Abstract
To determine whether afferent innervation regulates the strength of inhibitory connections in the gerbil auditory midbrain, both cochleas were surgically removed in postnatal day 7 animals, before sound-driven activity is first observed. Inhibitory synaptic currents were measured in a brain slice preparation 1–7 d after the ablations. Whole-cell and gramicidin-perforated patch recordings were obtained from inferior colliculus neurons, and IPSCs were evoked by stimulation of the commissure of the inferior colliculus (CIC) or the ipsilateral lateral lemniscus (LL) in the presence of kynurenic acid. Deafferentation led to a 24 mV depolarizing shift in the IPSC equilibrium potential within 1 d of deafferentation. As a consequence, there was a large reduction of IPSC amplitude at a holding potential of −20 mV in neurons from bilaterally ablated animals. Furthermore, both afferent pathways displayed a 50% reduction of the inhibitory synaptic conductance after deafferentation, indicating that driving force was not solely responsible for the decline in IPSC amplitude. When paired pulses were delivered to the LL or CIC pathway in control neurons, the evoked IPSCs exhibited facilitation. However, paired pulse facilitation was nearly eliminated after deafferentation. Thus, normal innervation affects inhibitory synaptic strength by regulating postsynaptic chloride homeostasis and presynaptic transmitter release properties.
Spontaneous and evoked electrical activity play a critical role in the maturation of central neuronal circuits. Complete or partial sensory deprivation can produce alterations at all levels of the system, and these effects are most profound during early development. For example, the loss or complete inactivation of the cochlea leads to the cessation of protein synthesis, cell shrinkage, dendrite retraction, and cell death in the chick auditory brainstem (Parks, 1979; Deitch and Rubel, 1984; Born and Rubel, 1988). To understand how alteration of synaptic architecture and protein expression might affect synaptic processing, it is important to learn the functional properties of these connections. Because central neurons receive a rich array of excitatory, inhibitory, and modulatory afferents, it is necessary to study their response to disuse on an individual basis. The present study examines the effect of deafferentation on inhibitory afferents in the auditory midbrain.
Use-dependent alterations of inhibitory synapses have been demonstrated in the developing and adult nervous system. When organotypic cultures of the mouse cerebellum are grown in tetrodotoxin to block electrical activity, the number of inhibitory synapses is selectively reduced (Seil and Drake-Baumann, 1994). The regulation of inhibitory synaptic strength may derive, in part, from the stimulation of receptor synthesis and clustering by GABA and glycine (Poulter et al., 1997;Kirsch and Betz, 1998). Alternatively, the accumulation and release of transmitter may also be compromised by changes in the level of activity (Hendry and Jones, 1986; Wickesberg et al., 1994; Hirsch et al., 1999).
We have previously shown that denervated afferent pathways undergo rapid changes in strength. For example, evoked inhibitory postsynaptic responses in the lateral superior olivary nucleus (LSO) become smaller within 24 hr of deafferentation (Kotak and Sanes, 1996). Furthermore, this decrease is accompanied by a 8 mV depolarization in the IPSP reversal potential, suggesting that chloride homeostasis might be affected. Inhibitory disuse also results in heterosynaptic affects. Thus, when the contralateral inhibitory input is disrupted, the unmanipulated excitatory pathway becomes stronger, apparently because of the addition of functional NMDA receptors (Kotak and Sanes, 1996). Changes in the strength of inhibitory afferents have also been suggested by in vivo studies of deafness and presbycusis (Kitzes and Semple, 1985; Milbrandt et al., 1994; Bledsoe et al., 1995;Caspary et al., 1995; McAlpine et al., 1997; Milbrandt et al., 1997).
These studies raise interesting questions about the functional status of synapses in higher regions of the CNS. Therefore, we examined the inferior colliculus where the role of synaptic inhibition in auditory processing has been most broadly studied (Park and Pollak 1993a,b, 1994; Le Beau et al., 1996; Sanes et al., 1998). The present study asked whether the disruption of all spontaneous activity generated in the cochlea before the onset of sound-driven activity commenced would influence the functional development of two different inhibitory pathways through the lateral lemniscus (LL) and the commissure of the inferior colliculus (CIC).
