentiate the response-incremental effects of a pavlovian CS+ (Wyvell and Berridge, 2000), much as they potentiate conditioned reinforcers (Taylor and Robbins, 1984).

In summary, this study has helped to resolve apparent discrepancies in the literature in which conditioned stimuli have been reported to increase, or to have no effect on, DA overflow in the NAcc. The present investigation has shown both effects: a lack of impact when the CS+ is acting to maintain drug-seeking behavior under a second-order schedule (analogous to habitual responding postulated in human drug abusers) (O'Brien and McLellan, 1996; Tiffany and Carter, 1998); and a significant increase when the same CS+ is presented noncontingently for the first occasion after a history of contingent presentation, possibly consistent with the postulated role of such stimuli in eliciting drug craving and reinstatement behaviors.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Medical Research Council (MRC) Program Grant G9537855 (to B.J.E. and T.W.R.) and an MRC Cooperative in Brain, Behavior, and Neuropsychiatry.

    Correspondence should be addressed to Prof. B. J. Everitt, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK. E-mail: bje10{at}cus.cam.ac.uk.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Arroyo M,
    2. Markou A,
    3. Robbins TW,
    4. Everitt BJ
    (1998) Acquisition, maintenance and reinstatement of intravenous cocaine self-administration under a second-order schedule of reinforcement in rats: effects of conditioned cues and unlimited access to cocaine. Psychopharmacology 140:331–344.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Barr GA,
    2. Sharpless NS,
    3. Cooper S,
    4. Schiff SR
    (1983) Classical conditioning, decay and extinction of cocaine-induced hyperactivity and stereotypy. Life Sci 33:1341–1351.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bassareo V,
    2. Di Chiara G
    (1999) Differential responsiveness of dopamine transmission to food-stimuli in nucleus accumbens shell/core compartments. Neuroscience 89:637–641.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bradberry CW,
    2. Barrett-Larimore RL,
    3. Jatlow P,
    4. Rubino SR
    (2000) Impact of self-administered cocaine and cocaine cues on extracellular dopamine in mesolimbic and sensorimotor striatum in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci 20:3874–3883.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Brown EE,
    2. Fibiger HC
    (1992) Cocaine-induced conditioned locomotion: absence of associated increases in dopamine release. Neuroscience 48:621–629.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Cadoni C,
    2. Di Chiara G
    (1999) Reciprocal changes in dopamine responsiveness in the nucleus accumbens shell and core and in the dorsal caudate-putamen in rats sensitized to morphine. Neuroscience 90:447–455.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Caine SB,
    2. Lintz R,
    3. Koob GF
    (1992) Intravenous drug self-administration techniques in animals. in Behavioural neuroscience: a practical approach, ed Sahgal A (Oxford UP, Oxford), pp 117–143.
  8. ↵
    1. Carelli RM,
    2. Deadwyler SA
    (1994) A comparison of nucleus accumbens neuronal firing patterns during cocaine self-administration and water reinforcement in rats. J Neurosci 14:7736–7746.
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    1. Carlezon WA,
    2. Wise RA
    (1996) Rewarding actions of phencyclidine and related drugs in nucleus accumbens shell and frontal cortex. J Neurosci 16:3112–3122.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Cory-Slechta DA,
    2. Pazmino R,
    3. Bare C
    (1997) The critical role of nucleus accumbens dopamine systems in the mediation of fixed interval schedule-controlled operant behaviour. Brain Res 764:253–256.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. David JD,
    2. Zahnister NR,
    3. Hoffer BJ,
    4. Gerhardt GA
    (1998) In vivo electrochemical studies of dopamine clearance in subregions of rat nucleus accumbens: differential properties of the core and shell. Exp Neurol 153:277–286.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Davis WM,
    2. Smith SG
    (1979) Role of conditioned reinforcers in the initiation, maintenance and extinction of drug-seeking behaviour. Pavlov J Biol Sci 11:222–236.
