Abstract
In the avian auditory brainstem, nucleus magnocellularis (NM) functions to relay phase-locked signals to nucleus laminaris for binaural coincidence detection. Although many studies have revealed that NM neurons exhibit intrinsic physiological and anatomical specializations for this purpose, the role of inhibition has not been fully explored. The present study characterizes the organization of GABAergic feedback to NM. Anterograde and retrograde labeling methods showed that NM receives a prominent projection from the ipsilateral superior olivary nucleus (SON). The functional features of this projection were explored in a brain slice preparation. Stimulating fibers from the SON evoked long-lasting, depolarizing responses in NM neurons that were blockable by bicuculline, a GABAAreceptor antagonist. The slow time course of these responses allowed them to undergo temporal summation during repetitive stimulation. The summed GABAergic response was capable of blocking spikes generated in NM neurons by suprathreshold current injection. This inhibitory effect was attributable to a large reduction in input resistance caused by a combination of the opening of a GABAergic Cl−conductance and the recruitment of a low-voltage activated K+ conductance. This large reduction of input resistance increased the amount of current necessary to drive NM neurons to threshold. The results lead us to propose that GABAergic inhibition enhances phase-locking fidelity of NM neurons, which is essential to binaural coincidence detection in nucleus laminaris.
- GABA
- nucleus magnocellularis
- avian auditory brainstem
- phase locking
- superior olivary nucleus
- K+conductance
In the avian auditory brainstem, auditory nerve fibers bifurcate to innervate neurons of two spatially segregated cochlear nuclei: nucleus angularis (NA) and nucleus magnocellularis (NM) (Boord, 1969; Ramón y Cajal, 1971;Rubel and Parks, 1975; Parks and Rubel, 1978; Jhaveri and Morest, 1982a,b; Carr and Boudreau, 1991). NM, the avian homolog of the mammalian anteroventral cochlear nucleus, projects bilaterally to nucleus laminaris (NL). By preserving temporal characteristics of acoustic inputs, NM neurons provide the information necessary for coincidence detection of interaural time differences (Young and Rubel, 1983; Sullivan and Konishi, 1984; Carr and Konishi, 1988, 1990;Warchol and Dallos, 1990; Fujita and Konishi, 1991; Overholt et al., 1992; Joseph and Hyson, 1993).
Many anatomical and physiological features make NM neurons well suited for coding temporal information. First, excitatory transmission to NM cells is very secure. Each NM neuron receives two or three large calycine terminals, each of which can generate suprathreshold currents (Parks and Rubel, 1978; Hackett et al., 1982; Zhang and Trussell, 1994a,b). Second, excitatory input is not attenuated or electrotonically filtered because NM neurons have spherical somata and few if any dendritic processes (Parks 1981; Jhaveri & Morest, 1982a;Reyes et al., 1994; Zhang and Trussell, 1994a). Third, NM neurons express a complement of channels for rapid signaling. For instance, a robust low voltage-activated (LVA) K+conductance activates at membrane potentials just positive to rest. LVA K+ currents are rapid to activate and only slowly inactivate, allowing NM neurons to recover rapidly from excitation and preventing temporal summation of multiple inputs (Reyes et al., 1994; Koyano et al., 1996; Rathouz and Trussell, 1998). Furthermore, ionotropic glutamate receptors on NM neurons desensitize rapidly, keeping excitatory synaptic currents brief despite their large size (Raman and Trussell, 1992; Trussell et al., 1993; Raman et al., 1994).
NM neurons also receive inhibitory inputs. Electron microscopy reveals punctate endings containing round vesicles at symmetrical membrane specializations, characteristics of inhibitory terminals (Parks, 1981). Immunohistochemical studies have shown that these terminals are GABA-positive (Müller, 1987; Carr et al., 1989; Code et al., 1989, 1991; von Bartheld et al., 1989; Lachica et al., 1994). Furthermore, early in vitro studies on NM identified two types of evoked synaptic potentials: short latency, rapid EPSPs and longer latency and long duration depolarizing PSPs (Hackett et al., 1982). More recent in vitro studies have further confirmed the presence of GABA receptors on NM neurons; NM neurons respond to GABA directly by depolarization, and Cl−appears to be the ion mediating this response (Hyson et al., 1995; Lu et al., 1997). The depolarizing nature of GABAergic responses is presumably attributable to relatively high intracellular [Cl−] of NM neurons.
