Abstract
Voltage-gated K+ channels of the Kv3 subfamily have unusual electrophysiological properties, including activation at very depolarized voltages (positive to −10 mV) and very fast deactivation rates, suggesting special roles in neuronal excitability. In the brain, Kv3 channels are prominently expressed in select neuronal populations, which include fast-spiking (FS) GABAergic interneurons of the neocortex, hippocampus, and caudate, as well as other high-frequency firing neurons. Although evidence points to a key role in high-frequency firing, a definitive understanding of the function of these channels has been hampered by a lack of selective pharmacological tools. We therefore generated mouse lines in which one of the Kv3 genes, Kv3.2, was disrupted by gene-targeting methods. Whole-cell electrophysiological recording showed that the ability to fire spikes at high frequencies was impaired in immunocytochemically identified FS interneurons of deep cortical layers (5-6) in which Kv3.2 proteins are normally prominent. No such impairment was found for FS neurons of superficial layers (2-4) in which Kv3.2 proteins are normally only weakly expressed. These data directly support the hypothesis that Kv3 channels are necessary for high-frequency firing. Moreover, we found that Kv3.2 −/− mice showed specific alterations in their cortical EEG patterns and an increased susceptibility to epileptic seizures consistent with an impairment of cortical inhibitory mechanisms. This implies that, rather than producing hyperexcitability of the inhibitory interneurons, Kv3.2 channel elimination suppresses their activity. These data suggest that normal cortical operations depend on the ability of inhibitory interneurons to generate high-frequency firing.
- K+ channels
- neocortex
- fast spiking
- knock-out inhibitory interneurons
- high-frequency firing
- seizure susceptibility
- GABA
- epilepsy
Approximately 10–20% of the neurons in the cerebral cortex are inhibitory GABAergic interneurons. These cells play a critical role in a number of important functions, including the gating and processing of sensory information, the establishment and plasticity of sensory receptive fields, the synchronization of cortical circuits, the generation of rhythms, and the limiting of seizure activity (Fairen et al., 1984; Gilbert, 1993;Jones, 1993; Amitai and Connors, 1995; Keller, 1995; Singer and Gray, 1995; Freund and Buzsaki, 1996; Jefferys et al., 1996; Steriade, 1997).
Cortical GABAergic interneurons represent a heterogenous population of cells with subtypes differing in morphological appearance, expression of specific markers such as calcium-binding proteins or neuropeptides, firing patterns, synaptic properties, and axonal connectivity (Jones, 1975; Somogyi et al., 1984; Hendry et al., 1989; Freund and Buzsaki, 1996; Cauli et al., 1997; Gonchar and Burkhalter, 1997; Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Gupta et al., 2000).
The largest group of neocortical inhibitory interneurons (∼50%) consists of cells that contain the calcium-binding protein parvalbumin (PV). These neurons are characterized by a “fast-spiking” firing pattern, i.e., the ability to fire long trains of very brief action potentials at high frequency with little firing frequency adaptation (McCormick et al., 1985; Celio, 1986; Cauli et al., 1997; Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997). These neurons are interconnected by electrical synapses and form a network of fast-spiking cells, suggesting a role in the generation of synchronized cortical activity (Galarreta and Hestrin, 1999; Gibson et al., 1999).
Several lines of evidence have led to the hypothesis that specific voltage-gated, delayed rectifier-type K+channels composed of K+ channel pore-forming subunits of the Kv3 subfamily (Kv3.1–Kv3.3) are critical for the ability of neurons to fire at high frequencies in a sustained or repetitive fashion. First, the properties of these channels, which include activation at voltages positive to −10 mV and very fast deactivation rates on membrane repolarization, naturally lend themselves to a specific role in spike repolarization. Second, there is a strong correlation between the specific expression of Kv3 RNA transcripts and Kv3 proteins in neuronal populations that fire at high frequencies. Third, pharmacological experiments show that blockade of native Kv3-like currents with low concentrations of tetraethylammonium (TEA) or 4-aminopyridine (4-AP) impairs the ability of these neurons to fire sustained and/or repetitive-action potentials at high frequency. Fourth, computer modeling indicates that selective blockade of Kv3 currents impairs high-frequency firing (Perney et al., 1992; Lenz et al., 1994; Weiser et al., 1994, 1995; Du et al., 1996; Massengill et al., 1997; Sekirnjak et al., 1997; Martina et al., 1998; Wang et al., 1998; Chow et al., 1999; Erisir et al., 1999; Atzori et al., 2000) (for review, see Coetzee et al., 1999; Rudy et al., 1999).
To further test the hypothesis, and given the absence of selective channel blockers, we used gene-targeting methods to produce mice lines that do not express Kv3.2 K+ channel subunits (McCormack et al., 1990; Rudy et al., 1992), which are prominently expressed in PV-containing interneurons in deep cortical layers (Chow et al., 1999), and compared the properties of fast-spiking neurons in the neocortex from these mice with those from normal wild-type littermates. Results from these experiments provide direct evidence that Kv3 channels are critical for both sustained and repetitive high-frequency firing. Moreover, the Kv3.2 −/− mice show both an enhanced susceptibility to seizures and disturbed cortical rhythmic activity. The availability of mice in which fast-spiking is compromised in specific neuronal populations provides a model to investigate the consequences of this impairment on the behavior of cortical circuits, which in turn can help in the understanding of the function of fast-spiking, the roles of the interneurons in cortical function, and the mechanisms by which they achieve these functions.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Generation of mice lacking Kv3.2 proteins
Isolation of a mouse 129 genomic clone containing exon I of Kv3.2. A mouse 129 genomic library (∼1 × 106 pfu) in λDashII (kind gift from Drs. J. Rossant and A. G. Reaume, Mount Sinai Hospital, Toronto, Canada) was screened at high stringency with a 380 bp fragment containing the first 301 bp of the coding region of Kv3.2 and 79 bp of the 5′ untranslated region, derived from a rat Kv3.2 cDNA (McCormack et al., 1990).
Bacteriophage DNA from positive clones was isolated with the Midi λ Phage DNA Prep (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany) from fresh liquid lysates. Genomic clone inserts were excised from the bacteriophage arms by restriction digest with NotI and subcloned into theNotI site of the bacterial vector pBluescript (Stratagene, La Jolla, CA). One of the isolated clones, E2, shown by hybridization to contain sequence from the first coding exon (exon I) of Kv3.2, was used for these studies. The restriction recognition sites of the following enzymes were mapped on the E2 clone: BamHI,ClaI, EcoRI, HindIII, SacI, and XbaI. Each of the EcoRI fragments was subcloned individually into pBluescript (Stratagene) to facilitate mapping and the generation of the targeting construct. The 3′ half of the clone E2, consisting of two contiguous EcoRI fragments of 3.6 and 8.5 kb, was used for the construction of the targeting construct and is illustrated in Figure1A.
Generation of the targeting construct. Regions within the 3.6 and 8.5 kb EcoRI fragments of the clone E2 were selected to be the short and long arms of homology (Fig. 1A). The 1.9 kb short arm of homology was isolated from the 3.6 kbEcoRI fragment by digestion with SacI andEcoRI. The 5.0 kb long arm of homology was isolated from the 8.5 kb EcoRI fragment by restriction digestion withXbaI and NotI (in the polylinker of pBluescript). The neomycin resistance gene flanked by EcoRI andXbaI sticky ends was ligated between the two arms. The thymidine kinase gene was placed 5′ of the short arm of homology, and the entire construct was cloned into pBluescript. The final construct was mapped by restriction digest and subsequent Southern hybridization, as well as by sequencing of key junctions to confirm its integrity.