Parts of these results were published previously in abstract form (Vale and Sanes, 1999).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Cochlear ablation. All protocols were reviewed and approved by the New York University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Cochlear ablations were performed as described previously (Sanes et al., 1992). Gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus) pups at postnatal day 7 (P7) were anesthetized with hypothermia until respiration ceased and animals did not respond to nociceptive stimuli. A small hole was made in the cochlear wall, and the contents were rapidly removed with a forceps. A piece of Gelfoam was placed in the cavity, and the wound was closed. Ablations were performed bilaterally. After surgery, animals were warmed on a heating pad and returned to the litter when respiration and motor activity recovered. Bilaterally ablated animals were fed with a sterile saline solution containing 0.1% glucose at 2–3 d after surgery. Successful ablations were confirmed before each brain slice experiment.
Brain slice preparation. Control and bilaterally ablated P8–P14 gerbils were anesthetized with chloral hydrate (350 mg/kg). After decapitation, the brain was blocked at the level of the thalamus and the caudal hindbrain. The ventral surface of the brain was affixed to an agar block (cyanoacrylate glue), and the block was secured to the stage of a vibratome (Leica, Nussloch, Germany). The tissue was cut in cold oxygenated artificial CSF (ACSF) containing (in mm): 123 NaCl, 4 KCl, 1.2 KH2PO4, 1.3 MgSO4, 28 NaHCO3, 15 glucose, 2.4 CaCl2, and 0.4l-ascorbic acid, pH 7.3 when oxygenated with 95%O2/5%CO2. Slices of 300 μm were obtained in cold ACSF and maintained in an incubation chamber at room temperature for 2 hr. Slices containing the rostral inferior colliculus (IC) were placed in a recording chamber and superfused with oxygenated ACSF (7 ml/min) at room temperature.
Electrophysiology. Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings (Warner Instruments PC-501A) were obtained as described previously (Kotak et al., 1998; Moore et al., 1998). Recording electrodes were fabricated from borosilicate glass microcapillaries (1.5 mm outer diameter), and the tip resistance was 5–10 MΩ. For whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings, the internal pipette solution contained (in mm): 127.5 cesium gluconate, 0.6 EGTA, 10 HEPES, 2 MgCl2, 5 KCl, 2 ATP, 0.3 GTP, and 5 QX-314 (Alamone, Jerusalem, Israel), pH 7.2. A measured liquid junction potential of ∼5 mV was not corrected, because our primary objective was to compare measures between control and deafferented neurons. This error should affect both values equally. Biocytin (∼0.2%) was also added to the recording solution. Extracellular stimuli (200 μsec pulses) were delivered through paired Teflon-insulated platinum electrodes driven by isolated biphasic stimulators (Intronics Instruments). Custom personal computer-based software was used for programmed stimulus delivery, data acquisition, and analysis (Sanes, 1993). Stimulating electrodes were placed in the afferent pathways from the CIC and the LL. The maximum amplitude of evoked synaptic currents was typically obtained at membrane holding potentials (Ehold) of −80 and −20 mV. IPSC duration was determined from the rising latency to the time at which the signal returned to the baseline noise level. The holding potential at which IPSCs reversed (EIPSC) was also measured. At the end of each recording, slices were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde, and a subset were processed to visualize biocytin-filled neurons using an avidin–biotin complex coupled to horseradish peroxidase (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA).
Passive membrane properties were evaluated using whole-cell current-clamp recordings (Warner Instruments PC-501A) in the presence of kynurenic acid. The internal pipette solution contained (in mm): 135 potassium gluconate, 2 MgCl2, 5 KCl, 0.6 EGTA, 10 HEPES, 2 MgCl2, 2 ATP, and 0.3 GTP, pH 7.2. The neuron input resistance was calculated from the slope of current–voltage plots. Time constants were determined as the time to reach 67% of the plateau amplitude after 0.1 nA hyperpolarizing current pulses.