The origin of GABAergic input to NM neurons has only been briefly described. Besides the contribution of a small number of local GABAergic neurons, NM receives a large projection from the superior olivary nucleus (SON), which is mainly composed of GABAergic neurons (von Bartheld et al., 1989; Lachica et al., 1994; Westerberg and Schwarz, 1995). In addition to its NM projection, SON has reciprocal connections with NA and NL (Takahashi and Konishi, 1988;Carr et al., 1989; Carr and Boudreau, 1993; Lachica et al., 1994). Because of this connectivity and its GABAergic nature, it has been suggested that the SON functions as a “gain-control” system, especially under intense acoustic input (Lachica et al., 1994;Peña et al., 1996; Yang et al., 1999).
The present study addresses two issues: first, whether the connections between the SON and NA, NL, and NM are unilateral (ipsilateral) or bilateral; second, how the SON input affects the physiological properties of NM neurons. We demonstrated that the SON projects only to the ipsilateral NM, NA, and NL and that direct stimulation of SON input evokes a GABAergic inhibition that profoundly affected the firing properties of NM neurons. The results lead us to propose that by dampening the excitability of NM neurons, inhibition from the SON could improve their phase locking, which may be essential to binaural coincidence detection in the avian auditory brainstem.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experiments were conducted on White Leghorn chicken hatchlings (2- to 3-weeks-old) and late stage embryos (20- to 21-d-old). Animals were obtained from H & N Farms (Redmond, WA). Hatchlings were used to study the connectivity between the SON, NM, and other auditory nuclei in the brainstem. Embryos were used for in vitrophysiological investigation in brain slice preparations. Most of the techniques have been described previously (Hyson and Rubel, 1989;Overholt et al., 1992; Lachica et al., 1994; Reyes et al., 1994, 1996;Yang et al., 1999). All methods were approved by the University of Washington Animal Care Committee.
Surgical procedures and tracer injection. Thirty-five hatchling chickens were used to characterize the afferent and efferent connections of the SON. The animals were anesthetized by intraperitoneal injection of ketamine (8 mg/100 gm) and Nembutal (1.8 mg/100 gm). Supplemental doses were administrated at half the original dose every 60 min. The feathers on the head were trimmed short and then removed with a depilatory. The head was then placed on a stereotaxic apparatus through a beak holder. The skin and muscles overlying the skull were reflected, and a foundation layer of cyanoacrylate and dental cement was created on the surface of the skull. The foundation was then connected to a metal bar for the fixation of the head on the stereotaxic apparatus.
A reference point was marked along the midline of the skull. Subsequently, a 2-mm-diameter hole was drilled to expose the underlying cerebellum. A glass micropipette filled with 1 m NaCl was advanced by a micromanipulator through the cerebellum and into the auditory brainstem. During electrode penetration, 20 msec duration of pure tones of varying frequency and intensity were presented continuously. Because each auditory nucleus (namely, SON, NA, NL, and NM) had characteristic response properties (latency, discharge pattern, and relative location), they could be readily identified through multiple penetrations. After brief characterization of the nucleus, the micropipette was replaced with an injection electrode with a tip diameter of 20 μm, filled with 10% lysine-fixable dextran conjugated to tetramethylrhodamine (3 kDa, pH 7.4; Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR). The tracer was iontophoresed into the nucleus by passing 5 μA of on–off positive current for 20 min.
After a survival time of 24–72 hr, animals were deeply anesthetized with a lethal dose of Nembutal, then perfused transcardially with PBS (0.1 m, pH 7.4) followed by a fixative solution of 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. After fixation, the brain was dissected out, post-fixed for 2 hr, and then transferred to a 30% sucrose solution overnight for cryoprotection. The brain was sectioned on a freezing microtome at a thickness of 40 μm. Sections were mounted on slides, coverslipped with Dako (Carpinteria, CA) glycergel mounting medium, and examined under light microscope with epi-illumination through a rhodamine filter cube.
Brain slice preparation and whole-cell recording. Chicken embryos were rapidly decapitated. A 4 mm segment of the skull containing the brainstem was removed with a razor blade and quickly submerged in ice-cold artificial CSF (ACSF). ACSF contained (in mm): 130 NaCl, 26 NaH2CO3, 3 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 1.25 NaH2 PO4, and 10 dextrose. The brainstem segment was dissected out and transferred to a Vibratometissue slicer (Pelco, St. Louis, MO) where it was mounted on an agar block with cyanoacrylate glue and cut in ice-cold ACSF. Several 150 μm coronal slices containing the SON, NA, NL, and NM were collected and incubated in a holding chamber filled with ACSF at room temperature (22–23°C). ACSF was constantly gassed with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 and had a pH of 7.4.
