Abstract
Cerebellar nucleus neurons were recorded in vitro,and dynamic clamping was used to simulate inhibitory synaptic input from Purkinje cells likely to occur in vivo. Inhibitory input patterns with varying synaptic amplitudes and synchronicity were applied to determine how spike rate and spike timing can be controlled by inhibition. The excitatory input conductance was held constant to isolate the effect of dynamic inhibitory inputs on spiking. We found that the timing of individual spikes was controlled precisely by short decreases in the inhibitory conductance that were the consequence of synchronization between many inputs. The spike rate of nucleus neurons was controlled in a linear way by the rate of inhibitory inputs. The spike rate, however, also depended strongly on the amount of synchronicity present in the inhibitory inputs. An irregular spike train similar to in vivo data resulted from applied synaptic conductances when the conductance was large enough to overcome intrinsic pacemaker currents. In this situation subthreshold fluctuations in membrane potential closely followed the time course of the combined reversal potential of excitation and inhibition. This indicates that the net synaptic driving force for realistic input levels in vivo may be small and that synaptic input may operate primarily by shunting. The accurate temporal control of output spiking by inhibitory input that can be achieved in this way in the deep cerebellar nuclei may be particularly important to allow fine temporal control of movement via inhibitory output from cerebellar cortex.
The final output of the cerebellum is generated by the neurons of the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN). A major source of control over DCN neurons is derived from cerebellar cortex via GABAA-type inhibitory Purkinje cell input (Billard et al., 1993). Purkinje cell synapses provide 73% of the total synapses to DCN neurons, and almost all somatic synapses of DCN neurons are inhibitory (Palkovits et al., 1977; De Zeeuw and Berrebi, 1995). This arrangement leads to the question of how inhibitory input can control spiking in postsynaptic neurons accurately. The required accuracy of this control appears to be quite high, because the function of the cerebellum has been related to precise temporal aspects of movement control (Braitenberg, 1967; Ivry and Keele, 1989; Diener et al., 1993; Braitenberg et al., 1997; Wang et al., 1998). For example, a recent study by Timmann et al. (1999) showed that throwing a ball accurately required a temporal precision of the coordination between arm movement and finger opening of ∼10 msec. Cerebellar patients were not able to throw accurately because the temporal precision was reduced. In the present study we investigated how the Purkinje cell projection onto the DCN might support such temporally precise control apparent from the behavioral level. Although the question of precise temporal coding of neural activity via excitatory input has been discussed extensively (Shadlen and Newsome, 1994, 1998; Softky, 1995; König et al., 1996), the possibility of such coding via inhibitory input has not been addressed to our knowledge.
To study how inhibitory Purkinje cell input might control DCN spiking precisely, we used the technique of dynamic current clamping (Robinson and Kawai, 1993; Sharp et al., 1993). This method allowed us to record DCN neurons in vitro and to apply a conductance waveform that simulates the synaptic input of several hundred GABAA-type input elements. This technique is distinctly different from directly injecting current waveforms because the current flowing through an applied synaptic conductance is dependent on the postsynaptic membrane potential. Furthermore, synaptic input conductances can change the temporal integration properties of neurons by reducing their input resistance and thereby their membrane time constant (Holmes and Woody, 1989; Rapp et al., 1992; Häusser and Clark, 1997). By applying simulated synaptic conductances to DCN neurons, we find that the rate and temporal precision of DCN spiking can be controlled by specific aspects of inhibitory input patterns.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Tissue preparation and electrophysiology. All animal procedures fully complied with the National Institutes of Health guidelines on animal care and use. Whole-cell recordings were obtained at 32°C with an Axoclamp-2B amplifier from deep cerebellar nuclei neurons in 300 μm sagittal cerebellar slices from 14- to 17-d-old male Sprague Dawley rats. The bridge was balanced and the voltage offset was zeroed before each recording. The slice medium contained (in mm): NaCl 124, KCl 3, KH2PO4 1.2, NaHCO3 26, CaCl2 2, MgSO4 1.9, and glucose 20. Electrodes were filled with the following (in mm): K-gluconate 140, HEPES 10, NaCl 10, EGTA 0.2, MgATP 4, NaGTP 0.4, spermine 0.05, glutathione 5, and 1% biocytin. Electrode resistance ranged between 6 and 12 MΩ. Excitatory and inhibitory synapses were blocked with 100 μm AP-5, 10 μm CNQX, and 40 μm picrotoxin. Cells had to show a spike amplitude of at least 50 mV during spontaneous activity and a drift of <5 mV in the mean membrane potential during the recording to be kept for data analysis. We did not subtract a junction potential from the recorded membrane potential. On theoretical grounds a junction potential of ∼10 mV can be expected for recording solutions made with K-gluconate (Barry, 1994). This junction potential, however, can be primarily offset by a Donnan potential of opposite sign (Barry and Lynch, 1991). Nevertheless, we cannot exclude some error in the absolute value of the recorded membrane potential. The results of the present study would not be affected by a small offset in the absolute membrane potential except for a matching shift in the described value of the combined synaptic reversal potential required to yield realistic spike rates.