Many recordings (n = 86) were performed using the perforated patch technique (Rhee et al., 1994; Kyrozis and Reichling, 1995). Gramicidin (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) was used as the membrane-perforating agent to permit the recording of IPSCs without influencing the cytoplasmic chloride concentration. Gramicidin was dissolved in dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO; 2–5 mg/ml) and then diluted in the pipette solution to a final concentration of 2–5 μg/ml (0.2% DMSO). In our preparation, a further increase in gramicidin concentrations led to rapid perforation and membrane rupture during the recordings. Gramicidin concentrations used in this study were lower than those used in some preparations (Kazaku et al., 1999) but close to those used for recordings from immature neurons of the cortex or auditory brainstem (Owens et al., 1996; Backus et al., 1998). For perforated patch recordings, KCl was used in the pipette solution to preserve the function of potassium chloride cotransporters. With this solution, the measured liquid junction potential was ∼4 mV, and the measurements were uncorrected, because our primary objective was to compare measures between control and deafferented neurons. QX-314 was retained in the intracellular pipette solution to confirm that the membrane did not rupture. The presence of depolarization-evoked breakaway action potentials was taken as indication of the integrity of the gramicidin perforation. The progress of perforation was evaluated by monitoring the decrease in membrane resistance. After the membrane resistance had stabilized (between 5 and 40 min after obtaining the GΩ seal), data were obtained.
Ionotropic glutamate receptors were blocked by adding a broad-spectrum ionotropic glutamate receptor antagonist, 5 mm kynurenic acid (Fluka Chemical, Ronkonkoma, NY) to the ACSF. The total IPSC was blocked by the sequential addition of 2 μm strychnine (Sigma), a glycine receptor antagonist, and 10 μmbicuculline methobromide (Research Biochemicals, Natick, MA), a GABAA receptor antagonist, to the ACSF. During the course of these experiments, we discovered a subpopulation of GABAA receptors that were less sensitive to bicuculline, and this antagonist was used at higher concentrations in some experiments, as indicated.
A one-way ANOVA followed by pairwise comparisons (Student'st test) was used to assess whether significant differences existed between neurons from control and bilaterally ablated animals, except as noted in Results. All values are expressed as mean ± SEM, with the number of observations in parentheses. TheEIPSC was calculated from linear fits of the current–voltage curves plotting IPSC amplitude versus membrane holding potential. Inhibitory synaptic conductance was calculated as the slope of these same current–voltage curves.
RESULTS
Of the total number of IC neurons recorded in whole-cell voltage-clamp mode from control and experimental animals (n = 49), 27 were recovered histologically and were assigned to locations in the rostrocaudal plane and the central or lateral position. Fourteen cells were located in central positions, and the remaining stained cells were located either in lateral positions in the IC or in the border between the central and external cortex of the IC. Centrally located neurons within the IC generally exhibited the largest postsynaptic currents. All recorded IC neurons displayed a synaptic response to independent stimulation of the CIC and the LL afferent pathways. Before the addition of kynurenic acid, LL- and CIC-evoked compound PSCs were typically inward atEhold = −80 and outward or mixed atEhold = −20 (Fig.1).
Synaptic physiology in control neurons
The LL-evoked PSC amplitudes are shown for control neurons in Table 1 and Figure 1A. At Ehold = −80, the application of kynurenic acid (KYN) reduced LL-evoked inward synaptic currents by >60% but did not affect PSC amplitude atEhold = −20 (Table 1). The synaptic current that remained in the presence of KYN (total IPSC) was composed of glycinergic and GABAergic components. The addition of 2 μm strychnine (SN) decreased the IPSC by 35–40%, and the addition of 10 μm bicuculline (BIC) led to a further reduction in amplitude (Table 1, Fig.1A). However, a significant synaptic current (>20 pA) remained in 7 of 17 control IC neurons, even in the presence of all three antagonists. Three of these neurons were tested with 5 μm SN, but this did not decrease the remaining current. In four of these neurons, increasing BIC to 20–50 μm led to the elimination of the remaining currents.