Slices were transferred to a 0.5 cc volume recording chamber mounted on a Zeiss Axioskop FS with a 40× water-immersion objective and infrared, differential interference contrast optics (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) and superfused with ACSF at a rate of 3 ml/min. Using a multiple valve system, a slice was perfused with normal ACSF or ACSF containing 50 μm bicuculline methiodide (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) or ACSF with 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (DNQX; 100 μm) and d,l-2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate (AP-V; 50 μm) (Research Biochemicals, Natick, MA). All recordings were performed at room temperature (22–23°C).
Patch pipettes were drawn from 75 μl of hematocrit tubing (VWR Scientific, San Francisco, CA) using a two-stage electrode puller. The pipette tips were 2 μm in diameter and had open-tip resistances between 4 and 8 MΩ (DC). Resistance was compensated with the pipette submerged in the grounded bath using a standard “bridge balance” adjustment. Pipettes were filled with intracellular pipette solution that contained (in mm): 105 K-gluconate, 35 KCl, 5 EGTA, 10 K-HEPES, and 1 MgCl2. The pH of the solution was adjusted to 7.2 with KOH, and osmolality was measured between 280 and 290 mOsm. The junction potential for this intracellular pipette solution was 7 mV with reference to the grounded bath medium. All data are presented with correction for the junction potential.
Whole-cell voltage signals were recorded under current clamp using an Axoclamp 2B microelectrode amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA). Tight seals (>1GΩ) were established on the somata of visually identified NM neurons by applying slight negative pressure to the recording pipette on contact with the cell surface. The formation of gigaohm seals and the subsequent rupturing of the underlying membrane were monitored in voltage-clamp mode by measuring the resistive current resulting from a high-frequency, −5 mV pulse command. Stable recordings could be maintained for up to 1 hr. During recordings, we periodically monitored series resistance and capacitive currents to ensure good electrical access to the interior of the cell. Recordings were aborted if the membrane potential of a neuron depolarized to −50 mV or greater, and/or if a ruptured patch “resealed” and could not be ruptured again. Data were low-pass filtered at 10 kHz and digitized with an ITC-16 (Instrutech, Great Neck, NY) at 20 kHz for both on-line and off-line analysis. All recording protocols were written and run using the acquisition and analysis software Synapse, version 3.2 (Synergy, Bethesda, MD).
Concentric bipolar electrodes (Frederick Haer) with tip diameter of 0.5 mm were used for extracellular stimulation. The electrode was placed in the fiber tract dorsal to NM. Square electric pulses of 100 μsec duration were delivered through a stimulus isolation unit and interval generator (WPI 1830). The stimulus was either a single pulse or a train of pulses at an intensity of 10–90 V. Synaptic potentials were measured from a membrane potential of −67 ± 5 mV. Neurons with membrane potentials higher or lower were adjusted with DC current.
RESULTS
The goal of present study was to further understand the anatomical and functional properties of the GABAergic projections from the SON to NM and the other brainstem auditory nuclei. Fluorescent tracer was injected into the SON, NA, and NL for anterograde and retrograde labeling. The results showed that SON neurons project to the ipsilateral NM and to several other nuclei. To be inclusive, we present overall efferent and afferent connections of the SON with emphasis on its input to NM. We used a brain slice preparation to investigate the influence of the SON on NM. Stimulating SON fibers evoked long-lasting PSPs in NM neurons that were blockable by bicuculline, a potent antagonist of the GABAA receptor. We then describe a series of experiments aimed at evaluating the influence of the GABAergic input of SON on the membrane properties of NM neurons.
SON connectivity
Focal injection of rhodamine-conjugated to dextran (RCD) into the SON resulted in both retrograde and anterograde labeling in the ipsilateral NM, NL, and NA. The anterograde labeling represents the projecting terminals of the SON neurons, whereas retrograde labeling is the backfilling of cell bodies that innervate the SON. Figure1 shows the prominent anterograde labeling in the ipsilateral NM, NL, and NA (Fig.1A–C) and some retrograde labeling in the ipsilateral NA (Fig. 1C). For reasons described below, it was important to note that labeled SON fibers approached the ipsilateral NM from its dorsal margin. They branched extensively and formed numerous puncta that were distributed in patches or columns, a pattern similar to those of NM cell bodies (Fig.1A).