    OpenUrl
  13. ↵
    1. De Vries TJ,
    2. Schoffelmeer ANM,
    3. Binnekade R,
    4. Vanderschuren LJMJ
    (1999) Dopaminergic mechanisms mediating the incentive to seek cocaine and heroin following long-term withdrawal of IV drug self-administration. Psychopharmacology 143:254–260.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. de Wit H,
    2. Stewart J
    (1981) Reinstatement of cocaine-reinforced responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology 75:134–143.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Di Chiara G
    (1999) Drug addiction as dopamine-dependent associative learning disorder. Eur J Pharmacol 375:13–30.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Di Chiara G,
    2. Imperato A
    (1988) Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:5274–5278.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Di Chiara G,
    2. Tanda G,
    3. Frau R,
    4. Carboni E
    (1993) On the preferential release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens by amphetamine: further evidence obtained by vertically implanted concentric dialysis probes. Psychopharmacology Berl 112:398–402.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Di Ciano P,
    2. Blaha CD,
    3. Phillips AG
    (1998) Conditioned changes in dopamine oxidation currents in the nucleus accumbens of rats by stimuli paired with self-administration or yoked-administration of d-amphetamine. Eur J Neurosci 10:1121–1127.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Dickinson A,
    2. Smith J,
    3. Mirenowicz J
    (2000) Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental learning under dopamine antagonism. Behav Neurosci 114:468–483.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Ehrman RN,
    2. Robbins SJ,
    3. Childress AR,
    4. O'Brien CP
    (1992) Conditioned responses to cocaine-related stimuli in cocaine abuse patients. Psychopharmacology 107:523–529.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Ettenberg A,
    2. MacConnell LA,
    3. Geist TD
    (1996) Effects of haloperidol in a response-reinstatement model of heroin relapse. Psychopharmacology 124:205–210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Everitt BJ,
    2. Parkinson JA,
    3. Olmstead MC,
    4. Arroyo M,
    5. Robledo P,
    6. Robbins TW
    (1999) Associative processes in addiction and reward: the role of amygdala-ventral striatal systems. Ann NY Acad Sci 877:412–438.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Fontana DJ,
    2. Post RM,
    3. Pert A
    (1993) Conditioned increases in mesolimbic dopamine overflow by stimuli associated with cocaine. Brain Res 629:31–39.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Goldberg SR
    (1973) Comparable behavior maintained under fixed-ratio and second-order schedules of food presentation, cocaine injection or d-amphetamine injection in the squirrel monkey. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 186:18–30.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Goldberg SR,
    2. Tang AH
    (1977) Behavior maintained under second-order schedules of intravenous morphine injection in squirrel and rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology 51:235–242.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Gratton A,
    2. Wise RA
    (1994) Drug- and behavior-associated changes in dopamine-related electrochemical signals during intravenous cocaine self-administration. J Neurosci 14:4130–4146.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Hall J,
    2. Parkinson JA,
    3. Connor TMF,
    4. Di Ciano P,
    5. Dickinson A,
    6. Everitt BJ
    (1999) The role of amygdala-ventral striatal sub-systems in Pavlovian to instrumental transfer. Soc Neurosci Abstr 41:2.
    OpenUrl
  28. ↵
    1. Heidbreder C,
    2. Feldon J
    (1998) Amphetamine-induced neurochemical and locomotor responses are expressed differentially across the anteroposterior axis of the core and shell subterritories of the nucleus accumbens. Synapse 29:310–322.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Heimer L,
    2. Zahm DS,
    3. Churchill L,
    4. Kalivas PW,
    5. Wohltmann C
    (1991) Specificity in the projection patterns of accumbal core and shell in the rat. Neuroscience 41:89–125.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Hemby SE,
    2. Co C,
    3. Koves TR,
    4. Smith JE,
    5. Dworkin TR
    (1997) Differences in extracellular dopamine concentrations in the nucleus accumbens during response-dependent and response-independent cocaine administration in the rat. Psychopharmacology 133:7–16.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hitchcott PK,
    2. Phillips GD
    (1998) Double dissociation of the behavioural effects of R(+) 7-OH-DPAT infusions in the central and basolateral amygdala nuclei upon Pavlovian and instrumental conditioned appetitive behaviours. Psychopharmacology 140:458–469.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Hurd Y,
    2. Weiss F,
    3. Koob GF,
    4. Ungerstedt U
    (1989) Cocaine reinforcement and extracellular DA overflow in rat nucleus accumbens: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res 498:199–204.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Jones SR,
    2. O'Dell SJ,
    3. Marshall JF,
    4. Wightman RM
    (1996) Functional and anatomical evidence for different dopamine dynamics in the core and shell of the nucleus accumbens in slices of rat brain. Synapse 23:224–231.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Kelley AE,
    2. Smith-Roe SL,
    3. Holahan MR
    (1997) Response-reinforcement learning is dependent on N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor activation in the nucleus accumbens core. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:12174–12179.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Kiyatkin EA,
    2. Stein EA
    (1995) Fluctuations in nucleus accumbens dopamine during cocaine self-administration behaviour: an in vivo electrochemical study. Neuroscience 54:599–617.