Given the apparent differential sensitivity of IC neurons to bicuculline, a set of dose–response experiments were performed with BIC. Figure 2A shows the response of increasing BIC concentrations on the amplitude of the postsynaptic GABAergic current in one IC neuron (in the presence of KYN and SN). The inhibition produced by 10 μm BIC varied from 50 to 100% among the individual IC neurons (Fig.2B). In all cases, the remaining currents in the presence of KYN, SN, and 10 μm BIC were completely abolished by increasing the concentration of BIC to 50 μm.
Bilateral deafferentation: passive membrane properties
Bilateral cochlear ablation (BCA) did not produce a large change in the passive membrane properties of IC neurons. Membrane input resistance was not significantly altered in BCA neurons (control, 311 ± 28 MΩ; n = 15; BCA, 268 ± 45 MΩ;n = 16). However, BCA caused a small, but significant, decrease in the membrane time constant from 30 ± 2 msec (n = 15) in control to 22 ± 2 msec (n = 16) in BCA neurons (df = 29;t = −2.388; p = 0.02). A decrease in the membrane time constant suggests that synaptic responses should be faster in BCA neurons. However, LL-evoked IPSCs from control neurons actually exhibited a slightly larger rising slope compared with BCA neurons (control, −55 ± 9 pA/msec; BCA, −32 ± 5 pA/msec; df = 43; t = 2.112; p = 0.04). Therefore, passive membrane properties of BCA neurons were not expected to influence our measures of synaptic transmission.
Bilateral deafferentation: whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings
The following experiments document the size of evoked synaptic currents under different pharmacological conditions. The goal was to isolate the inhibitory component (IPSCs) in control and BCA neurons. The IPSCs were analyzed at an Ehold of −20 and −80 mV because the EIPSC was generally in between these two potentials. The changes that we observed were similar for LL- and CIC-evoked synaptic currents. Therefore, the results are presented together in this section, and all results are summarized in Tables 1 and 2.
Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were obtained with cesium gluconate in the internal pipette solution. Figure 1 shows examples of afferent-evoked synaptic currents in control and BCA neurons under each pharmacological condition. Although there was a significant decrease in the maximum amplitude of the LL-evoked mixed PSCs (Table 1, Fig. 1A), we do not believe that these effects can be interpreted easily. Therefore, our analyses focused on pharmacologically isolated synaptic inhibition.
The amplitudes of LL-evoked total IPSCs were significantly decreased, both at Ehold = −20 and −80 mV (Fig. 1A, Table 1). The maximum amplitude of LL-evoked IPSCs was ∼70% smaller at −80 mV (df = 43;t = 2.76; p = 0.008) and ∼80% smaller at −20 mV (df = 41; t = −3.63;p = 0.0008). Furthermore, IPSC duration was also decreased significantly after ablation (df = 43; t= −3.81; p = 0.0004). For example, at a holding potential of −80 mV, control IPSCs had a maximum duration of 125 ± 12 msec (n = 25), and this declined to 61 ± 11 msec (n = 20) in BCA neurons.
The amplitude of CIC-evoked postsynaptic currents in control and BCA neurons under each pharmacological condition is shown in Figure1B and Table 2. Interestingly, BCA neurons only displayed smaller IPSCs at Ehold = −20 mV (df = 44; t = −2.82; p = 0.007). At this holding potential, the maximum CIC-evoked total IPSCs were ∼80% smaller than those of control neurons (Fig.1B, Table 2). Although the mean total IPSC amplitude was 40% below controls at Ehold = −80, this value did not reach significance.
The dissimilar findings at each holding potential could be explained ifEIPSC shifted toward −20 mV in BCA neurons. That is, the reduced IPSC amplitude at a holding potential of −20 mV may be attributable to reduced activity of GABA or glycine receptors, decreased presynaptic transmitter release, and a decreased driving force. In contrast, the IPSC amplitude recorded at a holding potential of −80 mV was not reduced, because an increase in chloride driving force could compensate for decreased receptor function or transmitter release.