Morphological characteristics of recorded cells. Cell bodies were visualized in the slice with a Zeiss 63× water immersion objective, and the image was captured on camera (Hamamatsu C2400) mounted on a 4× phototube and displayed on a monitor. The size of cell somata was clearly visible, and recordings were limited to large cells. Cells that appeared swollen, however, were also rejected. Recorded cells were labeled with biocytin, and the soma length and width of 15 out of 33 recorded neurons could be recovered after standard histological procedures. The length of the somata ranged from 20 to 34 μm (26.1 ± 5.1μm, mean ± SD), and their width ranged from 12 to 23 μm (16.3 ± 3.1 μm, mean ± SD). GABAergic DCN neurons in rats have soma sizes between 5 and 22 μm (mean, 10 μm), and glutamatergic DCN neurons have soma sizes between 10 and 35 μm (mean, 20 μm). These distributions are almost identical for all three DCN (Batini et al., 1992). Because the DCN neurons in our experiments had soma sizes >20 μm, it is very likely that we recorded mostly from glutamatergic DCN neurons that project to the red nucleus or thalamus. A small overlap with the largest of the inhibitory neurons projecting to the inferior olive, however, cannot be excluded. Dendritic arborizations were large and sparsely branched for all reconstructed cells without any indication of two distinct types. No physiological differences were observed between the smallest and the largest cells in our sample, and the data were pooled.
Simulated synaptic input. Dynamic clamping was used to inject a current (Iinj) at the cell soma that mimics the synaptic current of several hundred input neurons. The injected current was calculated during the recordings on the basis of the following equation: Iinj = (Eex − Vm) *Gex + (Ein − Vm) *Gin. The reversal potentials of the excitatory (Eex) and the inhibitory (Ein) synaptic currents were set to 0 and −70 mV, respectively. The recorded membrane potential (Vm) and the injected current were updated with a frequency of 10 kHz. The excitatory and inhibitory conductances, denoted Gex andGin, were calculated before the experiments. The excitatory conductance was always held at a constant value to isolate the effect of inhibitory input patterns on output spiking. A single inhibitory input element was simulated as a dual-exponential function with a rise time constant (τ1) of 0.93 msec and a decay time constant (τ2) of 13.6 msec:gin =gmax / (τ2 − τ1) * (e−(t/τ2) − e−(t/τ1)). These time constants replicated experimental data of Anchisi et al. (1998). Our own measurements revealed almost identical values with a rise time constant of 1 msec and a decay time constant of 13 msec at a holding potential of −40 mV (data not shown). The parametergmax has not been determined experimentally yet, and we used different gain factors to explore a realistic range of input conductance amplitudes. Agmax of 4.3 pS corresponds to a gain factor of 1 in our experiments. The applied inhibitory conductance trace (Gin) was the sum of all inhibitory input conductances over time. It has been estimated that on average 860 Purkinje cells converge onto each nucleus neuron (Palkovits et al., 1977). Purkinje cell synapses are located to 12% (103) on the soma and to 37% (318) on the proximal dendrites of nucleus neurons (De Zeeuw and Berrebi, 1995). For our dynamic-clamp stimuli, we chose 400 inhibitory input elements because this number approximates the sum of the Purkinje cells that contact each nucleus neuron electrotonically close to its spike initiation zone. Purkinje cells in vivoshow an ongoing activity with mean frequencies of 35 Hz (Savio and Tempia, 1985; Stratton et al., 1988), which we used as the mean frequency of our input elements. The interspike intervals of the input elements were generated randomly (exponential distribution). The resulting amplitude of Gin had a mean of 1 nS at an input gain of 1. The amplitude ofGex was set so that the mean value of the combined excitatory and inhibitory synaptic reversal potential (Vsyn) was close to −40 mV: Vsyn = (Eex *Gex +Ein *Gin) / (Gex +Gin). This value was required to result in realistic output spike frequencies. Using the equation describing Vsyn, we can express the injected current as: Iinj = (Gex +Gin) * (Vsyn − Vm), showing thatIinj is proportional to the total input conductance and the deviation of the synaptic reversal potential from the membrane potential. For technical details of the dynamic current-clamp experiments, see Jaeger and Bower (1999).
Analysis of the spike-time precision. Each simulated synaptic input pattern lasted 5 sec and was applied repeatedly (two to eight times) with pauses of 5 sec in between. The cross-correlogram was calculated from the spike times of successive spike trains with a bin width of 1 msec. The spike-timing precision was defined as the percentage of the spikes that did fall into time windows of ±1 and ±5 msec in the cross-correlogram. The chance level was subtracted from these values. To calculate the chance level, we randomly redistributed (shuffled) the interspike intervals and calculated the cross-correlogram from the shuffled spike trains. The subtracted chance level depended on the spike frequency. For example, at a mean spike rate of 20 Hz and a precision criterion of ±5 msec, the subtracted chance level was 20%, and therefore the maximal proportion of precisely timed spikes was limited to 80%.
