Abstract
Spatiotemporal maps of brain activity based on magnetoencephalography were used to observe sequential stages in language processing and their modification during repetition priming. Subjects performed word-stem completion and produced either novel or repeated (primed) words across trials. Activation passes from primary visual cortex (activated at ∼100 msec after word presentation), to left anteroventral occipital (∼180 msec), to cortex in and near Wernicke's (∼210 msec) and then Broca's (∼370 msec) areas. In addition, a posteroventral temporal area is activated simultaneously with posterosuperior temporal cortex. This area shows an early (∼200–245 msec) increase in activation to repeated word stems. In contrast, prefrontal and anterior temporal regions showed activity reductions to repeated word stems late (∼365–500 msec) in processing. These results tend to support classical models of language and suggest that an effect of direct item repetition is to allow word-form processing to increase its contribution to task performance while concurrently allowing reductions in time-consuming frontal temporal processing.
- word-stem completion
- magnetoencephalography
- temporal lobe
- planum temporale
- prefrontal cortex
- language
- occipital lobe
- word form
Lesion-based studies of language provide a general framework for interpreting brain activation during verbal processing. In the classical Wernicke–Geschwind model of language, seen words acquire meaning only after conversion into a phonological image (i.e., only after entering the same stream as heard words) (Geschwind, 1965; Benson, 1979). This model proposes that initial visual processing in occipital cortex is followed by grapheme to phoneme conversion mediated by the left angular and supramarginal gyri. Afterward, lexical access occurs in left posterosuperior temporal cortex (classically defined “Wernicke's area”). Left posteroventral prefrontal cortex (classically defined “Broca's area”) is recruited last and may subserve syntax and verbal production.
This influential but incomplete early model was based on language deficits produced by brain lesions (Benson, 1979). Positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) permit hemodynamic changes associated with processing of visually presented words to be mapped in normal subjects. In addition to the classical language areas (Howard et al., 1992; Pujol et al., 1996;Phelps et al., 1997; Thompson-Schill et al., 1999), such studies commonly find activation in occipital (Petersen et al., 1988, 1989,1990; Binder, 1997) and medial frontal (Fiez and Petersen, 1998) areas. Finally, language-related activation to visual words has been found in basal temporal areas with intracranial event-related potentials (iERPs) (Smith et al., 1986; Halgren et al., 1994a; Nobre and McCarthy, 1995), and electrical stimulation in these areas can cause specific language deficits (Burnstine et al., 1990; Luders et al., 1991). Collectively, these studies build an understanding of the network of brain areas that subserve language processing.
To accurately characterize higher cognitive functions such as language, however, knowledge about the timing and sequence of responses is also needed. For example, activity within anterior occipital cortex could be associated with word-form encoding only if it occurs early. Conversely, medial frontal activation may be involved in response organization, so recruitment of these areas should occur relatively late. Activity in posteroventral prefrontal cortex, near classically defined Broca's area, is ubiquitous and could be related to an earlier process, such as lexical or semantic access, rather than to response production as suggested in classical models.
Although fMRI and PET have good spatial resolutions, their temporal resolutions are inadequate to sequence language-related brain activity. iERPs provide accurate temporal and spatial resolution but only in limited areas in patients with brain disease. The current study uses magnetoencephalography (MEG) to map language-related activation in normal subjects with excellent temporal resolution and adequate spatial resolution (Dale et al., 2000). A word-stem completion task requiring word generation was chosen because it has been well studied hemodynamically (Squire et al., 1992; Buckner et al., 1995; Schacter et al., 1996; Ojemann et al., 1998). Furthermore, presenting just parts of words would be expected to prevent immediate lexical access and thus allow the successive stages to be more clearly distinguished. Within the word-stem completion task, repetition priming was used to help separate different aspects of language processing.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Stem completion task. Eight normal right-handed males viewed centrally presented three-letter word stems projected onto a screen to subtend a <5% visual angle. Using Mac Probe software (Hunt, 1994), word stems were presented in Geneva font as white letters on a black background. Subjects were shown 224 word stems, with seven word stems repeated randomly throughout the experiment. Each repeated word stem was shown 15 times. Word stems were presented for 250 msec, and stimulus onset asynchrony was 2.3 sec. The stimulus list was the same as that used in previous fMRI studies (Buckner et al., 2000) and consisted of word stems that were selected from a pool of 288 unique three-letter word stems that each had at least five English word completions. Between word-stem presentations, subjects fixated on a “+” sign in the center of the screen. Word stems consisted of all capital letters (i.e., “GRE”). Subjects were given a study block before the experiment in which they generated words to a set of 42 word stems. Seven word stems were repeated randomly six times and were the same “repeated” word stems used during the actual experiment. For the experimental phase, subjects were required to silently generate words that began with the presented word stems. Subjects were told not to generate words that were proper nouns.