Homologous recombination in embryonic stem cells. W9.5 embryonic stem (ES) cells (28 × 106) were harvested (Robertson, 1987) and resuspended in 1 ml of culture medium in a sterile electroporation cuvette (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA). We mixed 40 μg of the NotI linearized targeting construct (in sterile PBS) with the suspended cells and electroporated it with a Gene-Pulser electroporator (Bio-Rad) at 0.23 kV, 500 μF. The pulsed ES cells were cultured onto 60 mm feeder plates at 37°C in an atmosphere of 5% CO2. The basic culture medium consisted of DMEM plus 15% serum. Leukemia inhibitory factor (106 U/ml), used to retard ES cell differentiation, was added to all culture media except replica plates (see below). After a day in culture, G418 (350 μg/ml Geneticin; Life Technologies, Gaithersburg, MD) and 1-(2-deoxy-2-fluoro-1-β-d-arabino-furanosyl)-5-iodouracil (2 μg/ml; a gift from Eli Lilly, Indianapolis, IN) were added to the culture medium. The medium was changed 2 d after drug introduction and then daily afterward. Five days after drug introduction, surviving undifferentiated ES cell colonies were transferred individually to multiwell cell culture plates (Falcon).
A total of 380 ES cell colonies were harvested. The medium was changed 2 d after harvesting and then daily. Four days later, the ES cell cultures were trypsinized and passaged into two sterile 48-well multiwell cell culture plates (one colony per well), a master plate that was frozen and stored at −70°C, and a replica plate that was further expanded. The replica plates were fed every 2 d. After 1 week in culture, the cell culture medium was discarded, and the replica plates were washed with PBS. To each well, 250 μl of lysis buffer (1.0 m NaCl, 10 mm EDTA, 50 mmTris, pH 8, 0.5% SDS, and 0.2 μg/μl proteinase K) was added, and the plate was incubated overnight at 55°C. We added 250 μl of isopropanol to each well and the genomic DNA pellets were transferred individually to microcentrifuge tubes. The DNA was washed with 70% ethanol and resuspended in 50 μl of Tris-EDTA (TE). The DNA was used to genotype the colonies by Southern blot analysis (as described below) to identify ES cells that had undergone homologous recombination.
Chimera generation. From the master plate, identified, targeted ES cell colonies were expanded in culture. C57BL6 blastocysts that were 2.5 d old were harvested from the uterine tubes of timed pregnant females, and 8–10 targeted ES cells were introduced to the blastocoel with a beveled glass micropipette. The injected blastocysts were implanted into pseudopregnant mothers (Joyner, 2000). Chimeric character was estimated by coat color, and males with >95% chimerism were selected and bred with C57BL6 females. Offspring heterozygotes were identified by Southern blot genotyping using genomic DNA obtained from tail biopsies (see below) and were bred against C57BL6 mice for backcrossing or bred against other heterozygotes to generate knock-out mice. All knock-out mice used in our experiments had been backcrossed at least seven generations onto the C57BL6 genetic background.
Genotyping by genomic Southern blot analysis
Genomic DNA from cultured ES cells or tail biopsies was digested overnight with XbaI (Promega, Madison, WI). The digested samples were electrophoresed on 0.7% agarose–Tris-borate–EDTA gels at 7.5–9.0 V/cm until DNA fragment sizes from 2 to 6 kb were clearly separated. The gels were stained with ethidium bromide and photographed on a UV light table with a fluorescent ruler for orientation. The agarose gels were incubated in 5 gel volumes of denaturing solution (1.0 m NaOH and 1.5m NaCl) with gentle agitation for 30 min and a change to a fresh solution after 15 min. After denaturing, the gels were incubated in 5 gel volumes of neutralizing solution (1.0m Tris-HCl, pH 7.5, and 1.5m NaCl) with gentle agitation for 30 min and a change to a fresh solution after 15 min. The DNA in the gels was transferred onto nylon membranes (Stratagene) overnight via capillary action. The blotted membranes were marked, and the DNA was UV-crosslinked to the nylon membrane in a Stratalinker (Stratagene). The membranes were stored dry at room temperature until hybridization.
The probe used for genotyping was a 1.0 kb band between theXbaI and SacI sites in the 3.6 kbEcoRI fragment of the E2 clone (Fig. 1A,bottom diagram). The probe corresponds to sequences in the intron preceding exon I of the Kv3.2 gene. Probes were labeled with [32P]dCTP with the Redi-Prime random primer labeling kit (Amersham Pharmacia Biotech, Arlington Heights, IL). With XbaI-digested genomic DNA, the probe hybridized to a 3.0 kb band derived from the wild-type allele and a 5.0 kb band from the targeted null allele (Fig.1B).
The Southern blots were prehybridized in QuikHyb (Stratagene) for 15 min at 68°C. The denatured probe was added at a final concentration of 1.5 × 106 TCA precipitable counts per milliliter, and the blots were hybridized in a Hybaid oven (Labline) at 68°C for 1 hr with gentle rotation. The hybridized Southern membranes were washed (three times) at room temperature with 2× saline-sodium phosphate-EDTA buffer (SSPE) and 0.1% SDS, followed by a 60°C hot wash (in 0.1× SSPE and 0.1% SDS) for 30 min. The blots were then exposed to x-ray film between two intensifying screens at −70°C for 2 hr to 1 week, depending on the intensity of the signal.
Isolation of genomic DNA from tail biopsies
We harvested 0.5–1.0 cm of tail from ∼3-week-old mice. Tails were digested overnight at 60°C in tail lysis buffer (100 mm NaCl, 50 mm Tris, pH 7.4, 1 mmEDTA, 0.1% SDS, and 0.75 mg/ml proteinase K). Tail lysates were extracted with 1 vol of chloroform, and genomic DNA was precipitated with 2 vol of ethanol. DNA pellets were washed with 70% ethanol and briefly air-dried to remove residual ethanol. The genomic DNA was resuspended in TE and used for restriction enzyme digestion.
Northern blot analysis
Total RNA was obtained with the guanidine–isothiocynate method and quantified by optical density measurements (Chomczynski and Sacchi, 1987). Total RNA (10 μg) from knock-out and wild-type mice was electrophoresed in denaturing formaldehyde gels and transferred to Duralon–UV membranes (Stratagene) as previously described (Rudy et al., 1988). The Northern blots were hybridized as described for Southern genotyping. The probe for Kv3.2 was the 380 bp probe described previously. A full-length cDNA clone of Kv3.1b was used as a probe template for Kv3.1 mRNA detection.
Western blot analysis
Brain membrane extracts were prepared from a P3 fraction of tissue homogenate from adult knock-out and wild-type mice (Hartshorne and Catterall, 1984) and solubilized in Triton X-100 as previously described (Chow et al., 1999). To prepare the Western blots, membrane protein (25 μg/lane for detection with Kv3.1b-Ab and 50 μg/lane for Kv3.2-Ab) were electrophoresed in a 9% SDS polyacrylamide gel and then transferred onto nitrocellulose membranes (Bio-Rad) as previously described (Chow et al., 1999). The blots were incubated with either Kv3.1b-Ab (Weiser et al., 1995) at 1:1000–1:2000 dilution or Kv3.2-Ab (Chow et al., 1999) at 1:50–1:100 dilution. This was followed by incubation with horseradish peroxidase-linked anti-rabbit secondary antibodies (Promega). Detection of the secondary antibody was performed using chemiluminescence (Pierce, Rockford, IL). The Kv3.2-rAb was derived from immunizing rabbits to a peptide corresponding to a sequence present in the region of the Kv3.2 protein that is before the first membrane-spanning domain in the N-terminal area and recognizes all Kv3.2 isoforms (Chow et al., 1999). The Kv3.1-rAb is directed against the C terminal of the predominant isoform of theKv3.1 gene, Kv3.1b (Weiser et al., 1995).