In contrast to a previous study (Lachica et al., 1994), we never observed anterogradely labeled terminals or puncta in the contralateral NA, NM, and NL. Nevertheless, we did observe a contralateral projection from the SON to the contralateral SON and the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus (LLV), a nucleus located immediately anterior to the SON (Fig. 1E,F). At the level of LLV and above, the contralateral SON projection converged with the terminal field of another fiber bundle; the ascending fibers from the contralateral NA and NL (Fig. 1F, pointed by the lower arrow). The labeling of this fiber bundle was attributable to retrograde filling of terminals from NA and NL innervating SON. These axons from NA and NL that terminate in the ipsilateral SON also have collaterals that continue across the midline and innervate the nucleus of trapezoid body (NTB) and LLV before projecting further to other parts of the lateral lemniscus and midbrain. However, as shown in Figure 1F and in the following experiments, these ascending fibers from NA and NL do not innervate the contralateral SON.
After RCD injection in the SON, there were always some retrogradely labeled cell bodies in ipsilateral NA and NL, but no labeled cells were observed in the ipsilateral NM. On average, more labeled cells were found in NA than in NL (Table 1). In all cases, there were some retrogradely labeled cell bodies in contralateral NA and NL, but they only constituted a small percentage of the total retrograde labeled cell bodies (<7%). We found that the location of injection site influenced the number of retrograde labeled cells in the contralateral NA and NL. In contrast to the cases when the injection sites were inside the SON, injections that were made slightly anterior or ventral to the SON produced a substantial increase of labeling in the contralateral NA and NL (Table 1). The areas anterior and ventral to the SON are occupied by the LLV and NTB, respectively. Both nuclei receive input from the contralateral NA and NL (Westerberg and Schwarz, 1995). Given the close proximity of the SON to NTB and LLV, it was not surprising that a deposit outside the SON or spillover of tracer from the injection site would yield varying numbers of retrograde labeled cells in the contralateral NA and NL.
That the SON did not receive significant inputs from the contralateral NA and NL was further demonstrated by injecting tracer directly into the NA and NL. Eight animals received such injections (four in NL and four in NA). The anterogradely labeled NL and NA fibers were seen sending branches to the ipsilateral SON, but the same fibers never reached the contralateral SON. Figure 2shows the retrograde and anterograde labeling in the ipsilateral (B) and contralateral (C) SON after a focal RCD injection in the ipsilateral NA. Note that no anterograde labeling is seen in the contralateral SON. In addition, no labeled cell bodies are seen in the contralateral SON. In these eight animals, 441 cells were retrogradely labeled in the ipsilateral SON, but no cells were labeled in the contralateral SON.
In summary, based on the anterograde and retrograde labeling data we conclude that the SON projects to five nuclei in the auditory brainstem, the ipsilateral NM, NL, and NA and the contralateral SON and LLV. It receives inputs from three nuclei, the ipsilateral NL, NA, and contralateral SON.
The reciprocal connections between the SON and ipsilateral NA and NL
The data presented above depicted reciprocal connections between the SON and ipsilateral NA and NL. As to the ipsilateral NM, it only received input from the SON but did not project back to the SON. This organization was clearly shown in another set of experiments. In these experiments, we mixed RCD solution with 1% kainic acid. This concentration of kainic acid has been shown to kill cell bodies but spare passing fibers or innervating terminals (Glenn and Kelly, 1992). Thus, because SON cells are killed in this experiment, we expected only retrograde labeling in NA and NL. After injecting a mixture of kainic acid and RCD into SON, we compared the labeling patterns with those seen with injection of RCD alone (Fig. 1, no kainic acid). As shown in Figure 3D, after the mixture was injected into the SON, no labeled SON cells were seen at the injection site as with injections of RCD alone (Fig.1D). In the ipsilateral NA and NL, there were many retrogradely labeled cell bodies (Fig. 3A,C) but no labeled terminal axons or puncta. In NM, there were neither retrogradely labeled cells nor the dense network of terminals and puncta, in contrast to injections of RCD alone (compare Figs. 3B,1A). The experiments also confirmed that the terminals and puncta seen in the injections of RCD alone were indeed from SON projection and not from axons passing through or near the SON.
Evoked synaptic responses in NM neurons
One distinguishing feature of NM neurons is outward rectification, as illustrated by Figure4A. That is, the voltage response to hyperpolarizing current injection is much greater than that to depolarizing current injection of the same magnitude. Outward rectification is evident in the voltage range near resting potential, and is attributable to a LVA K+conductance that is rapidly recruited. For this reason, the LVA conductance plays an important role in regulating the integration of synaptic inputs. (Reyes et al., 1994; Zhang and Trussell, 1994b; Koyano et al., 1996; Rathouz and Trussell, 1998). Suprathreshold currents never evoke more than one action potential, followed by a depolarized plateau, regardless of the current magnitude or amount of depolarization.