Spike-triggered averaging and reconstruction of input conductance. Spike-triggered averages (STAs) of normalized input frequency and input conductance were computed for a duration of ±100 msec around the peak of action potentials. To obtain STAs of the input frequency, we convolved each input spike with a 1 msec Gaussian of a peak amplitude of 1.0. This operation leads to an analog trace of the instantaneous spike rate (Paulin, 1996). The STA was computed after normalizing the traces to a mean of 1.0. To reconstruct the estimate of the input conductance on the basis of its STA, we replaced each output spike from all presentations of the same stimulus with its STA and added up the STAs. The resulting trace was divided by the number of stimulus repetitions.
RESULTS
Spontaneous activity in current clamp
All recorded DCN neurons were spontaneously active when synaptic input was blocked, and no bias current was injected. Their mean spontaneous spike frequency was 7.7 ± 4.2 Hz (n = 33). The corresponding mean value of the subthreshold membrane potential was −49.4 ± 3.0 mV. The voltage from 3 msec before to 3 msec after the peak of each action potential was removed to obtain this value. Most of the recorded neurons were regular spiking (82%), whereas the remaining neurons showed some tendency of spontaneous bursting. These characteristics were similar to those seen previously (Jahnsen, 1986a; Aizenman et al., 1998). No differences between regular spiking and bursting neurons were observed in the dynamic-clamp experiments, and data from both groups were pooled.
Inhibitory synaptic input can control the spike timing of DCN neurons precisely
We used the activity of 400 simulated presynaptic Purkinje cell elements to construct an inhibitory conductance trace (Fig.1A,Gin). This conductance trace presents the sum of all unitary postsynaptic conductance changes caused by individual presynaptic elements. The excitatory input conductance was held constant throughout this study to examine the effects of inhibitory input patterns in isolation (Fig. 1A,Gex). The amplitude of the constant excitatory conductance was chosen so that the combined synaptic reversal potential of inhibition and excitation (Vsyn) had a mean value of −40 mV (Fig.1B, gray trace). Because the reversal potential of the inhibitory conductance (Ein) was −70 mV and that of the excitatory conductance (Eex) was 0 mV, the amplitude of the mean excitatory conductance needed to be 75% that of the mean inhibitory conductance. Vsyn can be computed from the inhibitory and excitatory conductances as: Vsyn = (Eex *Gex +Ein *Gin) / (Gex +Gin). The membrane potential trajectory (Fig. 1B, black trace) followed fluctuations in Vsyn quite well. These fluctuations reflect the changing amplitude of the inhibitory conductance. During depolarizing membrane potential fluctuations, action potentials could be generated. There was not a fixed voltage threshold, however. In particular, spikes that followed a preceding spike at a short interval showed a higher threshold (Fig.1B, asterisk). This effect is likely caused by a remaining sodium channel inactivation after the preceding spike.
Changes in membrane potential attributable to synaptic input are the consequence of the current injected across the synaptic conductance. The general equation for injected current (Iinj) via a mix of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances is:Iinj = (Gex +Gin) * (Vsyn − Vm). It is very important to notice thatIinj is exactly zero when the membrane potential equals Vsyn. For any deviation of Vm from Vsyn a synaptic current is injected that drives Vm back toward Vsyn. The larger the total synaptic conductance (Gex +Gin), the larger is the synaptic current that stabilizes Vm at Vsyn. Thus Vsyn can be seen as the command voltage of a partial voltage clamp operating at a gain proportional to Gex +Gin. The action of mixed excitatory and inhibitory conductances as a partial voltage clamp is in principle a generalization of the classical concept of shunting. The term shunting is used to describe the case in which an open inhibitory conductance stabilizes Vm near the inhibitory reversal potential. We ascribe the term partial voltage clamp to the situation in which a mix of open excitatory and inhibitory conductances stabilizes Vm near the combined reversal potential. Figure 1, B and C, shows that Vm stayed very close to Vsyn except during an action potential. Therefore the synaptic input operated quite effectively as a voltage clamp. The injected synaptic current was on average a small depolarizing current, indicating that the intrinsic currents of the neuron had a small outward bias at −40 mV, which the synaptic clamp current counteracted. During an action potential the neuron depolarized rapidly, and the clamp current showed a hyperpolarizing current peak that counteracted the intrinsic inward current.
All recorded DCN neurons (n = 33) showed a high reliability in the response to repetitions of the same stimulus (Fig.1D). This finding indicates that inhibitory input patterns can control output spiking precisely. The specific stimulus conditions that control spike timing and spike frequency are examined in the following sections.
Spike-timing precision increased with input gain and input correlation
An important mechanism by which cerebellar cortex may convey information to the DCN lies in the degree of synchronicity in the activity of many Purkinje cells converging onto DCN neurons. Climbing-fiber inputs for example may lead to highly synchronous activation of a population of Purkinje cells (Welsh et al., 1995; Lang et al., 1999). We tested the effects of input synchronicity on DCN spiking by using three different synchronization levels. At the highest level of input synchronization, our 400 presynaptic elements were divided into 10 groups of 40 synchronously activated elements. The activity between groups was uncorrelated. An intermediate level of input synchronization consisted of 100 groups of 4 synchronized elements. In the condition without input synchronization, all 400 input elements were activated independently.