Immunohistochemistry of mouse brain
Adult mice were anesthetized with intraperitoneal injections of sodium pentobarbital (∼80–100 mg/kg) and transcardially perfused with paraformaldehyde after the loss of pain reflexes as previously described (Chow et al., 1999). The brains were removed from the animals and processed for immunohistochemistry as described previously (Chow et al., 1999). The Vectastain Elite ABC kit was used to immunolabel via the horseradish peroxidase method. Kv3.1-rAb was used at 1:1000 dilution, Kv3.2-rAb was used at 1:300 dilution, and mouse monoclonal antibodies to parvalbumin (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) were used at 1:300.
In vivo physiology
Behavioral analysis. The following behavioral tests were all done in the laboratory of Richard Paylor (Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine) with a battery commonly used in this laboratory (Kimber et al., 1999; Peier et al., 2000). The tests were done blindly in a group of mice that included 13 mutant (four female, nine male) and nine wild-type (three female, six male) littermates. The mice had been backcrossed seven times to C57BL6. The tests were performed essentially as described byPaylor et al. (1998) and included the following: (1) general neurological screen for severe sensory and motor abnormalities, (2) open-field test for exploratory activity and anxiety-related responses, (3) light–dark test for anxiety-related responses, (4) rotarod test for motor coordination and skill learning, (5) acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response for sensorimotor gating, (6) habituation of the acoustic startle response for sensorimotor adaptation, (7) contextual and auditory-cued freezing to assess conditioned fear, and (8) the hotplate test for analgesia-related responses. Data were analyzed using two- or three-way ANOVA.
Chronic EEG. Adult mice were anesthetized with Avertin (1.25% tribromoethanol–amyl alcohol) by intraperitoneal injection (0.02 ml/gm). Silver wire electrodes (0.005 inches in diameter) soldered to a microminiature connector were implanted into the subdural space over the left and right cortical hemispheres. After several days of recovery, EEG activity was recorded daily during random 2 hr samples for 7–10 d using a TECA digital electroencephalograph. All recordings were performed on mice moving freely in the test cage in the laboratory of Jeffrey L. Noebels at Baylor College of Medicine.
EEG recording in anesthetized mice. Knock-out and wild-type mice were anesthetized with intraperitoneal injections of a mixture of ketamine (15 mg/kg) and xylazine (3 mg/kg). Depth of anesthesia was ascertained by recording EEG with monopolar electrodes placed in frontal cortex. Supplemental doses of ketamine–xylazine were given at the slightest sign of EEG desynchronization. After the loss of tail pinch reflexes, the mice were placed in a rodent stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA) equipped with mouse head holders. A midline sagittal incision was made along the scalp, and the skin was reflected. Petroleum jelly was applied over the eyes to prevent ulcers. Burr holes were drilled over the right somatosensory cortex and the right dorsal thalamus according to the stereotaxic coordinates (Franklin and Paxinos, 1997). Mineral oil was applied over the exposed brain to prevent desiccation. Bipolar tungsten electrodes were fashioned from two monopolar tungsten electrodes with resistance of 1 MΩ that were affixed with dental cement. The bipolar electrode pairs were lowered into the neocortex and thalamus with fine micromanipulators (Narishige, Tokyo, Japan) through the burr holes, and signals were amplified with a homemade DC amplifier with head stage and capacity compensation. In cortex, the electrodes were located in the pial surface and in layer 6 (∼0.7 mm apart); in the thalamus, the electrodes were side by side, separated by 0.4 mm. Electrical stimulation was also delivered through the recording electrodes with an isolated pulse stimulator (AM Systems). Data were sampled at 1 kHz with an InstruNet (GW Systems) analog-to-digital card and analyzed in Igor (WaveMetrics Inc., Lake Oswego, OR) with customized routines.
Seizure induction with pentylenetetrazole.Pentylenetetrazole (PTZ) (Research Biochemicals, Natick, MA) was dissolved in PBS and injected intraperitoneally at the indicated dose. After injection, the animal was placed in a transparent Plexiglas cage (30 × 20 × 25 cm) and observed for up to 30 min. Latencies to focal (partial clonic), generalized (generalized clonic), and maximal (tonic-clonic) behavioral seizures were recorded. The bottom of the cage was covered with clean paper towels that were replaced for each animal. Each cage was cleaned with water after each experiment, before introducing a new mouse. All the animals used in this study were housed in a facility with light (12 hr light/dark cycle) and temperature control, and all the experiments were performed in the laboratory between 12:00 P.M. and 2 P.M. in mice of similar age (10–14 weeks).
We defined several stages in the behavioral response to PTZ injection. Stage 1, designated as hypoactivity, was characterized by a progressive decrease in activity until the mice stood in a crouched or prone position with their abdomens in full contact with the bottom of the cage. Stage 2 was isolated jerks or twitches. Stage 3 was partial or focal clonic seizures affecting the face, head, and/or forelimbs. These seizures were usually very brief, typically 1–2 sec. Stage 4 was generalized clonic seizures. These usually occurred suddenly, could last 30 sec or more, and involved generalized whole-body clonus. Autonomic signs were frequently seen. The seizure was usually followed by a quiescent period. Stage 5 was tonic-clonic (maximal) seizures. Mice reaching this stage displayed wild running and jumping behavior and then had generalized seizures characterized by tonic hindlimb extension. Tonic-clonic maximal seizures were usually associated with death.
In vitro physiology
Slice preparation. Knock-out and wild-type mice of ages postnatal day 15 (P15) to P21 were used for acute brain-slice preparation (Agmon and Connors, 1991). Mice were anesthetized with an overdose injection of sodium pentobarbital and decapitated after the loss of pain reflexes. The brain was rapidly removed from the skull in a bath of ice-cold artificial CSF (ACSF) (in mm): 124 NaCl, 5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2, and 10 dextrose, pH 7.4, bubble-saturated with 95% O2, 5% CO2. Slices 250-μm-thick from the somatosensory cortex were prepared on a vibratome (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL) and placed in a holding chamber with continuous bubbling ACSF at room temperature. Slices were allowed to rest in the holding chamber for at least 1 hr before transfer to the recording chamber. The submersion-type recording chamber was perfused at a rate of 2–3 ml/min with ACSF saturated with 95% O2, 5% CO2, and all recordings were done at a temperature of 24°C controlled by an in-line solution heater (Warner Instruments, Hamden, CT).