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    1. Kiyatkin EA,
    2. Stein EA
    (1996) Conditioned changes in nucleus accumbens dopamine signal established by intravenous cocaine in rats. Neurosci Lett 211:73–76.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Ljungberg T,
    2. Apicella P,
    3. Schultz W
    (1992) Responses of monkey midbrain dopamine neurons during learning of behavioural reactions. J Neurophysiol 67:145–163.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Lovibond PF
    (1983) Facilitation of instrumental behaviour by a Pavlovian appetitive conditioned stimuli. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:225–247.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. McFarland K,
    2. Ettenberg A
    (1997) Reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior produced by heroin-predictive environmental stimuli. Psychopharmacology 131:86–92.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Neisewander JL,
    2. O'Dell LE,
    3. Tran-Nguyen LTL,
    4. Castaneda E,
    5. Fuchs RA
    (1996) Dopamine overflow in the nucleus accumbens during extinction and reinstatement of cocaine self-administration behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 15:506–514.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. O'Brien CP,
    2. McLellan AT
    (1996) Myths about the treatment of addiction. Lancet 347:237–240.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. O'Brien CP,
    2. Childress AR,
    3. McLellan T,
    4. Ehrman R
    (1990) Integrating systemic cue exposure with standard treatment in recovering drug dependent patients. Addict Behav 15:355–365.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. O'Brien CP,
    2. Childress AR,
    3. Ehrman R,
    4. Robbins SJ
    (1998) Conditioning factors in drug abuse: can they explain compulsion? J Psychopharmacol 12:15–22.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Parkinson JA,
    2. Olmstead MC,
    3. Burns LH,
    4. Robbins TW,
    5. Everitt BJ
    (1999) Dissociation in effects of lesions of nucleus accumbens core and shell in appetitive Pavlovian approach behavior and the potentiation of conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity by d-amphetamine. J Neurosci 19:2401–2411.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Parkinson JA,
    2. Willoughby PJ,
    3. Robbins TW,
    4. Everitt BJ
    (2000) Disconnection of the anterior cingulate cortex and nucleus accumbens core impairs Pavlovian approach behavior: further evidence for limbic cortical-ventral striatopallidal systems. Behav Neurosci 114:42–63.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Paxinos G,
    2. Watson C
    (1986) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates, Ed 2. (Academic, Sydney).
  47. ↵
    1. Phillips AG,
    2. Fibiger HC
    (1990) Role of reward and enhancement if conditioned reward in persistence of responding for cocaine. Behav Pharmacol 1:269–282.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Pierce RC,
    2. Kalivas PW
    (1995) Amphetamine produces sensitized increases in locomotion and extracellular dopamine preferentially in the nucleus accumbens shell of rats administered repeated cocaine. Brain Res 567:169–174.