In fact, BCA neurons exhibited a significant depolarizing shift in the total IPSC reversal potential (df = 41; t = 2.9;p = 0.005). The EIPSCof control neurons was −40 ± 3 (n = 23) versus −26 ± 4 (n = 20) mV in BCA neurons. A similar finding was observed for LL-evoked IPSCs (data not shown). Figure3 shows examples ofEIPSC measured in one control and one BCA neuron. Because the driving force (Ehold −EIPSC) influences IPSC amplitude, the inhibitory conductance was calculated from the slope of the IPSC current–voltage curves. For CIC-evoked currents, there was a significant decrease in the conductance of the total IPSC (df = 36; t = −2.66; p = 0.011) in BCA neurons (Fig. 4). The conductance for LL-evoked total IPSC was also significatively decreased in ablated animals (df = 36; t = −3.32; p = 0.002).
To determine whether the changes in evoked-IPSCs were attributable to changes in GABAergic afferents, neurons were bathed in 5 mm KYN plus 2 μm SN. Table 1 shows that LL-evoked GABAergic IPSCs were smaller in BCA neurons compared with controls. Table 2 shows that CIC-evoked IPSCs were smaller at a holding potential of −20 but not −80 mV. Again, this was probably because of the 15 mV depolarization of the GABAergicEIPSC (control, −44 ± 3 mV;n = 18; BCA, −29 ± 6 mV; n = 14; df = 30; t = 2.383; p = 0.0237). As shown in Figure 4, the calculated conductance for CIC-evoked GABAergic IPSCs was ∼50% smaller in BCA neurons (df = 27;t = −2.40; p = 0.023). A similar observation was made for LL-evoked GABAergic IPSCs (Fig. 4).
In control IC neurons, a significant component of the IPSC remained in the presence of all three antagonists (5 mm KYN, 2 μm SN, and 10 μm BIC). This was found for both LL- and CIC-evoked IPSCs (Tables 1, 2). However, this remaining current was absent in BCA neurons at all holding potentials. Therefore, the GABAergic component that was less sensitive to BIC in control neurons (Fig. 2) was apparently absent after deafferentation.
Gramicidin-perforated patch recordings
Because our initial results with whole-cell recordings showed that the IPSC equilibrium potential was regulated by ablation, we repeated the experiments using gramicidin-perforated patch recordings in which chloride ions do not pass through the perforation. Furthermore, we used normal potassium in the pipette so as not to perturb potassium chloride cotransporter mechanisms (Kazaku et al., 1999). In perforated patch recordings, the resting membrane potential of control and BCA neurons was −55 ± 1 mV (n = 45) and −58 ± 1 mV (n = 33), respectively.
Figure 5A shows LL-evoked IPSCs obtained at a series of holding potentials for individual control and BCA neurons. The current–voltage relationship was linear in both cases (Fig. 5B). As shown in Figure6A, theEIPSC measured in the presence of KYN was depolarized by 24 mV after deafferentation (control, −74 ± 2 mV; n = 45; BCA, −50 ± 2 mV; n = 33; df = 76; t = 8.537; p < 0.0001). The intracellular Cl−concentration ([Cl–]i) was calculated by the Nernst equation, assuming:ECl− =EIPSP = RT/Fln([Cl−]o/[Cl−]i). Given an extracellular free Cl−concentration ([Cl−]o) of 133.8 mm, and T = 293 K (20°C), the estimated [Cl−]i was elevated from 8 ± 1 mm (n = 45) in control neurons to 22 ± 3 mm(n = 33) in BCA neurons. This calculation assumes that the ligand-coupled channels are exclusively permeable to Cl−. As shown in Figure6B, the effect of deafferentation onEIPSC was rapid. The regression lines through the plot indicate that the control and experimental groups diverged within 1 d of the manipulation. Multiple ANOVA of these data indicated a significant effect of both age (F = 5.12; p = 0.026) and experimental condition (F = 73.96; p < 0.0001). Also shown in Figure 6B are the results of recordings from control P7 neurons, the age of the bilateral ablations, which showed thatEIPSC was −55 ± 5 mV (n = 8).