Silent generation was requested to prevent artifacts because of movement of oral musculature. Subjects were experienced in MEG experiments and had been selected on the basis of reliability and compliance with instructions. Data from previous studies using similar covert generation procedures indicates behavioral priming highly similar to that obtained from overt stem completion and that covert stem completion evokes robust fMRI responses, including frontal temporal language areas (Buckner et al., 2000).
MEG recording. MEG signals were recorded from 204 channels at 0.1–100 Hz using a Neuromag instrument (4D Neuroimaging, San Diego, CA) with orthogonal pairs of planar gradiometers at each of 102 locations over the entire scalp. Separate averages of novel and repeated word-stem completion trials were constructed for each subject after rejecting trials with eyeblinks or other artifacts using amplitude criteria confirmed with visual inspection. Head movement was minimized using a chinstrap and foam padding on the sides of the head.
Cortical surface reconstruction. Geometrical representations for the cortical surfaces of each subject were obtained using procedures described previously (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999). First, high-resolution three-dimensional T1-weighted structural images were acquired for each subject using a 1.5T Picker Eclipse (Marconi Medical, Cleveland, OH). Next, the cortical white matter was segmented and the estimated border between gray and white matter was tessellated, providing a topologically correct representation of the surface with ∼150,000 vertices per hemisphere. For the inverse computation, the cortical surface was decimated to ∼3000 dipoles per hemisphere, which is approximately equivalent to one dipole every 10 mm along the cortical surface. Finally, the folded surface tessellation was “inflated” to unfold cortical sulci, thereby providing a convenient format for visualizing cortical activation patterns (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999). For purposes of intersubject averaging, the reconstructed surface for each subject was morphed into an average spherical representation, optimally aligning sulcal and gyral features across subjects but minimizing metric distortions (Fischl et al., 1999).
Forward solution. The boundary element method was used to calculate the signal expected at each MEG sensor, for each dipole location (deMunck, 1992; Oostendorp and Van Oosterom, 1992). The computation of the MEG forward solution has been shown to only require the inner skull boundary to achieve an accurate solution (Meijs and Peters, 1987; Meijs et al., 1987; Hamalainen and Sarvas, 1989).
The MEG sensor coordinate system was aligned with the MRI coordinate system using three head position (HPI) coils attached to the scalp (Hamalainen et al., 1993). The HPI coils generate weak magnetic signals and thus can be directly localized by the MEG sensors. The positions of the HPI coils with respect to the subject's head (and thus MRI) were determined by measuring multiple points (including the HPI coils) using a Polhemus FastTrack three-dimensional digitizer (Polhemus, Colchester, VT).
Inverse solution. To estimate the time courses of cortical activity, the noise-normalized, anatomically constrained linear estimation approach described by Dale et al. (2000) was used. This approach is similar to the generalized least-squares or weighted minimum norm solution (Hamalainen and Ilmoniemi, 1984; Dale and Sereno, 1993), but the estimate is normalized for noise sensitivity (Dale et al., 2000). The noise normalization has the effect of greatly reducing the variation in the point-spread function between locations. This approach provides statistical parametric maps of cortical activity, similar to the statistical maps typically generated using fMRI or PET data but with a temporal resolution of 5 msec or better.