A second parameter that we manipulated was the total amplitude ofGin plusGex by multiplying bothGin andGex with the same gain factor. This manipulation does not change the time course of Vsyn, but the amount of synaptic current for a given offset between Vsyn and Vm is proportional to this gain factor. The use of a range of different synaptic strengths was important because the precise value of this parameter in vivo has not yet been determined.
We found that both the level of input synchronicity and the level of synaptic strength (input gain) had important consequences for the control of DCN spiking by inhibitory input (Fig.2). The accuracy of the temporal alignment of spikes with the stimulus was increased by a rise of the input gain as well as by a rise of the input synchronization (Fig.2A,B). The lowest input gain used was ineffective in controlling spike timing as demonstrated by the absence of spike alignment in the spike rasters and the absence of a central peak in the cross-correlation of spike trains between subsequent stimulus presentations (Fig. 2A,B). At the highest input gain and high input synchronicity, 62% of spikes were aligned to the stimulus with a ±5 msec precision, and 48% of spikes were aligned even with a ±1 msec precision (Fig. 2C,D; mean values of 21 cells). Lowering the synchronization to the intermediate level but remaining at a high gain resulted in a partial loss of spike-timing precision. The decrease of spike precision with a decrease in input gain was gradual, and the presence of ±1 msec precision was lost before the ±5 msec criterion, was also not reached at very low gains (Fig. 2C,D).
The input synchronization of presynaptic elements in several independent groups may misrepresent the possible case of just one synchronous group in the input to a given DCN neuron. To verify this case we used an input condition in which only one group of inhibitory input elements was synchronized while all remaining input elements were independently active. The number of synchronized elements was systematically increased (0, 25, 50, 100, 150, 200, and 250) for two different input gain factors (2 and 8). The findings generally mirrored the case of multiple synchronized input groups. The precision increased with the input synchronization and with the input gain (Fig.2E). At an input gain of 8, ∼150 of 400 input elements needed to be synchronized to reach a proportion of 55% of spikes timed within ±5 msec in the cross-correlation (Fig.2E), a precision corresponding to that for the high input synchronization at a gain of 8 in Figure 2C. At an input gain of 2, ∼100 of 400 input elements had to be synchronized to reach a spike timing precision of 20% (±5 msec) that corresponds to the precision for the high input synchronization at a gain of 2 in Figure 2C.
Increasing input gain transformed a regular into an irregular spike pattern
DCN neurons in vivo show a highly irregular spiking pattern (Thach, 1968; LeDoux et al., 1998). In contrast, the spontaneous spiking of most DCN neurons in vitro is very regular (Jahnsen, 1986a; Aizenman and Linden, 1999). A likely cause of this difference is the presence of network activity and patterned synaptic input in vivo. To determine the relationship between the presence of synaptic input and the spiking pattern, we examined how interspike interval (ISI) distributions and autocorrelograms (ACs) varied as a function of an increasing level of synaptic input. At the lowest input gain, DCN neurons showed a regular firing pattern (Fig. 2A,B) very similar to their spontaneous spiking in the absence of any input. Regular spiking was reflected in a narrow symmetrical ISI distribution and a periodic AC (Fig. 3A,B). In contrast, at a high input gain and a high input synchronization, the firing pattern was highly irregular (Fig. 2A). Therefore, the ISI distribution was broad and asymmetric, and the AC showed no periodicity (Fig. 3A). At a high input gain but an intermediate input synchronization, the neurons also showed an irregular firing pattern (Figs. 2B, 3B) and a nonperiodic AC (Fig.3B). The ISI distribution at the intermediate input synchronization, however, lacked the distinct peak of short ISIs present with high input synchronization (Fig. 3A,B). This difference is similar to the different ISI distributions of DCN neurons reported in awake monkeys in which the ISI distribution during arm movement showed a large peak that was absent without movement (Thach, 1968). Our results suggest that these differences may be caused by a higher amount of synchronicity in the Purkinje cell input during arm movement. None of the ISI distributions observed in vivocorresponded to our case of low input gain.
An increase in synaptic gain leads to an increase in spiking irregularity as well as an increase in spike-time precision. The mechanism underlying these two effects is most easily understood by the voltage-clamping effect of synaptic conductances. When no input or input at a very low gain was given, cells showed stimulus-independent pacemaker activity (Fig. 3C, black trace). The synaptic conductance was not large enough to result in sufficient current (Fig. 3D) to pull the membrane potential close to Vsyn (Fig. 3C, gray trace). When the input gain was increased, cells were forced to follow Vsyn more closely (Fig. 3C,D). In particular, the simulated synaptic current was now of sufficient amplitude to counteract the intrinsic hyperpolarizing current after each spike. This can be seen as a depolarizing current after each spike (Fig. 3D). Approximately 50 msec after each spike, Vm started to track Vsynquite closely, and the simulated synaptic current was near zero. Spike timing in this condition was precisely aligned to the stimulus, because threshold crossings were determined by the trajectory of Vsyn. The spike pattern was irregular because fluctuations in Vsyn of sufficient size to trigger a spike occurred at irregular intervals.