After briefly describing the various responses of NM we encountered when delivering electrical stimuli, we will focus on the responses of NM to stimulation of SON fibers and show how SON input can influence the firing of NM neurons. Synaptic responses of NM neurons were elicited by a stimulating electrode positioned just dorsal to NM. This site was chosen because SON fibers traverse this area before entering NM, and stimulation at this site was the most effective in evoking SON inhibition relative to other areas. However, in addition to SON fibers, this region also contains VIII nerve excitatory afferents and the axons of NM neurons (Young and Rubel, 1983). Therefore, several components could be evoked by stimulating this area, as shown in Figure4B–D. In some cells, stimulation evoked excitatory orthodromic synaptic responses that were attributable to activation of VIII nerve afferent fibers (Fig. 4B). The response could be an action potential or EPSP (asterisk) and typically had a 0.5–1 msec latency after stimulation (arrowindicates shock artifact). Other cells showed antidromic action potentials that were attributable to activation of NM neurons' own axons. These were distinguished from orthodromic responses in that EPSPs were never evoked by the stimulus, and spikes occurred with no measurable latency. Commonly, the leading edge of the antidromic action potentials merged with the shock artifact (Fig. 4C). In other cases, electric stimulation produced complex responses composed of two orthodromic components. As shown in Figure 4D, suprathreshold responses were accompanied by a slow depolarization that temporally summed with repetitive stimulation. During this train, orthodromically evoked spikes became progressively smaller in amplitude until ultimately failing, leaving only an EPSP response to the fourth stimulus.
By adjusting the stimulus amplitude or application of the glutamate receptor antagonists DNQX and AP-V, the slow orthodromic component could be isolated from excitatory responses. Figure5, A and B,illustrates the characteristics of slow PSPs in two NM neurons. In each neuron, responses to four successive shocks were recorded and superimposed. At a constant stimulus level, the amplitude and duration of PSPs fluctuated, possibly because of the variable number of fibers activated with each stimulus. For instance, the neuron shown in Figure5A responded to 30 V shocks with PSPs between 3 and 6 mV in amplitude and 40–50 msec in duration. One stimulus failed to elicit any response at all. Assuming that these responses were mediated by GABAA receptors, depolarizing responses were expected, because the reversal potential for Cl− under our recording conditions was −34 mV. In the neuron shown in Figure 5B, a stimulus level of 50 V evoked PSPs with a range of amplitudes and a more rapid decay. The decay of PSPs was quantified in recordings from eight neurons by measuring the time required for PSPs to decay to 50% of their peak amplitude. Figure 5C shows that these decay values varied between 2 and 25 msec, but were mostly in the range between 5 and 20 msec. For comparison, the decay of EPSPs in five different neurons is plotted on the same graph; note the comparatively large amplitude and rapid decay of these responses. The variation in PSP decay we observed was at least partly attributable to variability in the input resistance among neurons, either because of size of the cells or the amount and activation of LVA currents present.
Close examination of the recordings revealed that larger PSPs did not decay smoothly; there was an initial rapid phase of repolarization followed by a slow phase. The initial rapid phase was absent in smaller PSPs, as shown in Figure 5A. The rapid phase of repolarization is shown in another neuron in Figure 5D, in which a downward inflection follows the peak of the PSP (arrow). Because this phenomenon occurred only with larger depolarizations, it was likely that LVA outward currents were involved in hastening the repolarization, because these currents activate near resting potential (Reyes et al., 1994; Koyano et al., 1996; Rathouz and Trussell, 1998). If this were the case, then hyperpolarizing the membrane potential would prevent the activation of LVA channels during the PSP, and thus its decay should occur smoothly. As shown by the darker trace in Figure 5E, a PSP evoked from a membrane potential held at −93 mV indeed showed a smoother decay without the rapid component. Previous studies have established that the onset of LVA activation in NM neurons can be observed in responses to subthreshold, depolarizing current injection. These responses are characterized by an initial depolarized voltage peak that is followed by a less depolarized plateau, reflecting the initial “passive” depolarization before the rapid activation of LVA outward currents (Zhang and Trussell, 1994b). In Figure 5F, we superimposed the waveform of a PSP on a voltage response of approximately the same amplitude evoked by direct current injection in the same neuron. Note the similarity in the timing of the onset of hyperpolarization in both waveforms. In summary, these experiments provide evidence that under our recording conditions, individual PSPs evoked by stimulating SON inputs activate some LVA K+ channels in addition to GABAergic channels.