Because no a priori assumptions were made about the local dipole orientation in the current study, three components are required for each location. A sensitivity-normalized estimate of the local current dipole power (sum of squared dipole component strengths) at locationi is given by
where Gi is the set of (three) dipole component indices for the ith location, andwi denotes the ith row of the inverse operator w (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Liu et al., 1998; Dale et al., 2000). Note that, under the null hypothesis,qi(t) isF-distributed, with three degrees of freedom for the numerator. The degrees of freedom for the denominator are typically large, depending on the number of time samples used to calculate the noise covariance matrix C. For the novel and repeated word stems, significance thresholds were set at p < 10−8, with a full red response indicatingp < 10−11 andp < 10−17, indicating peak activation with a bright yellow color. For dynamic statistical parametric maps of novel–repeat subtractions, thresholds werep < 10−5,p < 10−7, andp < 10−12, respectively.
In summary, noise sensitivity-normalized cortical surface-constrained minimum norm inverse solutions were calculated every 5 msec for every condition and every individual. These movies were then averaged on the cortical surface across individuals after aligning their sulcal–gyral patterns.
RESULTS
Generating words to novel word stems
Novel word stems evoked a robust left-lateralized MEG signal (Fig.1). The response followed a posterior-to-anterior sequence of cortical recruitment involving visual processing areas, regions near classical language areas, and other sites (Fig. 2). The earliest activation was bilateral, first in the occipital pole (from ∼100 to 125 msec) (Fig. 2) and then in ventroanterior occipital regions (from ∼125 to 145 msec) (Fig. 2). Activity was then lateralized to the left ventroanterior occipital cortex beginning at ∼150 msec, becoming clearly lateralized from ∼165 to 190 msec (Fig. 2). Activity remained strongly biased to the left hemisphere during all subsequent time intervals.
Sample waveforms. MEG waveforms from all 204 channels in one subject in response to novel (black lines) and repeated (gray lines) word stems. Signals from four pairs of gradiometers are shown in expanded format on the left.
Estimated cortical activity patterns at different latencies after reading word stems. Activation begins with a bilateral visual response in posterior occipital cortex (100–125 msec) and subsequently spreads forward in ventral occipital cortex (125–145 msec) and lateralizes to the left hemisphere (170–190 msec). It then spreads to both posteroventral and lateral temporal areas (205–230 msec) and then progressively involves anterior temporal (235–365 msec) and ventral prefrontal (370–515 msec) cortices, before fading after 515 msec. Dynamic statistical parametric maps (see Materials and Methods) show the maximal activation during the indicated period.
A progressive posterior-to-anterior shift of activation followed the early ventral occipital responses. This shift began at ∼195 msec, and by ∼205–230 msec, activity was prominent in posterior temporal regions, including the planum temporale by ∼210 msec, as well as posterior segments of the superior, middle, and inferior temporal gyri, extending to include the fusiform gyrus (Fig. 2).
In the 235–365 msec period, the response progressed further anteriorly, encompassing lateral anterior temporal cortex and insulo-opercular regions (Fig. 2). Activity mainly involved the superior temporal sulcus and then extended broadly to include middle and anterior levels of the superior temporal sulcus, the temporal pole, posteroventral prefrontal cortex, and collateral sulcus. On the ventral surface of the temporal lobe, activation was most prominent in the inferior temporal gyrus and lateral occipitotemporal sulcus but continued to include the fusiform gyrus. Although not shown, activation became briefly focal in the left planum temporale at 300 msec.
Finally, from ∼365 to 415 msec (Fig. 2), responses were strongest in the planum temporale, anterior temporal cortex (laterally and ventrally), and inferior prefrontal regions. The prefrontal activation was most prominent in the inferior frontal gyrus, pars opercularis, including the pars triangularis, as well as orbital cortex. The same regions remained activated until after 500 msec; in addition, after ∼415 msec, activity was again seen in posteroventral temporal cortex. After 470 msec, activity became more bilateral, again involving mainly orbital and ventral occipitotemporal cortex on the right. Inferior prefrontal and anterior temporal activation was still apparent at 510 msec. Although some activation was visible for >1000 msec in some individuals (Fig. 1), in the group average, activity was minimal by ∼550 msec.