Whole-cell recordings. Neocortical neurons were visualized with near-infrared light (>775 nm) at 400× magnification with a nuvicon tube camera (Dage-MTI, Michigan City, IN) and differential interference optic (DIC) on a fixed-stage microscope (Olympus Optical, Tokyo, Japan) (Stuart et al., 1993; Erisir et al., 1999). Nonpyramidal cells were visually selected for current-clamp experiments. Recording microelectrodes of 6–9 MΩ resistance were made from standard wall borosilicate glass (Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA) with a Flaming/Brown type micropipette puller (Sutter Instruments). The micropipette filling solution consisted of (in mm): 144 K-gluconate, 0.2 EGTA, 3 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, 4 ATP-Mg, and 0.5 GTP-Tris. For subsequent histochemistry, biocytin (0.2% w/v; Sigma) was added to the internal solution just before recording. In the current-clamp mode of an electronic bridge amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA), repetitive firing, single spikes, and hyperpolarization responses were recorded in the whole-cell configuration. Protocols were delivered under the control of pClamp 7 software (Axon Instruments). Responses were sampled at 10 kHz. Neurons were held at −70 mV with small injections of direct current, except during protocols for spike doublets and rebound responses when they were held at −60 mV. Action potential shape parameters were measured from action potentials evoked by 150 msec current steps that were just above threshold. Spike amplitude was measured as the difference between the peak and the threshold of the action potential. Spike threshold was determined by finding the potential at which the second derivative of the voltage waveform exceeded three times its SD in the period preceding spike onset. The fast afterhyperpolarization (AHP) was measured as the difference between the spike threshold and voltage minimum after the action potential peak. Maximum rates of rise and decay of the action potential were computed from the maximum and minimum of the first derivative of the voltage waveform. Spike width was measured at half the spike amplitude. Spike times were measured by determining the time at which the rising phase of the action potential crossed a fixed-threshold potential. Instantaneous frequency (one per interspike interval) was computed from trains of action potentials evoked by 600 msec duration pulses. Steady-state firing rate was the average of instantaneous frequency for the last five intervals of a train. Current strength was increased until spike failure occurred within the 600 msec duration pulse. The maximum steady-state firing rate was the steady-state firing rate from the train evoked by the current strengths (at increments of 100 pA) before that which produced spike failure. Firing frequency adaptation was calculated by dividing the steady-state firing rate by the first instantaneous frequency of the train. All analysis was performed in customized routines in Igor and Sigma Plot. Results are reported as mean ± SEM. TEA (Research Biochemicals) was bath-applied. Only one fast-spiking neuron was recorded per brain slice.
Histochemistry and immunolabeling of recorded neurons
After electrophysiological characterization, brain slices were fixed for 1–2 hr at room temperature in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. The slices were transferred into 30% sucrose with 0.02% sodium azide and stored at 4°C. Slices were washed three times in PBS to remove sucrose and incubated in a blocking–permeablization solution [1% (w/v) BSA, 0.4% (v/v) Triton X-100, and 10% (v/v) normal goat serum] for 1 hr. For primary labeling, streptavidin conjugated to Cy2 (1:250 dilution; Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA) and mouse monoclonal parvalbumin IgG (1:400) (Sigma) were incubated with the brain slices in 10% blocking solution in PBS for 7 d at 4°C. The slices were washed twice in PBS and incubated with the secondary antibody, Cy3-conjugated anti-mouse IgG, for 5 d at 4°C. After three washes in PBS, the slices were mounted onto glass slides in 0.001m phosphate buffer and allowed to air dry. The sections were coverslipped with a polyvinyl alcohol–glycerol medium with 2% 1,4-diazabicyclo[2,2,2]octane (Goslin and Banker, 1991). The sections were examined and scored on a Zeiss (Oberkochen, Germany) Axiophot epifluorescence microscope. Sections containing biocytin-labeled neurons were examined for PV immunoreactivity without knowledge of the physiological characteristics of the recorded neuron. Only sections with distinct PV immunoreactivity present at the depth of the biocytin-labeled somata, as determined using a 40× objective, were considered for scoring. This precaution was taken to reduce the possibility of falsely scoring double-labeled cells as PV-negative because of incomplete antibody or chromophore penetration. Digital images were acquired on an Zeiss Axiovert 35 M confocal microscope with a 40× objective lens, a scanning laser attachment, and a krypton–argon mixed-gas laser, and transferred into a graphics program (Photoshop 5.0).
RESULTS
Generation of mice lacking Kv3.2 proteins
Gene targeting by homologous recombination in ES cells (Thomas and Capecchi, 1987) was used to generate mouse lines in which theKv3.2 gene has been disrupted. The targeting construct used to modify the mouse Kv3.2 gene was derived from a mouse 129 genomic clone and is shown diagrammatically in Figure1A, which also illustrates the structure of the gene in the mutated area before and after the targeting. The 3′ end portion of the first coding exon (exon I) of Kv3.2 was deleted and replaced by a neomycin gene. The portion of exon I that was deleted encodes the subunit (or tetramerization) domain (T domain) that is critical for the oligomerization of Kv channel subunits (Li et al., 1992; Shen and Pfaffinger, 1995; Xu et al., 1995). Therefore, if a truncated protein were to be made at the normal starting methionine of Kv3.2, it would lack the T domain and would not oligomerize with products of other Kv3 genes and produce dominant negative effects (McCormack et al., 1991; Babila et al., 1994; Ribera et al., 1996). ES cells with a targeted allele were selected and implanted in foster mothers. Several chimeras were obtained, from which two independent lines of Kv3.2 −/− mice were established. Both have been backcrossed (7 and 10 times so far) to C57BL6 mice and are being maintained in this genetic background. Kv3.2 −/− mice lack Kv3.2 mRNA and protein products (Figs. 1C,D,2), whereas heterozygous mice have reduced mRNA and protein levels (Fig. 1C,D). In contrast, the levels of products of the closely relatedKv3.1 gene were not affected (Fig.1C,D). We also determined whether the distribution of Kv3.1 protein was altered in the Kv3.2 −/− mice by immunohistochemistry (Fig. 2). Kv3.1 proteins have a wider expression pattern than Kv3.2 proteins (Weiser et al., 1995; Rudy et al., 1999) and overlap in several neuronal populations, including PV-containing neurons in the neocortex, globus pallidus, and hippocampus, in which Kv3.1 and Kv3.2 proteins may form heteromeric Kv3 channels (Chow et al., 1999; Hernandez-Pineda et al., 1999). In the Kv3.2 −/− mice, Kv3.1 proteins were detected in both these regions of overlap and the other structures in which they are normally found (Fig. 2). The overall brain histology (Fig. 2; see also Fig. 4) and the barrel structure (data not shown) of somatosensory cortex also appeared normal in these mice.
Phenotypic characterization of Kv3.2 −/− mice: increased susceptibility to epileptic seizures
Kv3.2 −/− mice in the mixed 129-C57BL6 or in the C57BL6 background have a healthy appearance and grow normally. Both male and female, homozygote (−/−) and heterozygote (+/−) mice are fertile. All the behavioral and functional analysis of the mice has been done in the nearly pure C57BL6 background. The mice show no evidence of severe sensory or motor abnormalities during neurological screens. Moreover, there were no significant differences (p > 0.05) detected in overall total distance traveled in the open-field test, light–dark test, rotarod test, prepulse inhibition, startle habituation, conditioned fear, spatial learning, or hotplate test (see Materials and Methods). There was one significant difference in the open-field test. Knock-out mice had a significantly lower (p < 0.04) center-to-total distance ratio, which is an indicator of anxiety in the open field. However, there were no statistically significant differences in the light–dark exploration box (an independent test of anxiety), so one must be cautious about making too much of the anxiety phenotype in the open field. Future experiments will be needed to determine whether there is a possible anxiety phenotype by evaluating the mice in other assays of anxiety such as the elevated plus-maze.