These PSPs could undergo temporal summation evoked by trains of stimuli. A 100 Hz train of 10 stimuli at 60 V was delivered to another neuron. Figure 5G shows that slow PSPs summed to produce a long-lasting depolarization with an amplitude of ∼15 mV and a duration that outlasted the stimulus train by >100 msec. Note change in time scale. For six neurons stimulated at 100 Hz for 100 msec, summation led to a maximum depolarization of 11.9 mV (± 3.4) (mean ±SE), and time-to-peak depolarization occurred at 70 msec (±13.6) relative to the onset of the train. The decay of the depolarized plateau was more variable: the mean time to decay by 50% of the peak depolarization was 89.5 msec (±40.6). This variability in decay reflects our observation that in some neurons, stimulation with trains of shocks led to a temporary increase in the frequency of spontaneous PSPs (e.g., PSPs indicated by an asterisk in Fig. 5G), which has been observed by others (Lu and Trussell, 2000) and is attributed to Ca2+ accumulation in GABAergic terminals.
Slow synaptic responses are GABAergic
Slow synaptic responses were blocked by bicuculline, a potent antagonist of GABAA receptors (n= 8 cells). As shown in Figure 6, in response to a 100 Hz train of pulses at 70 V, the NM neuron responded with slow PSPs that summed and lasted beyond the duration of train pulses (Fig. 6A). Addition of DNQX (40 μm) and APV (100 μm), blockers of glutamate receptor-mediated EPSPs, to the bath media did not affect the amplitude or duration of the response (Fig.6B). When bicuculline was added, the response was completely abolished (Fig. 6C), but recovered after 5–10 min of washout (Fig. 6D). Together, these data demonstrated that slow PSPs evoked in NM were attributable to direct activation of GABAergic fibers but not the glutamatergic inputs from VIII nerve fibers.
GABAergic input affects firing of NM neurons
Stimulating GABAergic inputs blocked action potentials in NM neurons (n = 6). This effect was demonstrated by activating the GABAergic fibers while evoking action potentials in single NM neurons by intracellular current injection. This paradigm is shown for one cell in Figure7A. Under control conditions, a train of 3 msec, 1.0 nA current pulses was injected into a NM neuron at a 20 msec interval. When the neuron was at rest, current injections reliably evoked action potentials, as illustrated by the first two spikes in Figure 7A. After the second spike, GABAergic fibers were activated by a train of 10 shocks at 100 Hz, and PSPs (e.g., arrow) summed to produce a slow depolarization of the baseline membrane potential. During the depolarization, spikes were inhibited, revealing the underlying electrotonic responses to each current pulse injected (Fig. 7A, arrowhead). The onset of inhibition and its time course was similar to the envelope of the summed synaptic response.
SON input activated a GABAergic conductance, and the resulting depolarization also recruited LVA K+channels. The activation of LVA K+channels alone could have been inhibitory by shunting injected current. To test whether opening of LVA K+ channels was sufficient to inhibit spikes, we directly depolarized the neuron with injected current to the same membrane potential reached during stimulation of GABAergic fibers (n = 5; Fig.7B). Direct depolarization to the same level as that during GABAergic activation did not inhibit action potentials, indicating that opening of LVA channels alone was insufficient to account for the inhibitory effects seen.
The inhibition of action potentials could be overcome by increasing excitatory drive (Fig. 7C,D). In another neuron, spikes evoked with current pulses of 1.0 nA and 2 msec duration could be inhibited by stimulating SON inputs (Fig. 7C). By increasing the injected current amplitude to 1.5 nA, spikes could be recovered during stimulation of the GABAergic fibers (Fig. 7D).