Repetition effects
Activation movies were made from the MEG waveforms obtained by subtracting the waveforms evoked by the repeated word stems from those evoked by the novel word stems (Fig.3). Activation present in the novel-minus-repeated movies indicates significant power in the difference waveform between these conditions at the indicated latency and estimated location. Because power is not a signed quantity, a single movie cannot be used to infer which condition had greater absolute activation at any particular location and time. Instead, the direction of that difference (i.e., which condition had greater absolute activation at that particular location and time) can only be inferred by comparing the activations in the nonsubtracted novel and repeated movies. In making these comparisons, one must bear in mind that the original (nonsubtracted) waveforms can be either positive or negative at each point, and thus a large difference in the subtracted waveform could arise from nonsubtracted activations that are equal in size but opposite in polarity. Therefore, the latency of differences in the brain response to different conditions can be directly inferred from these maps, but interpretation of how these differences arise requires reference to the nonsubtracted data.
Repetition effects. The earliest differential activation (in favor of repeated word stems) is estimated to lie in left ventral temporal cortex between 200 and 245 msec. Later differential activation (larger to novel word stems) occurs at 365–500 msec, mainly in anterior temporal, inferior prefrontal cortex, and the planum temporale.
The novel-minus-repeated differences were left-lateralized and localized to inferior ventral temporal regions, temporal pole, inferior prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, planum temporale, and occipitotemporal junction, in that temporal order (Fig. 3). Some areas strongly activated by the novel and repeated word stems, notably in the occipital lobe, showed no significant repetition effect. That is, the effects of repetition were concentrated in more anterior cortical areas and appeared anatomically selective for a subset of areas activated during word-stem completion.
The earliest significant repetition-associated differences appeared in left posteroventral temporal cortex (centered in the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus) at ∼200 msec after stimulus onset and reflected greater activation to repeated word stems (Fig. 3,left). The next phase of the repetition effect was considerably later (from ∼365 to 500 msec) (Fig. 3, right) and in the opposite direction. The responses were greater to novel word stems, moving from the temporal pole to inferior prefrontal regions by ∼420 msec and to superior temporal regions by ∼455 msec. Medial frontal activation peaked at ∼445 msec and decreased with repetition (data not shown). Activity remained strong in the above regions and was greater to novel word stems until ∼500 msec.
Finally, from ∼510 msec onward, activation began to fade in lateral regions of the cortex and temporal pole. The differential response shifted to inferior ventral temporal cortex, in which it was stronger to repeated word stems until ∼530 msec, after which all differential activity became minimal.
Activation at individual sites
Figures 2 and 3 show snapshots of the estimated activity over the entire lateral and ventral aspects of the cortical surface at selected moments in time. An alternative method for visualizing activation is to plot the normalized estimated dipole strength at selected locations at all latencies (Fig. 4). These time courses support the same general points made by the movies: (1) the posterior occipital lobe was activated with a short latency and equally for novel and repeated word stems; (2) slightly more anterior occipital sites showed later activation, which peaked at ∼165 msec and did not change with repetition; (3) posteroventral temporal sites (centered within the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus) showed an early increased activity to repeated word stems; (4) the planum temporale responded at approximately the same time but did not show a repetition effect until later, when it responded more to novel word stems; and (5) the anterior temporal lobe and (6) the inferior frontal gyrus were the last areas to be engaged and showed larger responses to repeated word stems.
Time courses for selected location. Occipital regions are activated early and do not change with repetition, whereas more anterior regions activate later and show strong repetition effects.
DISCUSSION
MEG was used to map the spatiotemporal orchestration of brain regions involved in the generation of words to visually presented word stems and their modification during repetition priming. As summarized in Table 1, the response during novel word-stem completion followed a posterior-to-anterior sequence with distinct spatiotemporal stages. The earliest activation was in the occipital pole, followed by the anteroventral occipital cortex. Activation subsequently spread to a region at or near classically defined Wernicke's area, as well as portions of the ventral temporal cortex, then to anterior temporal and prefrontal cortex near classically defined Broca's area, and finally to medial frontal cortex. Thus, the earliest area to be activated was cortex, which has been shown to contain retinotopic representations of foveal vision (Sereno et al., 1995), whereas the last area was within cortex associated with late phases of verbal response production (Penfield and Roberts, 1959; Fiez and Petersen, 1998). The timing of activation, from visual processing to Wernicke's and then Broca's areas, provides a striking consistency with the major components of classical models of language processing (Geschwind, 1965; Benson, 1979). In addition, activation in ventral occipital and temporal areas extends the results of previous neuroimaging studies, suggesting that these regions have an important role in language processing.