    OpenUrl
  49. ↵
    1. Pilla M,
    2. Perachon S,
    3. Sautel F,
    4. Garrido F,
    5. Mann A,
    6. Wermuth CG,
    7. Schwartz JC,
    8. Everitt BJ,
    9. Sokoloff P
    (1999) Selective inhibition of cocaine-seeking behaviour by a partial dopamine D3 receptor agonist. Nature 400:371–375.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Pontieri FE,
    2. Tanda G,
    3. Di Chiara G
    (1995) Intravenous cocaine, morphine, and amphetamine preferentially increase extracellular dopamine in the “shell” as compared with the “core” of the rat nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:12304–12308.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Redgrave P,
    2. Prescott TJ,
    3. Gurney K
    (1999) Is the short-latency dopamine response too short to signal reward error? Trends Neurosci 22:146–151.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Robbins TW,
    2. Everitt BJ
    (1999) Drug addiction: bad habits add up. Nature 398:567–570.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Robbins TW,
    2. Roberts DCS,
    3. Koob GF
    (1983) Effects of d-amphetamine and apomorphine upon operant behavior and schedule-induced licking in rats with 6-hydroxydopamine-induced lesions of the nucleus accumbens. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 224:662–673.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Roberts DCS,
    2. Koob GF,
    3. Klonoff HC,
    4. Fibiger HC
    (1980) Extinction and recovery of cocaine self-administration following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 12:781–787.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Robinson TE,
    2. Berridge KC
    (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18:247–291.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Robledo P,
    2. Maldonado-Lopez R,
    3. Koob GF
    (1992) Role of dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens in the rewarding properties of cocaine. Ann NY Acad Sci 654:509–512.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Romo R,
    2. Schultz W
    (1990) Dopamine neurons of the monkey midbrain and contingencies of responses to active touch during self-initiated arm movements. J Neurophysiol 63:592–606.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Salamone JD,
    2. Aberman JE,
    3. Sokolowski JD,
    4. Cousins MS
    (1999) Nucleus accumbens dopamine and rate of responding: neurochemical and behavioral studies. Psychobiology 27:236–247.
    OpenUrl
  59. ↵
    1. Schultz W
    (1998) Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol 80:1–27.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Schultz W,
    2. Romo R,
    3. Ljunberg T,
    4. Mirenovicz J,
    5. Hollerman JR,
    6. Dickinson A
    (1994) Reward and related signals carried by DA neurons. in Models of information processing in the basal ganglia, ed Houk, et al. (MIT, Cambridge, MA).
  61. ↵
    1. Self DW,
    2. Barnhart WJ,
    3. Lehman DA,
    4. Nestler EJ
    (1996) Opposite modulation of cocaine-seeking behavior by D1-like and D2-like dopamine-receptor agonists. Science 271:1586–1589.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  62. ↵
    1. Shaham Y,
    2. Stewart J
    (1996) Effects of opioid and dopamine receptor antagonists on relapse induced by stress and re-exposure to heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology 125:385–391.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Sokolowski JD,
    2. Salamone JD
    (1998) The role of accumbens dopamine in lever pressing and response allocation: effects of 6-OHDA injected into core and dorsomedial shell. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 59:557–566.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Sokolowski JD,
    2. Conlan AN,
    3. Salamone JD
    (1998) A microdialysis study of nucleus accumbens core and shell dopamine during operant responding in the rat. Neuroscience 86:1001–1009.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Stewart J,
    2. de Wit H,
    3. Eikelboom R
    (1984) Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug affects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychol Rev 91:251–268.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Tanda GL,
    2. Di Chiara G
    (1998) A dopamine-mu(1) opioid link in the rat ventral tegmentum shared by palatable food (Fonzies) and non-psychostimulant drugs of abuse. Eur J Neurosci 10:1179–1187.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Tanda G,
    2. Pontieri FE,
    3. Di Chiara G
    (1997) Cannabinoid and heroin activation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by a common mu 1 opioid receptor mechanism. Science 276:2048–2050.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. Taylor JR,
    2. Robbins TW
    (1984) Enhanced behavioural control by conditioned reinforcers following microinjections of d-amphetamine into the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology 84:405–412.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Taylor JR,