The inhibition of the firing of NM neurons was most likely the consequence of shunting, attributable in part to the opening of GABAergic Cl− channels and in part to opening of LVA channels. Consequently, the same amount of excitation (current injection in this case) would not produce sufficient depolarization for the cell to reach threshold. To evaluate the relative contributions of GABAergic channels and LVA channels to the shunting inhibition, we measured input resistance, under three conditions: at rest, during SON stimulation, and during membrane depolarization (n = 5). Voltage responses to low-amplitude (−50 to −150 pA), 50 msec hyperpolarizing pulses of current were recorded, and 10 responses were averaged to give a mean voltage response to a given current amplitude. Input resistance (Rin) was calculated asV–I. An example case is illustrated in the left panel of Figure 8, where the NM neuron had an input resistance of 67 MΩ at resting potential, which in all cases was adjusted to −67 (±5 mV). Under this condition, we assumed that GABAergic and LVA conductances were minimal. Although GABAergic inputs were stimulated (Fig. 8, middle panel),Rin was reduced to 31 MΩ, and this change was probably attributable to opening of GABAergic channels and LVA K+ channels. To estimate the contribution of the LVA conductance to shunting,Rin was measured in the same neurons while they were directly depolarized from resting potential with a baseline of positive current (Fig. 8, right panel). As expected, direct depolarization of the NM neuron also reduced the input resistance, but not to the same extent that it was reduced during GABAergic input. For this cell, depolarization alone reducedRin to 48 MΩ. For five NM neurons, stimulating GABAergic fibers reducedRin by an average of 61% from a mean (± SD) of 75 (± 38) MΩ to a mean (± SD) of 28 (± 20) MΩ. In the same neurons, depolarization alone reducedRin by an average of 41%, indicating that approximately two-thirds (65%) of the change inRin during stimulation of SON input is attributable to LVA channels. In summary, GABAergic input evoked a shunting inhibition in NM neurons that was partly attributable to GABA-activated channels and partly to low-threshold voltage activated K+ channels.
DISCUSSION
The present study addresses the functional significance of the SON in the avian auditory brainstem, and more generally, the role of inhibition in auditory processing. We show that the SON projects to the ipsilateral NM, NA, and NL as well as SON and LLV on the contralateral side. In return, the SON receives inputs from the ipsilateral NA and NL and contralateral SON. We further demonstrate that the GABAergic projection from the SON to NM can be blocked by bicuculline, a potent antagonist for GABAA receptors and that SON-mediated GABAergic inhibition decreases membrane input resistance through a shunting effect. The mechanism is unique in that it involves the cooperative action between GABAergic Cl− and LVA K+ channels. This reduced input resistance makes NM neurons less excitable, and, as discussed below, may lead to the enhancement of phase locking by NM neurons. One possible problem in our comparisons of anatomy in hatchlings to physiology in embryos is that at least one aspect of the GABAergic innervation of NM, terminal density, is still maturing in the first 2 weeks after hatching (Code et al., 1989). However, intrinsic properties of NM neurons are similar in hatchlings and late embryos (Reyes et al., 1994; Zhang and Trussell, 1994b).
SON feedback is primarily ipsilateral
The anatomical aspects of our study were intended to both confirm previous findings and clarify one issue that is inconsistent in previous studies: whether or not there are reciprocal connections between the SON and the contralateral NA and NL. Several studies describe a contralateral input to the SON from NA and NL, but the reported strength of this projection varies greatly (Lachica et al., 1994; Westerberg and Schwarz, 1995). Whereas the discrepancy may be in part because of the different tracers used, our data and those ofWesterberg and Schwarz (1995) suggest that most of the variability may be accounted for by the size of the tracer deposit. The SON is closely apposed to two other nuclei, LLV and NTB, which both receive input from the contralateral NA and NL (Takahashi and Konishi, 1988; Westerberg & Schwarz, 1995). Consequently, any spillover to LLV and NTB from a large injection would produce retrograde labeling in the contralateral NA and NL. Lachica et al. (1994) did not distinguish among the SON and these neighboring nuclei, and the injection sites in that study were usually large enough to extend beyond the borders of SON.
SON may act to equalize the activity in the ipsilateral and contralateral NM
The unilateral connectivity between SON and NM, NA, and NL that we observed has significant implications for the role that this circuitry plays in processing binaural information. Previous studies have demonstrated that SON neurons fire at rates that are graded with the amount of excitatory drive (Moiseff and Konishi, 1983; Yang et al., 1999) and are anatomically and physiologically suited for spatial and temporal summation. These findings are consistent with a gain control mechanism proposed previously (Lachica et al., 1994; Peña et al., 1996). The hypothesis is that without inhibition, high sound levels would strongly drive each NM, and the high firing rate from either side might lead to “false alarms” in NL neurons. That is, coincident firing of many NM fibers from either side alone can sum and evoke action potentials in NL neurons, deteriorating the ability of NL to discriminate binaural coincidences from strong unilateral excitation. SON potentially can prevent strong monaural excitation because inhibition from SON is proportional to the ipsilateral sound level and thus would reduce the activity of NM correspondingly. The unilateral projection from SON to NM allows separate control of NM activity on each side of the brain. Thus, the side receiving the strongest excitatory drive also receives proportionally greater inhibition; any large disparity in NM firing between the two sides will be reduced. Furthermore, the inhibition to each side is not entirely independent because of the reciprocal innervation between the two SONs, making SON activity dependent on the ipsilateral sound level and the degree of inhibition provided by the opposite SON. In this way, similar firing rates in ipsilateral and contralateral NM could be maintained at a level appropriate for coincidence detection, even though sounds located off the midline generate interaural intensity disparities as well as timing disparities (Hyson et al., 1994).