The number and location of Purkinje cell synapses onto DCN neurons (Palkovits et al., 1977; De Zeeuw and Berrebi, 1995) and the mean rate of Purkinje cell activity (Savio and Tempia, 1985; Stratton et al., 1988) are fairly well established and were approximated by our dynamic-clamp stimuli (see Materials and Methods). The amplitude of unitary postsynaptic conductances of Purkinje cell inputs has not been determined yet. Our data result in an approximate estimate of a possible range of unitary synaptic conductances of Purkinje cell inputs. We found that a total mean inhibitory conductance of at least 8 nS was required so that inhibition could control DCN spiking. This corresponds to a unitary conductance change with a peak value of 34 pS resulting in a postsynaptic peak current of 1 pA at a driving force of 30 mV. This is still a very small value for a synaptic conductance, indicating that the real value might be larger. Preliminary data with minimal electrical stimulation of Purkinje cell inputs in our lab suggest unitary synaptic currents of 10 pA or below. These considerations indicate that individual Purkinje cells have only a small influence on single DCN neurons but that the voltage-clamp gain of the total synaptic input in vivo is likely to be significant.
Spikes were triggered by transient decreases in inhibition
To understand how inhibitory input is coded in output spiking, it is important to determine which particular input patterns trigger spikes and how a precise timing relation to the input is achieved. To analyze what input events led to spike generation, we calculated STAs of the inhibitory input frequency and of the inhibitory input conductance (Fig.4A,B). STAs correspond to the first Wiener kernel of a stimulus reconstruction and frequently capture most of the information about the input present in the output spike times (Bialek and Rieke, 1992). We found that spikes were preceded by a transient decrease in input frequency that was most pronounced at a high input gain and high input synchronization (Fig.4A). This decrease in frequency started ∼18 msec before the output spike and abruptly ended right at the time of the output spike. As expected, there was no consistent stimulus waveform surrounding a spike at low input gains, because spikes were not aligned to the stimulus under these conditions (Fig. 2A,B). A reduction in input synchronization resulted in a reduced amplitude of the spike-triggered average, whereas the onset time of the reduction was not changed (Fig. 4A).
The STA of the input conductance (Fig. 4B) is closely related to the STA of the input frequency in that each input spike contributes a unitary postsynaptic conductance change. Because the conductance change follows an input spike and has a decay time constant of 13 msec, the spike-triggered conductance change has a slightly delayed onset and a longer duration compared with the change in the input frequency. We reconstructed the expected inhibitory input conductance using our spike trains by convolving each output spike with the STA of the conductance (Fig. 4C,D). The reconstruction showed that the output spikes contain reliable information about the occurrence of transient decreases in the inhibitory input. In contrast, increases in inhibition from the mean value were not at all coded in the output spikes. In addition, cells clearly did not preferentially respond to events consisting of releases in inhibition from increased levels to the mean level (Fig. 4C,D, asterisks). Such a response would indicate the presence of postinhibitory rebound spiking. Although such rebound spiking was elicited in our recordings after negative current injection pulses in vitro as observed previously (Jahnsen, 1986a; Aizenman et al., 1998), the stimulus condition of ongoing excitatory and inhibitory input conductances did not trigger this response. This can be explained by the fact that the combined reversal potential of inhibition and excitation (Vsyn) and thus Vm did not reach values negative to −60 mV to deinactivate the channels required for the rebound response.
The input sequences we selected were aimed to simulate possible patterns of Purkinje cell input to DCN neurons. Because only white-noise stimuli yield STAs that are unbiased by the stimulus (Rieke et al., 1996), our STAs may reflect stimulus properties such as the time course of unitary IPSCs. Stimuli with different properties such as gain and synchronicity, however, showed STAs with a nearly identical time course but a different amplitude. We also constructed a stimulus, in which we synchronized pauses (10 or 50 msec) of spiking between groups of input elements but not the occurrence of spikes. The STA of the conductance for this stimulus was also very similar (data not shown). The stimulus condition of just one single group of synchronized input elements briefly described above was a special case because in this situation each decrease in inhibition was preceded by an increase relative to the mean. Thus the STA of this stimulus also showed a peak preceding the decrease in Gin that triggered a spike (data not shown). Overall these data show that for a wide range of presynaptic input patterns the spiking of DCN neurons was reliably triggered by decreases in the inhibitory input conductance that showed a minimum duration of ∼15 msec.