Spontaneous, epileptic episodes lasting 5–40 sec and characterized by tonic-clonic convulsions have been observed in behaving Kv3.2 −/− mice (n = 14 out of several hundred Kv3.2-deficient mice under similar manipulations) but never in wild-type littermates. These always occurred while the animals were being manipulated but could not be reliably provoked by routine handling or auditory or photic stimuli. The electrographic record during one of these spontaneous episodes is shown in Figure3. Spontaneous epileptic episodes in the Kv3.2 −/− mice are rare, and usually there are none during typical studies with the mice. The waking background EEG activity in these mutants is unremarkable, and no abnormal patterns of spike-wave discharge were observed in particular. However, the seizures suggest cortical excitability increases in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse, a hypothesis that was supported by a series of experiments described later in this paper.
Impaired fast spiking in cortical interneurons from Kv3.2 −/− mice
We used the Kv3.2 −/− mice to directly test the hypothesis that K+ channels containing Kv3 proteins are required for sustained high-frequency firing in fast-spiking cortical interneurons. Because PV-containing cortical interneurons in the deep layers prominently express both Kv3.2 and Kv3.1 proteins (probably in heteromeric channels), whereas PV-containing neurons in superficial layers express mainly Kv3.1 subunits (Chow et al., 1999), we predicted that PV-containing neurons in the deep layers would be more affected in Kv3.2 −/− mice than neurons in superficial layers. We confirmed that the levels (data not shown) and distribution (Fig.4) of PV immunoreactivity in the cortex were not affected in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse.
The shape of action potentials and the repetitive firing properties of cells from knock-out and wild-type littermate mice in both superficial and deep cortical layers were compared using whole-cell recording methods from nonpyramidal neurons visualized by IR-DIC optics in slices of somatosensory cortex. Nonpyramidal neurons in both knock-out and wild-type mice had different firing patterns that could be classified into three types, similar to those previously reported in rat and mouse neocortex: regular spiking (RS), low-threshold spiking (LTS), and fast spiking (FS) (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1993, 1997; Cauli et al., 1997;Erisir et al., 1999; Gibson et al., 1999). Under our recording conditions, FS neurons were characterized by having short-duration action potentials and large, brief AHPs. In response to sustained current injection, they fired high-frequency spike trains with abrupt onset and little spike frequency adaptation. These neurons were easily distinguished from regular spiking neurons that sustained much lower maximum frequencies (≤50 vs >100 spikes/sec) and adapted much more (mean rates at the end of a 600 msec pulse were ≤40% of initial rates in RS neurons compared with ≥70% in FS cells). FS neurons could also be distinguished from LTS cells, which showed pronounced spike frequency adaptation (steady-state rates were, on average, <40% of initial rates) and generated low-threshold spikes or spike bursts in response to depolarization from hyperpolarized potentials [as previously described in rat by Kawaguchi and Kubota (1993) and Gibson et al. (1999)]. FS neurons had firing thresholds 10–15 mV more positive than RS and LTS cells and had significantly lower input resistance than the other two types of cells (133 ± 6.2 MΩ,n = 22 for FS, compared with 222.1 ± 22 MΩ,n = 52 for RS, and 349.7 ± 30.2 MΩ,n = 21 for LTS cells) [similar to observations in rat by Kawaguchi and Kubota (1993)].
The action potential and repetitive firing characteristics of a typical multipolar, PV-positive, layer 5 neuron from a wild-type mouse are compared with those of a multipolar, PV-positive, layer 5 neuron from a knock-out littermate in Figures 5 and6. The action potential from the knock-out mouse was broader (width at half maximum of 1.1 vs 0.72 msec) (Fig. 5A1 ) and had a slower maximum rate of repolarization (66 vs 110 mV/msec) (Fig.5A2 , dashed line) than the neuron from the wild-type littermate. In addition, the deceleration of the membrane potential as it entered into the AHP was smaller in the Kv3.2 −/− neuron, suggesting that the repolarization current decays more slowly (Fig. 5A3 , dashed line). These and other differences in action potential shape are summarized in Table 1 and indicate that spike repolarization was impaired in the deep-layer FS neurons from knock-out animals. Despite these differences, the maximum rate of rise of the spike (for single spikes or the initial spike in a train) was similar in the neurons from the two genotypes (Table 1), indicating that the mechanisms responsible for initiating the action potential were unimpaired. To verify that recordings were made from PV-containing neurons, slices were fixed and processed for PV immunohistochemistry after the recording and filling of neurons with biocytin (Du et al., 1996; Erisir et al., 1999). An example from a Kv3.2 −/− mouse is shown in Figure 5B. All of the FS cells that were scored for PV immunoreactivity (see details in Materials and Methods) were PV-positive (n = 14 from wild-type mice and 16 from knock-outs), and the inverse was also true; all of the neurons that were scored positive for PV had been classified as fast-spiking electrophysiologically.
Fast-spiking neurons from wild-type and knock-out mice also differed in their repetitive firing characteristics. Records from a typical PV-positive neuron in deep cortical layers of each genotype are shown in Figure 6. Both cells fired repetitively during steady depolarizations, and in both cases the steady-state firing rate increased as a function of injected current reaching near-saturation values before spike failure took place (Fig. 6D). However, the wild-type neuron was able to sustain higher steady-state firing frequencies (133 vs 66 spikes/sec) and showed significantly less firing frequency adaptation (mean firing rate at the end of a 600 msec pulse was 75% of initial rates for the wild-type cell and 46% for the cell from the Kv3.2 −/− mouse) than the neuron from the knock-out (Fig. 6A–C). Spike failure also occurred at much lower current strengths in the knock-out neuron than in the wild type (Fig. 6B,D).
These differences between wild-type and knock-out deep-layer neurons were reproducible and statistically different when neurons from a large number of mice of each genotype were compared (Fig.7, Table 1). In scatter plots comparing steady-state firing frequency and firing frequency adaptation (Fig.7A) or steady-state firing frequency and action potential width at half maximum (Fig. 7B), the cells from each genotype showed a different, although overlapping, distribution (p < 0.01 for the firing rate;p < 0.02 for the degree of adaptation;p < 0.001 for the spike width; one-way ANOVA). Most of the neurons from the knock-out were in a cluster of cells with lower steady-state frequencies, higher spike frequency adaptation, and wider spikes. Yet, even in the knock-out mice, fast-spiking cells fired faster and adapted less than regular spiking (Fig.7A) or LTS (data not shown) neurons. However, some of the fastest firing neurons from the Kv3.2 −/− animals fired nearly as fast as the fastest firing neurons from the wild-type animals. This may be related to the different relative levels of Kv3.1 and Kv3.2 proteins in individual neurons, given that the expression of Kv3.1 remained unaffected in the Kv3.2 −/− animals. Support for this idea was obtained from experiments with low TEA concentrations described below.
Several parameters that help distinguish fast-spiking neurons from other interneurons in wild-type animals remain unchanged in the mutant mice (Fig. 7C, Table 1) and were therefore also useful to distinguish the neurons electrophysiologically. As in the case of neurons from wild-type mice (see above), FS neurons in knock-out animals had lower input resistance than RS and LTS cells (142 ± 8.6 MΩ, n = 29; 231.8 ± 16.4 MΩ,n = 52; and 329.5 ± 19.7 MΩ, n= 21, respectively), as well as higher firing thresholds (10–15 mV). There usually was more spontaneous synaptic activity observed in records from FS neurons than from the other cell types. LTS cells could also be distinguished from RS and FS neurons in normal and knock-out animals by the presence of low-threshold spikes when depolarized from hyperpolarized potentials, as in normal animals (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1993, 1997; Gibson et al., 1999).