Functional implications for inhibition of NM neurons
Inhibition from SON may also influence phase locking in NM neurons via two different mechanisms. The first mechanism is through the activation of GABAB receptors. Brenowitz et al. (1998) showed that GABAB-mediated attenuation of transmitter release prevents depression, enabling relatively uniform EPSC amplitudes at high stimulus frequencies. During trains of stimuli, the regularity of EPSC amplitude conserves phase information by making the relative timing between presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes more uniform; i.e., spike threshold is crossed at the same time with each EPSC.
The second mechanism is through postsynaptic inhibition of the NM cell directly via activation of GABAA receptors, which should actually enhance the phase locking precision of NM neurons relative to their auditory nerve inputs. The postsynaptic GABAergic inhibition causes a substantial decrease in input resistance, thereby shunting inward currents and preventing NM cells from reaching threshold with currents that are suprathreshold in the absence of inhibition. The hypothesis we now advance is that with SON inhibition, currents generated by one afferent terminal are subthreshold, and only by coincident input from two or more afferents are excitatory currents suprathreshold. It is noteworthy that in our study, stimulating SON input blocked action potentials in NM that were evoked with current pulses of 1.0–1.5 nA. These current amplitudes are comparable to the amplitudes of EPSCs evoked by stimulation of single afferent fibers of NM (Brenowitz et al., 1998). The requirement of coincident subthreshold inputs improves phase locking by producing a tighter registration of discharges at a particular phase of the signal than would occur if each input was suprathreshold, an idea that has been proposed previously byCarney (1992), Rothman et al. (1993), Joris et al. (1994), and Rothman and Young (1996) to account for the improved phase locking of bushy cells in the cochlear nucleus of mammals over that of auditory nerve fibers. Comparisons of phase locking by NM neurons and auditory nerve fibers in the barn owl have reached conflicting conclusions on whether there is a similar enhancement of synchrony in avians (Sullivan and Konishi, 1984; Köppl, 1997).
Why is SON-evoked GABAergic inhibition depolarizing?
Depolarizing responses to GABA, although common in developing nervous systems, are rarely observed in the CNS of mature animals. In the present study, we obtained depolarizing GABAergic responses because our recording conditions gave rise to a ECl− of −34 mV; opening of Cl− channels allows Cl− efflux, depolarizing the cells. Nevertheless, depolarization by SON input probably reflects the normal polarity of GABAergic responses for neurons in NM, as depolarizing responses have been observed in 2-week-old hatchlings using methods that minimally disturbed intracellular [ Cl−] (Hyson et al., 1995).
The important consideration here is that the functional advantage that may result from depolarizing inhibition would not occur with the hyperpolarizing inhibition found more commonly in other systems. As has been suggested previously (Hyson et al., 1995), one advantage of having a depolarizing inhibition is to allow the recruitment of additional conductances that are activated slightly above resting potential. GABA-activated Cl− channels appear to act synergistically with LVA channels in NM neurons; Cl− efflux through GABAergic channels depolarizes the neuron and thereby recruits LVA channels. Opening of LVA channels contributed approximately two-thirds of the change in input resistance we measured when SON fibers were stimulated. Thus, depolarizing inhibition is particularly effective because of the recruitment of LVA K+ channels. Indeed, one may argue that this strategy is more effective for shunting inward currents than a GABAergic response that is hyperpolarizing or has no effect on membrane potential. This appears to give the SON greater leverage in adjusting excitability and perhaps the temporal characteristics of NM neurons as well.
Footnotes
This work was sponsored by National Institutes of Health Grant 00395, a National Institutes of Health Neuroscience Training Grant 5T 32 GM 07108, and Fellowship DC00312. We are grateful to Drs. W. Lippe, G. Pollak, and W. Spain for critically reading and commenting on early versions of this manuscript and to M. Mazurek for many valuable discussions.
Correspondence should be addressed to Edwin W Rubel, Virginia Merrill Bloedel Hearing Research Center and Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, University of Washington, Box 357923, Seattle, WA 98195. E-mail: rubel{at}u.washington.edu.