Because the effect of BCA was very rapid (Fig. 6B), an additional set of control experiments were performed to determine whether the short period of surgical anoxia could account for the findings. P7 animals were anesthetized with hypothermia until respiration stopped, but no surgery was performed. Recordings were then made at P10–P11 to determine the possible influence of anoxia. TheEIPSC for neurons from anesthetized animals was −68 ± 3 mV (n = 6), and this value was not significantly different from neurons from nonanesthetized age-matched controls (df = 26; t = 1.769;p > 0.05).
Paired pulse facilitation in control and deafferented neurons
The presynaptic release characteristics were evaluated by delivering paired stimulus pulses and measuring the relative amplitude of the second IPSC. Two pulses of equal strength were delivered at interpulse intervals of 200, 100, 50, and 33 msec. This analysis was performed with stimuli that elicited a minimum IPSC amplitude (presumed to reflect the evoked response of one or a few inhibitory terminals). As shown in Figure 7A, control neurons showed an increase in the amplitude of the second LL-evoked IPSC compared with the first at all interpulse intervals tested, and this facilitation was small or absent in BCA neurons. Figure7B shows the mean values for each pulse interval. Significant differences between control and BCA neurons were observed at stimulus intervals of 10 (df = 29; t = −2.11;p < 0.043), 20 (df = 29; t = −3.81; p < 0.001), and 30 Hz (df = 27;t = −2.93; p = 0.007). Results were similar for paired pulses delivered to the CIC pathway (data not shown).
DISCUSSION
The major finding of this study was that deafferentation influenced the development of inhibitory synapses in IC neurons via a postsynaptic alteration of the chloride equilibrium potential and a change in presynaptic release mechanisms. The bilateral cochlea ablations were performed at P7, ∼4 d before the onset of sound-driven activity, and the synaptic effects were observed within 24 hr. Cell death can be detected in the gerbil cochlear nucleus at 2 d after ablation (Hashisaki and Rubel, 1989), although the process is not yet complete. Therefore, we suggest that changes in synaptic transmission were elicited by both the loss of spontaneous synaptic activity and the physical loss of afferents. In fact, spontaneous discharges have been recorded in the gerbil IC from P9 to P13 (Kotak and Sanes, 1995). Moreover, spontaneous activity in the IC is much lower in acutely ablated animals (Bock and Webster, 1974; Shepherd et al., 1999). The effects were similar, but not identical, for inhibitory afferents of the LL and CIC pathways. Although this could be attributable to the differential loss of afferents in the two pathways that is secondary to denervation, there is currently no information on this point. Deafferentation did not alter resting membrane potential or input resistance. There was a small, but significant, decrease in the membrane time constant, suggesting that BCA neurons may have atrophied, resulting in a smaller membrane capacitance. Because the slope of LL-evoked IPSC was actually faster in BCA neurons (see Results), the passive membrane properties did not apparently affect our measures of synaptic transmission.
Inhibitory synaptic currents in control IC neurons
Both CIC and the LL provide strong inhibitory and excitatory input to gerbil IC neurons, and such mixed input can be dissected pharmacologically in a brain slice preparation (Smith, 1992; Wagner, 1996; Lo et al., 1998; Moore et al., 1998). Because total PSCs are a result of complex interactions between excitatory and inhibitory components (Moore et al., 1998), we chose not to calculate the “excitatory” component by substracting total IPSCs from the compound PSCs. Therefore, the present study leaves open the question of whether there is BCA-induced modification of LL- and CIC-evoked excitatory synaptic currents. Furthermore, although much is known about the anatomy of ascending inhibitory projections to the IC, the present findings do not identify how each afferent source is affected by the manipulation (Adams, 1979; Nordeen et al., 1983; Coleman and Clerici, 1987; Shneiderman et al., 1988, 1993; Saint Marie and Baker, 1990;Saldaña and Merchan, 1992; Malmierca et al., 1995; Winer et al., 1995; González-Hernández et al., 1996; Merchan et al., 1997; Oliver et al., 1997; Saint Marie et al., 1997).