There were no differences between knock-out and wild-type littermates in the firing properties of regular spiking neurons (Fig.7A). Furthermore, in contrast to the large differences in action potential shape and repetitive firing properties of deep-layer FS neurons from wild-type and knock-out mice, no significant differences were observed when FS neurons in superficial layers were compared (Fig. 7D, Table 1).
Low TEA concentrations eliminate the differences between wild-type and Kv3.2 −/− fast-spiking neurons
The differences in the action potential and repetitive firing properties of fast-spiking neurons from wild-type and knock-out mice resemble the effects produced by application of low concentrations of TEA (<1 mm) to neurons from normal mice (Erisir et al., 1999). However, although low TEA concentrations also nearly completely blocked the AHP (Erisir et al., 1999), the AHPs in knock-out mice were on the average only ∼25% smaller than in wild-type mice (Table 1). Moreover, although submillimolar concentrations of TEA affect the magnitude but not significantly the kinetics of the AHP (Erisir et al., 1999), we found that the kinetics of the AHP was different in Kv3.2-deficient and wild-type animals (Fig.5A1 ,A3 ). The fast AHP characteristic of fast-spiking neurons from wild-type mice was replaced by a slower AHP in fast-spiking neurons from Kv3.2 −/− mice (Fig. 5A) (Fig. 6, compareA,B). We hypothesize that the slow AHP in FS neurons from Kv3.2 −/− mice is generated by the increased activation of an unidentified K+conductance (perhaps mediated by Ca2+-activated K+ channels), which deactivates at rates slower than those of Kv3 channels. There is increased activation of this conductance in Kv3.2-deficient mice because of the increase in the duration of the action potential. We would further like to suggest that this is not seen when TEA is used to block Kv3 channels because the drug also blocks this unidentified K+ conductance. Consistent with this idea, low TEA concentrations blocked the slow AHP in neurons from knock-out mice (Fig. 8).
Other than these differential effects on the AHP, all of the effects of the Kv3.2 −/− mutation on the spike and repetitive firing properties of deep-layer fast-spiking neurons resembled the effects of a partial block of Kv3 channels with TEA (∼0.2 mm) on wild-type FS neurons (Erisir et al., 1999). We expected the mutation to be equivalent to a partial block of Kv3 channels because FS neurons in the Kv3.2 −/− mice still express normal levels of Kv3.1 proteins (Figs.1, 2), and Kv3.1 and Kv3.2 proteins express similar currents in heterologous expression systems (Hernandez-Pineda et al., 1999; Rudy et al., 1999). To test this hypothesis, we compared the effects of TEA on the spike width, steady-state firing rate, and degree of adaptation of fast-spiking neurons from wild-type and knock-out mice. The values of these parameters in FS neurons from knock-out animals in the absence of the channel blocker were close to those in wild-type animals in the presence of submillimolar concentrations of TEA. For example, the spike width of deep-layer FS neurons from Kv3.2-deficient mice (0.94 ± 0.04 msec, n = 20) was comparable with the spike width of wild-type mice in 0.2 mm TEA (0.95 ± 0.03 msec, n = 17). Similarly, the steady-state firing frequency and firing frequency adaptation of deep-layer FS neurons from Kv3.2 −/− mice (116.6 ± 10.2 spikes per second,n = 20; and 0.68 ± 0.03, n = 20, respectively) were close to those for wild-type neurons in ≤0.2 mm TEA (92.9 ± 2.9, n = 16; and 0.62 ± 0.04, n = 15, in 0.2 mm TEA) (Fig. 9). Furthermore, although TEA still had effects on spike width and repetitive firing properties of FS neurons from knock-out animals, the effects were smaller than those observed in neurons from wild-type animals (Fig. 9). In 1.0 mm TEA, a drug concentration expected to block ≥80% of the total Kv3 channels, knock-out and wild-type neurons had similar properties (e.g., 63 ± 6 spikes/sec, n = 15; and 55 ± 3 spikes/sec,n = 18, for the steady-state firing rate of wild-type and knock-out FS neurons, respectively, in 1 mmTEA) (Fig. 9C). This would be expected if the mutation and the drug (at concentrations of ≤1 mm) are acting, for the most part, on the same conductance. These data are also consistent with the notion that we are recording from similar neurons in the two types of animals and that, other than the lack of the mutated channel proteins, the cells have not changed significantly in active conductances or other membrane properties. Moreover, these results also suggest that the dispersion in steady-state firing frequency values between different FS neurons might be the result of differences in the number of active Kv3 channels. Because Kv3 channel activity can be modulated by neurotransmitters and second messengers (Moreno et al., 1995; Atzori et al., 2000), different cortical neurons could have different proportions of active channels depending on having been recent targets of these modulators.
FS neurons can repetitively fire spike doublets at 40 Hz, an ability that requires Kv3 channels
Under certain physiological conditions, fast-spiking neurons may not undergo long, steady depolarizations such as those used in the previous experiments. For example, it has been shown in both the neocortex and the hippocampus (Traub et al., 1996, 1999; Steriade et al., 1998) that, when stimulated, fast-spiking neurons generate fast rhythmic (∼40 Hz) spike bursts. The bursts consist of two (spike doublets) to three spikes with intraburst frequencies that are similar to the steady-state frequencies observed during long, steady depolarizations (100–200 Hz at room temperature; 300–400 Hz at 37°C). We asked whether the ability to generate such rhythmic bursts repetitively is impaired in FS neurons from the Kv3.2 −/− mice. Cortical neurons were stimulated repetitively with brief depolarizations that generated one or more spikes per stimulus, repeated at various frequencies. We focus on the results at 40 Hz, the characteristic frequency observed in the EEG during periods of brain activation (Bouyer et al., 1981; Llinas and Ribary, 1993; Murthy and Fetz, 1996a,b; Steriade et al., 1996). In wild-type mice, both regular-spiking and fast-spiking GABAergic interneurons in both superficial and deep cortical layers could follow 40 Hz stimuli that generated a single spike per stimulus for periods up to 1 min (the longest tested; data not shown). However, only fast-spiking neurons could follow larger stimuli that generated a spike doublet without failing (Fig. 10). In contrast, deep-layer, fast-spiking neurons from knock-out mice failed in their ability to fire spike doublets repetitively much more rapidly than did neurons from wild-type animals (Fig.10A,B). In fact, in Kv3.2 −/− mice, during the first stimulus, the second spike was already smaller than the first spike, and it got smaller, slower, and more delayed with subsequent depolarizations (Fig. 10A), suggesting a reduction in the available Na+ current. This is consistent with the hypothesis that Kv3 channels facilitate fast spiking by increasing Na+ channel recovery from inactivation (Erisir et al., 1999). This behavior was similar to that observed after application of low concentrations of TEA on deep-layer FS neurons from wild-type mice (data not shown).
Increased cortical excitability in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse
The cellular analysis showed that the ability of fast-spiking interneurons to fire at high frequencies for long durations or repetitively is impaired in Kv3.2 −/− mice. The knock-out mice are thus useful to test hypotheses of the function of these neurons and the significance of fast spiking in their performance. The presence of sporadic epileptic seizures in Kv3.2-deficient mice suggests that the mice have an increased susceptibility to seizures. Here we present a series of observations that provide further evidence of increased cortical excitability and susceptibility to seizures in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse.