The precision of spike timing stayed high when synchronized inputs were jittered
Perfect input synchronization as used above is probably not usually achieved in vivo. To examine the effects of decreasing precision in the presynaptic synchronization, we constructed a set of stimuli with jitter. Using a stimulus with high gain (factor 8) and high synchronization, we systematically decreased the input synchronization by jittering the input spikes. The jitter value used for each spike was random within a range of 5 msec (±2.5 msec), 10 msec (±5 msec), 20 msec (±10 msec), or 50 msec (±25 msec). The resulting alignment of DCN output spikes with the stimulus stayed high for most jitter conditions (Fig.5A). For the precision criterion of ±5 msec, the proportion of stimulus-aligned spikes stayed nearly constant up to a jitter of 20 msec (Fig. 5C). For the more stringent precision criterion of ±1 msec, the number of well aligned spikes started to fall off markedly at a jitter of 5 msec (Fig.5C). Even at a jitter of 50 msec the stimulus alignment of spikes was better compared with the reference case in which all input elements were independently active (Fig. 2C,D). The maintained output precision despite input jitter can be explained by the averaging effect resulting from jittering many spikes independently. In essence this manipulation corresponds to a low-pass filtering of the synaptic conductance traceGin (Fig. 5B). Physiologically this finding signifies that the convergence of many presynaptic inhibitory elements allows for the precise control of output spiking even when the input events are not aligned as precisely. A similar function of sharpening the timing of output spikes by large sets of less well aligned input spikes has been put forth for excitatory inputs in the so-called synfire chain hypothesis (Abeles, 1991).
The spike rate was controlled by the rate of inhibitory input, whereas the spike precision remained unaffected
Spike-rate coding is often considered to be the most important mechanism of information transfer between neurons in the brain. In a spike-rate code the frequency of the synaptic inputs over some time window controls the frequency of output spiking. There is ample evidence in the literature that Purkinje cells show changes in spike rate related to behavior for time periods of 10–500 msec (Mano et al., 1991; Fortier et al., 1993; Krauzlis and Lisberger, 1996). The baseline spiking frequency in awake rats is ∼35 Hz (Savio and Tempia, 1985; Stratton et al., 1988), which we used as the mean frequency of our inhibitory input elements for the stimuli described above. To determine how frequency changes in the inhibitory input to DCN neurons affects output spiking, we tested one set of cells (n = 5) for five different inhibitory input frequencies. The effect of input frequency was tested at two input gains (factor 2 and 8) with high or without input synchronization. We found that under all stimulus conditions the output frequency increased with decreases in the inhibitory input frequency (Fig.6A,D). The average membrane potential after removal of spikes was more depolarized at high output frequencies (Fig. 6E), reflecting the depolarization in Vsyn with decreases in inhibitory input. In general, a linear relationship was found between the output frequency and the input frequency (Fig.6D) and between the output frequency and the membrane potential (Fig. 6E). Without input synchronization the output frequencies were zero for large input frequencies, and therefore, the frequency curves showed a threshold (Fig.6D,E, triangles). Overall the spike frequency of DCN neurons can be regulated in a linear way by the frequency of inhibitory input neurons, and thus these neurons can be said to use a rate code in decoding the rate of Purkinje cell inputs.
The spike-timing precision of DCN neurons did not change for different inhibitory input frequencies (Fig. 6A,B), but it depended on the input gain and input synchronization (Fig.6C). For all four conditions of gain and synchronicity, the proportion of stimulus-aligned spikes for the ±5 msec precision criterion was independent of the input frequency (Fig. 6C). These results show that the frequency of DCN neurons can be regulated by the input frequency without affecting the spike-timing precision.
The spike rate was strongly influenced by the input synchronization
Although the spike rate was nearly linearly dependent on the input frequency for a given combination of input gain and synchronicity, it was quite different at the same input frequency for different levels of gain and synchronicity (Fig. 6D). Therefore spike-rate coding of DCN neurons does not simply reflect the rate of input events but also the pattern of inputs. To quantify this effect we calculated the frequency response and membrane potential of DCN neurons to our first stimulus set in which input gain and input synchronization were varied systematically (Fig. 7). We found that an increasing input gain led to a similar increase in membrane depolarization for intermediate and high input synchronization (Fig. 7A). As described above (Fig. 3) the depolarization with increasing gain is explained by a stronger voltage-clamp effect toward the synaptic reversal potential of −40 mV for high input gains. For a high level of input synchronicity this increase in depolarization was paralleled by an increase in spike rate (Fig. 7B). In contrast, an intermediate level of input synchronization led to a final decrease in spike rate with increasing input gain even though the neuron was overall more depolarized than with high input synchronization (Fig. 7A,B). A reduction in spike rate was found for all of the 21 DCN neurons when input synchronicity was absent (Fig. 7C). A similar dependence between spike rate and input synchronization was seen in the experiments in which only one group of input elements was synchronized. Although the spike rate showed almost no dependence on the input synchronization at a gain of 2, it increased linearly with an increasing number of synchronized inputs at a gain of 8 (Fig. 7D). These results can be explained by a different mechanism of triggering spikes when the input gain was low than when it was high. At low gains, spikes are initiated by intrinsic pacemaker activity based on the level of membrane depolarization. This corresponds to the case of current injection in vitro, in which DCN neurons show a nearly linear relationship between current-induced depolarization and output spike rate (Jahnsen, 1986a). At high input gains a fundamentally different mechanism determined the generation of spikes in our experiments. In this case intrinsic pacemaker currents could not determine the trajectory of Vm, because it was clamped to the level given by Vsyn (Fig. 3C,D). In the absence of large fluctuations in the input, spikes could not escape from the clamping potential even when the membrane was depolarized. Therefore, spike generation at high input gains required the presence of significant fluctuations in Vsyn. The amplitude of such fluctuations declined with a reduction of input synchronization (Fig. 4D), which led to a reduction in spike rate.