All neurons displayed LL- and CIC-evoked IPSCs (Fig. 1; Tables 1, 2), similar to our published results (Moore et al., 1998), with the exception that the recorded currents were somewhat larger in the present study. This may be attributed to the location of recording. In this study, recording sites were restricted to the most central region of the IC at a rostrocaudal level that included the largest number of lemniscal fibers entering the ventral IC. Our experience is that neurons in this location tend to have the largest evoked inhibitory currents. Both LL- and CIC-evoked IPSCs had comparable GABAergic and glycinergic components. Moore et al. (1998) showed that the LL pathway provided GABAergic and glycinergic input to IC neurons, as expected from the diverse innervation that IC neurons receive from auditory brainstem nuclei (Saint Marie and Baker, 1990; Shneiderman et al., 1993; González-Hernández et al., 1996; Saint Marie et al., 1997). It is possible that the SN-sensitive component of CIC-evoked IPSCs was an artifact, because immunocytochemical studies indicate that inhibitory CIC projections are GABAergic (González-Hernández et al., 1996), and glycinergic neurons are absent in adult IC (Winer et al., 1995). The close proximity of the CIC stimulating electrode to the border of IC could have directly activated glycinergic terminals within the IC. However, direct stimulation of terminals attributable to current spread would have produced a negligible latency between stimulus artifact and IPSC onset, and this was not observed (Fig. 1B). A more plausible explanation is the coexistence of both GABA- and glycine-mediated inhibition in immature CIC afferents. For example, inhibitory afferents in the developing LSO co-release GABA and glycine, but the GABAergic component declines with age (Kotak et al., 1998).
Control neurons also displayed a differential sensitivity to bicuculline (Fig. 2). Similar results have been reported previously in developing tissue (Liu et al., 1998). This observation suggests that GABAA receptor subunit expression in the developing IC may produce heteromeric receptors with different pharmacological characteristics (Laurie et al., 1992; Fritschy et al., 1994).
Inhibitory synaptic strength decreases after deafferentation
Bilateral cochlear ablation at P7 caused a significant decrease in the amplitude of evoked inhibitory synaptic currents in the IC (Fig. 1; Tables 1, 2). This effect was more pronounced in the LL pathway compared with the CIC pathway, possibly because of a decrease in the number of presynaptic afferents from lower brainstem nuclei. A decrease in the number of inhibitory LL afferents would be expected, because partial or complete cochlear dysfunction during early postnatal development can produce rapid cell death in the cochlear nucleus (Parks, 1979; Hashisaki and Rubel, 1989). It is not known whether there is compensatory sprouting after bilateral ablation, as occurs in unilaterally ablated animals (Moore and Kitzes, 1985; Kitzes et al., 1995; Russell and Moore, 1995).
Whether or not afferents were lost after deafferentation, this study demonstrated that the physiology of remaining inhibitory afferents was altered significantly. In particular, the reversal potential of evoked IPSCs was depolarized. Our initial whole-cell recordings showed a 14 mV depolarization for total IPSCs and for the pure GABAergic IPSCs (Fig.3). However, even control neurons displayed IPSC reversal potentials that were positive to that predicted by the intracellular and extracellular Cl− concentrations. One factor that may lead to a depolarizedEIPSC is the presence of cesium in the recording pipette, which may cause a reversal of the potassium chloride cotransporter (Kazaku et al., 1999). Therefore, an extensive set of recordings was made in control and ablated animals using gramicidin-perforated patch recordings (Rhee et al., 1994; Kyrozis and Reichling, 1995; Owens et al., 1996) with potassium chloride in the pipette solution.
In perforated patch recordings, the meanEIPSC from BCA neurons was depolarized by 24 mV. Furthermore, this effect was present within 1 d of bilateral ablation (Figs. 5, 6). A similar observation was made previously in the LSO, although the data were obtained with whole-cell recordings in current clamp (Kotak and Sanes, 1996). A depolarization of EIPSC would be expected to alter the IPSC driving force, and this would affect the IPSC amplitude recorded at different holding potentials. Thus, our failure to see significant changes on the CIC-evoked total and GABAergic IPSCs and LL-evoked GABAergic IPSCs at −80 mV is attributable to the fact that the depolarization of EIPSC increased the driving force when BCA neurons were held at −80 mV, enhancing the evoked IPSC amplitude.