The first experiment consisted of recording spontaneous EEG from cortex and thalamus in ketamine–xylazine anesthetized Kv3.2 −/− and wild-type mice. Results were obtained with multisite extracellular and field potential recordings using arrays of high-impedance tungsten electrodes. As shown in Figure11A, wild-type mice (n = 21) showed the characteristic slow rhythm observed during natural slow-wave sleep and during ketamine–xylazine anesthesia in other species (Steriade et al., 1993a; Contreras and Steriade, 1995). In wild-type mice, the slow rhythm was characterized by recurring sequences at <1 Hz of depth-negative followed by depth-positive waves in the EEG, which occurred in synchrony with the corresponding thalamic territory. Such sequences correspond intracellularly to neuronal depolarization and hyperpolarization, respectively (Steriade et al., 1993a; Contreras and Steriade, 1995). The EEG of the Kv3.2 −/− mice (n = 18) showed, superimposed on the underlying slow rhythm, spontaneous high-amplitude sharp potentials, lasting from 30 to 100 msec and with the same polarity as the slow oscillation (depth-negative and surface-positive). Such sharp potentials pervaded throughout all phases of the slow oscillation and produced a very irregular slow rhythm (Fig.11B). Nevertheless, the slow rhythm is clearly present, suggesting that the cortical synchronization responsible for this rhythm still occurs in the knock-out. The “spikiness” of the slow rhythm in Kv3.2-deficient mice, illustrated with the example in Figure 11B, is the most characteristic effect of the mutation that we have observed until now. All Kv3.2 −/− mice tested showed similar irregularities on the EEG, which we have never seen in wild-type littermates or in mice from commercial sources. The high degree of spatiotemporal synchrony of the slow rhythm is achieved by massive reentrant corticocortical circuits, as shown by the absence of effects of massive ipsilateral thalamectomy followed by sectioning of the callosum (Steriade et al., 1993b). We propose that the sharp potentials in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse reflect a poor local control of the strong synchronized corticocortical inputs that reach the cortex during the slow oscillations.
In the course of the previous experiments, we found that seizures were prevalent in Kv3.2 −/− mice under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. Under these conditions, most (12 of 18) Kv3.2 −/− mice, but not their wild-type littermates (0 of 21), showed electrographic seizures that occurred spontaneously (typically every 10–20 min) (Fig.12A). Similar seizures were easily induced in Kv3.2 −/−mice (12 of 12 mice tested) by electrical stimulation of the thalamus (Fig. 12B). In contrast, we were never able to evoke seizures in wild-type animals (n = 19), even with stimuli that were 10 times stronger or longer in duration (and in a variety of patterns) than those required to evoke seizures in knock-out mice. Both spontaneous and evoked seizures were often associated with motor manifestations that varied from small jerks of the limbs and movements of the whiskers to full convulsions. In principle, these seizures could result from increased thalamic input into the cortex or from reduced cortical control in Kv3.2-deficient mice. This is an important consideration, given the prominent expression of Kv3.2 in thalamic relay neurons and thalamocortical projections (Fig. 2) (Rudy et al., 1992; Weiser et al., 1994; Moreno et al., 1995). However, the failure to induce seizures when large doses of bicuculline are injected into the thalamus (Steriade and Contreras, 1998) shows that the feedforward and feedback inhibitory mechanisms of the cortex are capable of handling the entrance into the cortex of thalamic stimuli of increased magnitude. Therefore, the presence of thalamic-induced seizures in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse is likely to result from increased cortical excitability.
The last set of experiments consisted of testing seizure susceptibility to chemical convulsants in vivo. Kv3.2 −/− mice were more sensitive to the convulsant PTZ, a GABAAreceptor antagonist. When challenged with a single dose of PTZ (50 mg/kg), Kv3.2 −/− mice showed more severe responses than wild-type littermates (Fig. 13A). For example, at this dose, 52% of Kv3.2 −/− mice (n = 23) compared with 16% of wild types (n = 19) developed severe stage 4 tonic-clonic convulsions associated with death. Similarly, the delay between drug application and the first evidence of epileptic activity was significantly shorter in the knock-out mice (Fig. 13B).
Together, the data provide strong evidence that mice deficient in Kv3.2 subunits have susceptibility to seizures probably reflecting an increase in cortical excitability. Because Kv3.2 proteins are expressed in inhibitory but not in excitatory neurons in the cortex (Chow et al., 1999), and assuming that compensatory changes in other cortical elements have not taken place, the inferred increase in cortical excitability most likely arises from impaired cortical inhibition. Deficits in cortical inhibition may occur in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse if the fast-spiking ability of GABAergic interneurons is necessary to achieve proper levels of inhibition (see Discussion). This increased cortical excitability is likely to contribute to the spontaneous seizures observed in awake animals.
DISCUSSION
Kv3 channels and high frequency firing
Three (Kv3.1–Kv3.3) of the four Kv3 genes are prominently expressed in brain tissue and show different but overlapping patterns of expression (Perney et al., 1992; Rudy et al., 1992; Vega-Saenz de Miera et al., 1994; Weiser et al., 1994). Many, if not most, of the neuronal populations expressing these genes fire spikes at high frequency. This correlation led to the suggestion that Kv3 channels may play an important role in high-frequency firing (see introductory remarks). Experiments using the K+ channel blockers TEA and 4-AP, at concentrations that block Kv3 channels in heterologous expression systems (Grissmer et al., 1994; Coetzee et al., 1999; Rudy et al., 1999), and Kv3-like currents in native neurons (Du et al., 1996; Wang et al., 1998; Erisir et al., 1999; Hernandez-Pineda et al., 1999) support the idea that Kv3 channels play a dominant role in repolarizing the action potential of expressing neurons and are necessary to maintain high-frequency firing (Du et al., 1996;Massengill et al., 1997; Martina et al., 1998; Wang et al., 1998;Erisir et al., 1999). However, the possibility that these drugs are also blocking other types of K+ channels has been difficult to eliminate.
In this study, we used the genetic elimination of the Kv3.2gene, which is prominently expressed in deep-layer FS neurons in the neocortex, to test directly the role of Kv3 genes in fast spiking. The results show that fast spiking is impaired in neocortical deep-layer FS neurons but not in superficial layer FS neurons in which Kv3.2 is weakly expressed. These results provide strong, independent evidence that Kv3 channels play a critical role in enabling high-frequency firing in FS neurons. Together with data showing that the PKA-dependent modulation of the firing frequency of FS interneurons in the hippocampus is absent in Kv3.2 −/− mice (Atzori et al., 2000), the experiments described here provide the most direct evidence for a critical role of Kv3 channels in sustained or repetitive high-frequency firing.
The ability of Kv3 channels to facilitate sustained or repetitive high-frequency firing is a direct consequence of the special properties of these channels that distinguish them from other voltage-gated K+ channels. (1) By activating at very depolarized potentials, the cell can use large numbers of Kv3 channels to produce fast-spike repolarization and a large AHP with minimum effects on input resistance, threshold, or rise time, thus keeping action potentials brief without compromising action potential generation. (2) By keeping action potentials brief, Kv3 channels minimize the inactivation of the Na+conductance during the spike. (3) By generating a large AHP, Kv3 channels accelerate recovery from the Na+channel inactivation that did take place during the spike. (4) The brief duration of the AHP (produced by the fast deactivation of Kv3 channels) restores high-input resistance quickly. The increase in the number of Na+ channels that have recovered from inactivation and the fast termination of the AHP minimize the duration of the refractory period, allowing the cell to reach firing threshold sooner than in the absence of Kv3 channels (Sekirnjak et al., 1997; Erisir et al., 1999).