As argued above (Fig. 3) the condition of high input gain is expected to operate in vivo because low-gain stimuli do not show the irregular spike pattern recorded in the DCN of behaving animals (Thach, 1968). We found that at the highest input gain the spike frequency was four times faster with high than without input synchronization (Fig.7C). To achieve the same effect by a change in input frequency, an average reduction of Purkinje cell input rate by >20 Hz was required (Fig. 6). Thus, changes in the synchronicity of inhibitory input can have as large an effect on the output spike rate as substantial changes in the total rate of input events.
DISCUSSION
Rate and temporal coding via inhibitory input
An important question in computational neuroscience is whether information might be encoded exclusively in the spike rate of neurons or in addition in the precise timing of spikes (Softky, 1995; Shadlen and Newsome, 1998). Current injection experiments illustrated that the spike timing of neurons can be controlled very precisely by temporally modulated currents (Mainen and Sejnowski, 1995). Information theoretical approaches indicate that a large amount of information about sensory inputs is potentially available in the precise spike timing (de Ruyter van Steveninck and Bialek, 1988; Borst and Theunissen, 1999). The discussion of possible mechanisms of a precise control of spike timing, however, has focused almost exclusively on synchronicity in excitatory inputs (Shadlen and Newsome, 1994, 1998;Holt et al., 1996; König et al., 1996). In the present study we examined whether and by what mechanisms inhibitory inputs may contribute to rate and temporal coding of output spikes. We found that synchronous activity and synchronous pauses in the input spike pattern resulted in the presence of transient decreases in the inhibitory conductance, which reliably and precisely triggered spikes. The spike rate of DCN neurons was strongly affected by the amount of synchronicity present in the input, and it depended also in an almost linear way on the rate of the inhibitory inputs. Thus, the spike rate of DCN neurons contained information about the spike rate as well as the synchronicity of inhibitory inputs, and the spike timing retained information about synchronous input events. A significant proportion of all presynaptic elements needed to be involved in synchronous activity patterns before they were effective. This is in contrast to a form of coincidence detection that has been postulated for excitation, in which strong dendritic nonlinearities would allow much smaller assembles of coactive inputs to trigger precisely timed spikes (Abeles, 1991;Aertsen et al., 1996; Holt et al., 1996). We used DCN neurons as an example to demonstrate possible coding by inhibition in a structure that is known to receive an important inhibitory projection pathway. Inhibitory projection pathways also occur outside the cerebellum, most notably in the basal ganglia. The effects shown, however, may also operate when a sufficient number of inhibitory inputs is received from local interneurons, as may be the case for pyramidal cortical cells.
Inhibition can only act in balance with excitation
We used simulated synaptic conductances as input patterns to DCN neurons in vitro to simulate an in vivo-like situation in which neurons receive thousands of synaptic inputs per second. This situation results in an ongoing baseline in the level of excitatory and inhibitory inputs, which provides a voltage clamp at the combined synaptic reversal potential (Vsyn; see Materials and Methods). The waveform of Vsyncreates an attractor toward which the membrane potential is pulled with a force proportional to the clamp gain. We showed that at realistic input levels the actual membrane potential stayed relatively close to the command potential given by Vsyn. To achieve realistic spike rates, the value of Vsyn in DCN neurons must be approximately −40 mV, which dictates the required balance of excitation and inhibition.
DCN neurons receive excitatory inputs as collaterals from mossy and climbing fibers that also excite cerebellar cortex. These inputs are not likely to be constant over time as in our study and thus may control spike timing in addition to inhibitory input. A majority of excitatory inputs on DCN neurons, however, is located distal on dendrites (De Zeeuw and Berrebi, 1995), which is likely to attenuate fast transients. Furthermore, excitatory inputs have a large NMDA component even at negative membrane potentials (Anchisi et al., 1998), which leads to a prolonged effect of excitation. These characteristics suggest that coincident excitatory input events might not be very effective in the rapid control of single spikes but may be important on a slower timescale, perhaps in conjunction with depolarized dendritic plateau states. The presence of active dendritic conductances that could lead to an amplification of fast AMPA inputs cannot be excluded at the present time, however. The actual dynamics of the excitatory input and their interaction with intrinsic properties may form an important component of DCN function and remain to be determined.