    2. Robbins TW
    (1986) 6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens, but not the caudate-nucleus, attenuate enhanced responding with reward-related stimuli produced by intra-accumbens d-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology 90:390–397.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Tiffany TT,
    2. Carter BL
    (1998) Is craving the source of compulsive drug use? J Psychopharmacol 12:23–30.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Weiss F,
    2. MaldonadoVlaar CS,
    3. Parsons LH,
    4. Kerr TM,
    5. Smith DL,
    6. BenShahar O
    (2000) Control of cocaine-seeking behavior by drug-associated stimuli in rats: effects on recovery of extinguished operant-responding and extracellular dopamine levels in amygdala and nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:4321–4326.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Whitelaw RB,
    2. Markou A,
    3. Robbins TW,
    4. Everitt BJ
    (1996) Excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala impair the acquisition of cocaine-seeking behaviour under a second-order schedule of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology 127:213–224.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Wilson JM,
    2. Nobrega JN,
    3. Corrigal WA,
    4. Coen KM,
    5. Shannak K,
    6. Kish SJ
    (1994) Amygdala dopamine levels are markedly elevated after self-administration but not passive-administration of cocaine. Brain Res 668:39–45.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Wise RA,
    2. Murray A,
    3. Bozarth MA
    (1990) Bromocriptine self-administration and bromocriptine-reinstatement of cocaine-trained and heroin-trained lever pressing in rats. Psychopharmacology 100:355–360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Wolterink G,
    2. Phillips GD,
    3. Cador M,
    4. Donselaar-Wolterink I,
    5. Robbins TW,
    6. Everitt BJ
    (1993) Relative roles of ventral striatal D1 and D2 dopamine receptors in responding with conditioned reinforcement. Psychopharmacology 110:355–364.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    Wyvell CL, Berridge KC (2000) Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the pure incentive salience of a Pavlovian cue for food reward: enhancement of “wanting” without either “liking” or reinforcement. J Neurosci, in press.
  77. ↵
    1. Zahm DS,
    2. Heimer L
    (1990) Two transpallidal pathways originating in the rat nucleus accumbens. J Comp Neurol 302:437–446.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 20 (19)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 20, Issue 19
1 Oct 2000
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dissociation in Conditioned Dopamine Release in the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell in Response to Cocaine Cues and during Cocaine-Seeking Behavior in Rats
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dissociation in Conditioned Dopamine Release in the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell in Response to Cocaine Cues and during Cocaine-Seeking Behavior in Rats
Rutsuko Ito, Jeffrey W. Dalley, Simon R. Howes, Trevor W. Robbins, Barry J. Everitt
Journal of Neuroscience 1 October 2000, 20 (19) 7489-7495; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07489.2000

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dissociation in Conditioned Dopamine Release in the Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell in Response to Cocaine Cues and during Cocaine-Seeking Behavior in Rats
Rutsuko Ito, Jeffrey W. Dalley, Simon R. Howes, Trevor W. Robbins, Barry J. Everitt
Journal of Neuroscience 1 October 2000, 20 (19) 7489-7495; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07489.2000
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • cocaine
  • dopamine
  • nucleus accumbens
  • second-order schedule
  • conditioned reinforcement
  • microdialysis

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

ARTICLE

  • Emotion Processing in Chimeric Faces: Hemispheric Asymmetries in Expression and Recognition of Emotions
  • Gating of Hippocampal-Evoked Activity in Prefrontal Cortical Neurons by Inputs from the Mediodorsal Thalamus and Ventral Tegmental Area
  • Cytoskeletal and Morphological Alterations Underlying Axonal Sprouting after Localized Transection of Cortical Neuron AxonsIn Vitro
Show more ARTICLE

Behavioral/Systems

  • Genetic Dissociation of Opiate Tolerance and Physical Dependence in δ-Opioid Receptor-1 and Preproenkephalin Knock-Out Mice
  • Functional Heterogeneity in Human Olfactory Cortex: An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study
  • Induction of Hippocampal Long-Term Potentiation during Waking Leads to Increased Extrahippocampal zif-268 Expression during Ensuing Rapid-Eye-Movement Sleep
Show more Behavioral/Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.