The mechanisms responsible for EIPSCdepolarization are not yet understood; a growing body of literature indicates that the complement of chloride transporter proteins may underlie the depolarizing GABA- and glycine-evoked responses in immature neurons (Payne et al., 1996; Plotkin et al., 1997;Backus et al., 1998; Kazaku et al., 1999; Rivera et al., 1999; Williams et al., 1999). Furthermore, anoxia has been shown to depolarize the reversal potential of GABAergic IPSCs in the hippocampus (Katchman et al., 1994). Thus, we would speculate that the intracellular chloride accumulation induced by deafferentation is caused by an alteration of chloride extrusion mechanisms, such as the K+/Cl−cotransporter. Alternatively, the Na+/K+/Cl−(NKCC) cotransporter, which pumps chloride into the postsynaptic cell, may be overactive. It has been found that NMDA receptor activation can stimulate an NKCC cotransporter (Sun and Murali, 1998). Whether BCA causes GABAergic and glycinergic transmission to become excitatory (e.g., to evoke action potentials) remains to be determined.
There was also a large reduction of inhibitory strength that was apparently independent of driving force. The conductance of evoked IPSCs was reduced by ∼50%, and this was found for both the LL and CIC afferents (Fig. 4). This could have occurred as a result of loss of inhibitory afferents, reduction in the amount of GABA or glycine release, reduction of postsynaptic GABA or glycine receptors, or an alteration in the functional status of these receptors. In fact, we observed that deafferentation resulted in the apparent loss of a class of GABAA receptors that are less sensitive to BIC. In control neurons, there was often an evoked IPSC that remained in the presence of KYN, SN, and 10 μm BIC. However, this remaining IPSC was not observed in BCA neurons (Tables 1, 2). This may indicate an alteration in the expression of GABAAreceptor subunits, as shown in presbycusis (Milbrandt et al., 1997).
We also found that the inhibitory transmission is disrupted at presynaptic terminals after deafferentation. A paired stimulus protocol was used to evaluate the function of inhibitory terminals in control and BCA neurons (Fig. 7). Control neurons displayed an increase in the synaptic response to the second stimulus pulse at rates of 10, 20, and 30 Hz, and this potentiation was virtually absent in BCA neurons. This observation indicates that neurotransmitter production and/or release from the presynaptic terminal is disrupted by deafferentation. A decrease in GABA release in the inferior colliculus has been reported after BCA (Bledsoe et al., 1995) and in hippocampal neurons from animals treated with kainate or pilocarpine (Hirsch et al., 1999). There is also a decrease of GABA and glycine release in several nuclei with projections to IC after partial deafferentation (Suneja et al., 1998a).
Compensatory sprouting of afferent projections after the removal of one cochlea leads to novel projections in the gerbil superior olive and inferior colliculus (Moore and Kitzes, 1985; Kitzes et al., 1995;Russell and Moore, 1995). These changes extend to a molecular level. Sensory deprivation results in alterations of transmitter release (Bledsoe et al., 1995; Potashner et al., 1997; Suneja et al., 1998a) and changes in the distribution of excitatory and inhibitory receptors (Suneja et al., 1997, 1998b; Koch and Sanes, 1998). In vivoelectrophysiological studies confirm that these structural alterations produce novel sound-evoked coding properties (Kitzes and Semple, 1985;Bledsoe et al., 1995). The present results indicate that normal innervation, before sound-driven activity, regulates inhibitory transmission in IC neurons. Short-term deafferentation led to a decrease in LL- and CIC-evoked IPSC amplitude, partially because of a postsynaptic alteration in chloride equilibrium potential. In addition, deafferentation caused a decrease in the functionality of single inhibitory synaptic terminals. Therefore, alterations in auditory processing after deafness may be caused by physiological changes of inhibitory synaptic transmission.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant DC00540.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dan H. Sanes, Center for Neural Science, 4 Washington Place, New York University, New York, NY 10003. E-mail: sanes{at}cns.nyu.edu.