Contribution of intrinsic conductances to the control of spiking
We found that the voltage-clamp effect of large numbers of synaptic inputs effectively controls the subthreshold membrane potential. Although this effect to some degree shunts voltage-gated currents present in the neuron, their activation can still have a profound influence on the observed spike pattern. This is apparent when different cell types are stimulated with the same synaptic conductance patterns, and characteristically different output spike patterns are obtained (D. Jaeger, unpublished observations). A significant influence on the spike pattern is likely given by the voltage dependence and time constants of the depolarizing spike current itself, which needs to escape from Vsyn before any spike can occur. DCN neurons also have a persistent Na current (Gardette et al., 1985; Jahnsen, 1986b), which likely contributes to depolarizing the cell to spike initiation (Koch et al., 1995; Crill, 1996). Other subthreshold currents are also important, however. In DCN neurons a substantial hyperpolarizing current is activated after each action potential (Jahnsen, 1986b; Aizenman and Linden, 1999). In our experiments this current was partly counteracted by a depolarizing synaptic-clamp current (Fig. 3D), but nevertheless it led to a significant postspike hyperpolarization (Figs. 1B,3C). This hyperpolarization limited the spike frequency during depolarized phases of Vsyn. Another potential current affecting the spike pattern of DCN neurons is the low-threshold calcium current, which could lead to postinhibitory rebound spiking (Llinás and Mühlethaler, 1988; Aizenman et al., 1998; Aizenman and Linden, 1999). In our experiments the contribution of this current was likely to be minimal, however, because the prolonged periods of membrane hyperpolarization required to deinactivate this current were not present.
Spike irregularity and the gain of input conductances
Irregular spike patterns are found in many brain structures, and the debate has been lively for several years whether they reflect coincidence detection within a temporal code (Softky and Koch, 1993;Softky, 1995) or whether they represent randomness within a rate code (Shadlen and Newsome, 1994, 1998). Softky and Koch (1993) suggested that excitatory inputs could cause irregular firing only when the input synchronicity is large or when strong dendritic nonlinearities exist. We found that inhibition can cause irregularity in the spike pattern as recorded in DCN neurons in vivo (Thach, 1968) for all levels of input synchronization tested when the input gain was high. At high input gains, the total synaptic conductance was strong enough to overcome the intrinsic pacemaker dynamics of the neuron and to clamp Vm close to Vsyn. Therefore, fluctuations in Vsyn determined the timing of spike threshold crossings. At low or high levels of input synchronization fluctuations that led to threshold crossings in Vm were irregular, but at a reduced input synchronization they occurred less frequently, and the spike rate was reduced. Random small fluctuations in the input cannot add up in this system because Vm is continuously forced toward Vsyn. Thus, the voltage-clamp control of spiking we observed is not compatible with the view of synaptic current creating a “random walk,” as proposed by Shadlen and Newsome (1994,1998). Also in contrast to the model of Shadlen and Newsome, the membrane time constant does not determine the input integration time constant. Instead, the time required to depolarize Vm to trigger a spike at a high input gain closely followed the time course of Vsyn.
The presence of a large voltage-clamp gain in other brain structures is supported by in vivo intracellular data showing that fluctuations in subthreshold membrane potential are rapid and of large amplitude in cortical pyramidal neurons as well as other cell types (Paré et al., 1998; Stern et al., 1998; Lampl et al., 1999). The large synaptic current required to drive such changes in Vm indicates the presence of large synaptic conductances and hence the operation of a synaptic voltage-clamp mechanism at high gain as described.
The control of DCN spiking by Purkinje cell input
Many investigators have linked the function of the cerebellum to fine temporal aspects of movement control (Braitenberg, 1967; Ivry and Keele, 1989; Houk et al., 1996; Braitenberg et al., 1997; Mauk et al., 1998; Wang et al., 1998; Timmann et al., 1999). Such control requires that the output from the DCN is temporally precise on the order of a few milliseconds. Furthermore, the DCN consist of relatively few neurons for each effector system such that a mechanism of averaging the spike rate of a large population of DCN neurons to control movement timing seems unlikely. Therefore spike trains of a small number of DCN neurons need to be sufficient to transmit temporally precise information. Recordings from DCN neurons in relation to saccades show indeed that the temporal alignment of DCN spiking to external events can be at least 5 msec (Takikawa et al., 1998). Our data show that inhibitory Purkinje cell input is suitable to control DCN spiking at this precision, if synchronous activation changes are present in a large population of Purkinje cell inputs. This requirement places constraints on the computation taking place in cerebellar cortex, because only events that influence substantial numbers of Purkinje cells appear to be significant. The exact geometrical layout of the convergence pattern of Purkinje cells onto single DCN neurons is still unknown. If distinct patches or sagittal stripes of Purkinje cells were to contact single DCN neurons, further inferences about cerebellar computation could be drawn. For example, the tidal wave theory byBraitenberg et al. (1997) would require that Purkinje cells placed along a parallel fiber bundle project to the same DCN neurons. In contrast, the model of coactivated patches of Purkinje cells (Bower, 1997) would predict a convergent projection of patches of Purkinje cells onto single DCN neurons.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH-57256 and by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Research Fellowship GA-627-1 to V.G.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Dieter Jaeger, Department of Biology, 1510 Clifton Road, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322. E-mail: djaeger{at}emory.edu.