The effects of blocking Kv3 channels become stronger during long trains or repetitive activity because there is accumulation of Na+ channel inactivation resulting from the repeated activation of the channels. The larger increase in spike rise time during repetitive activity in Kv3.2-deficient mice (Figs.5A1 ,A2 , 10) supports this view. The results with Kv3.2-deficient mice also suggest that the increased activation of slower K+conductances active during the AHP, produced by the increase in action potential duration, may also contribute to reducing firing frequency and increasing firing frequency adaptation after Kv3 channel removal.
Evidence that Kv3 channels are necessary for sustained or repetitive high-frequency firing has now been obtained for FS neurons in the neocortex and hippocampus (Martina et al., 1998; Erisir et al., 1999;Atzori et al., 2000; this study), for neurons in the medial vestibular nucleus (MVN) (S. du Lac and C. Sekirnjak, unpublished observations) and the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB) in the auditory brainstem (Wang et al., 1998). Kv3 gene products are found in many other neurons capable of high-frequency firing (Weiser et al., 1994), suggesting a similar role in these neurons. Although it appears that Kv3 channels are necessary for sustained or repetitive high-frequency firing in many (if not all) high-frequency firing neurons, their presence is clearly not sufficient. The effects on excitability of the Kv3 channels will depend on the other conductances active on the cell and their precise localization in the neuron. In fact, the three cell types (FS interneurons in the neocortex and hippocampus, neurons in the MNTB, and neurons in the MVN) for which a role of Kv3 channels in high-frequency firing has been suggested have different firing properties. Both FS interneurons and MVN neurons (du Lac and Lisberger, 1995; du Lac, 1996) fire long, high-frequency spike trains in response to steady depolarizations. However, FS interneurons produce long, high-frequency spike trains abruptly, and the steady-state firing frequency saturates as a function of injected current (as in the examples shown here). In contrast, MVN neurons are spontaneously active, and the relationship between injected current and firing frequency is nearly linear (du Lac and Lisberger, 1995; du Lac, 1996). The presence of low voltage-activating or persistent Na+ channels and differences in other conductances probably account for these differences in firing properties.
On the other hand, the MNTB neurons produce one or, at most, two action potentials independently of the duration or strength of the depolarization, probably because depolarization activates large, low voltage-activating “D”-type K+currents that limit repetitive activity (Brew and Forsythe, 1995). However, these neurons can fire action potentials entrained to very high-frequency inputs (>600 Hz), the property which appears to depend on Kv3 (probably Kv3.1 and Kv3.3) channels (Wang et al., 1998).
Kv3 channels are an excellent example of how the specialized properties of certain channels contribute to functional specificity. This helps to explain the biological significance of K+channel diversity as a key factor contributing to the diversity of the electrophysiological properties of neurons and to the specificity of neuromodulator actions (Adams and Galvan, 1986; Llinas, 1988; Rudy, 1988; Baxter and Byrne, 1991; Hille, 1992).
Increased cortical excitability in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse
Several observations indicate that Kv3.2-deficient mice have increased cortical excitability. Increased excitability, resulting in increased susceptibility to seizures, if not a full epileptic behavior, has also been observed in two other K+channel knock-out mice lines: Kv1.1 (Smart et al., 1998) and one of the G-protein-gated inward rectifier K+channel genes (GIRK2) (Signorini et al., 1997). Moreover, mutations in the K+ channel genes KCNQ2 and KCNQ3, subunits of M-type K+ channels, have been found in humans with benign familial neonatal convulsions (Charlier et al., 1998; Singh et al., 1998). A strong association between increased excitability and K+ channel dysfunction may not seem surprising. K+ channels typically suppress and limit cell excitability by competing with depolarizing currents, and thus, reduced K+ channel expression is expected to generate hyperexcitability. Indeed, 4-AP and other K+ channel blockers are often used as convulsants in experimental animals (Rutecki et al., 1987; Velluti et al., 1987).
However, it is very likely that the underlying mechanism responsible for the increased cortical excitability of Kv3.2-deficient mice is different from that producing the hyperexcitability phenotypes in the other examples. K+ channels containing Kv1.1, GIRK2, and KCNQ2 and KCNQ3 subunits operate close to the resting potential and act as breaks dampening the effects of depolarizing inputs. Moreover, these channels are prominently expressed in excitatory neurons in neocortex, hippocampus, and many other brain areas. Hyperexcitability of these cells, resulting from suppressing these channels, is likely to underlie the hyperexcitability behavior (Signorini et al., 1997; Smart et al., 1998). However, in the neocortex and hippocampus, Kv3.2 is only expressed by inhibitory neurons (Chow et al., 1999; Atzori et al., 2000). The defects in the function of these cells are likely to be key contributors to the observed increases in cortical excitability, although this remains to be conclusively demonstrated.
Hyperexcitability of these interneurons should produce increased cortical inhibition and therefore suppression of hyperexcitable phenotypes, contrary to what we see in the Kv3.2 −/− mouse. The solution to this apparent paradox lies in the unique role of Kv3 channels in neuronal excitability as illustrated by the results obtained with the Kv3.2-deficient mouse, which suggest that suppression of this K+ channel results in impaired cellular firing instead of hyperexcitability. Kv3 channels are not active anywhere near threshold potentials, and therefore their removal is not expected to affect much the responsiveness to synaptic inputs. As shown here and in previous pharmacological studies (Du et al., 1996;Massengill et al., 1997; Erisir et al., 1999), suppression of Kv3 channels produces broadening of the action potential. This could increase GABA release from these cells, which should result in increased cortical inhibition. On the other hand, cortical inhibition might be reduced if the ability to sustain repetitive firing at high frequencies is even more important in determining inhibitory levels. Our data suggest that this is the case and that impaired fast spiking decreases the performance of the inhibitory circuits in the cortex. This view is consistent with observations that FS neurons fire long, high-frequency trains or repetitive high-frequency bursts of action potentials in response to physiological stimuli (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1993; Benardo, 1994; Zhu and Connors, 1999). Future studies on the performance of inhibitory synaptic transmission in knock-out and wild-type animals should provide further tests of this view.
The fact that a single type of K+ channel plays a dominant role in enabling high-frequency firing has allowed us to generate mice lines in which this property is impaired in selective neuronal populations. Preliminary experiments show cellular changes similar to those described here on FS neurons in superficial cortical layers in Kv3.1-deficient mice (Ho et al., 1997), and larger effects resembling those produced by complete Kv3 channel block (and throughout all layers of the cortex) are expected on mice deficient in both genes. GABAergic interneurons are thought to play important roles in many cortical functions. These mice lines will be interesting models to test hypotheses on the role of GABAergic interneurons in these functions. For example, preliminary observations in the hippocampus revealed a decrease in high-frequency oscillations in Kv3.2 −/− mice, suggesting impaired synchronization (Atzori et al., 2000), one of the key roles attributed to cortical GABAergic interneurons.
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health, American Lebanese Syrian Associated Charities, and the National Science Foundation. D.L. was supported by the Medical Scientists Training Program grant at New York University School of Medicine.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Bernardo Rudy, Department of Physiology and Neuroscience, New York University School of Medicine, 550 First Avenue, New York, NY 10016. E-mail:Rudyb01{at}med.nyu.edu.