Summary of MEG responses: novel word stems
Summary of MEG responses: effects of repetition
Classical language areas
Between 200 and 500 msec, there was strong activity in and near Wernicke's area; from ∼370 msec onward, there was also activity in and near Broca's area. The activation in Wernicke's area involved what would, in other contexts, be considered to be auditory association cortex. Although the current results do not identify the process performed by this area, similar activation has been found in other studies in which words are generated in response to visually presented letter strings (Frith et al., 1991; Demonet et al., 1992; Raichle et al., 1994; Menard et al., 1996; Buckner et al., 2000). These findings lend support to a core assumption of classical language models: that auditory (or perhaps polymodal) association cortex in the posterior superior temporal region participates in the encoding of visually presented stimuli associated with word or word-like forms.
Late activation near Broca's area suggests that it may participate in the late stages of word production. Neuroimaging studies with PET, fMRI, iERPs, and MEG have found activation in this region even when no overt or covert verbal response was required (Halgren et al., 1994b; Buckner et al., 2000; Dale et al., 2000). It appears that the left posteroventral prefrontal cortex may be involved in the manipulation of higher-level language representations that are temporally extended and occur late in the act of word production (for review, see Gabrieli et al., 1998; Smith et al., 1998; Ungerleider et al., 1998), perhaps to guide response selection (Thompson-Schill et al., 1999).
Visual association cortex
Subsequent to initial localization within the occipital pole, activation rapidly spread to ventral anterior occipital cortex. A variety of neuropsychological, neurophysiological, and neuroimaging data in humans and animals identify this region as part of the ventral visual pathway used in the processing of visual stimuli as objects (Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982; Ungerleider and Haxby, 1994; Farah, 1995). Evidence for wordform-specific activation in this area has been found with iERPs (Nobre and McCarthy, 1995), MEG (Salmelin et al., 2000), and hemodynamic imaging (Petersen and Fiez, 1993). The response observed in the current study was initially bilateral at ∼125–145 msec and then clearly 2lateralized to the left hemisphere at ∼165–190 msec. The later activation is occurring in a location anterior to the retinotopic visual areas (Tootell et al., 1998) and at a time closely following their recruitment (Regan, 1989). In addition, previous iERP studies have reported a language or word-form-specific negativity at similar latencies within the ventral anterior occipital cortex (Nobre et al., 1994; Allison et al., 1999).
A similar activation is observed with faces and has been suggested to embody a material-specific encoding stage (Halgren et al., 1994a, 2000; Allison et al., 1999; Haxby et al., 2000). Together with these previous results, the current study suggests that word stems may pass through a specific encoding stage in the left ventral occipitotemporal cortex from ∼165 to 190 msec, mediating between earlier visual and later cognitive processing; this process is likely associated with learned word forms.
Posteroventral temporal cortex
At the same time that activation spreads from the putative word-form area to Wernicke's area, it also spreads to left posteroventral temporal cortex, including parts of the fusiform and inferior temporal gyri. Electrical stimulation of this area has been found to produce profound and specific language deficits (Burnstine et al., 1990; Luders et al., 1991). The location of this activation, just anterior to the putative word-form area in inferotemporal cortex, and the timing of this activation, just after the putative word-form activation (beginning ∼200 msec after word stem presentation onset), are both consistent with this activation supporting visual access to the lexical representations. Some models suggest that highly familiar words are preferentially processed through direct visual access (for review, see Humphreys and Evett, 1985; Caplan, 1990). This may correspond to the current observation that the left posteroventral activation was much larger in response to repeated word stems. Consistent with this speculation, a recent fMRI study found that well-learned mirror-reversed words also evoked increased activation in what appears to be the same area (Poldrack et al., 1998). However, repeated normal whole words do not always evoke increased activation in this area (Dale et al., 2000). These data suggest that this area may be especially invoked when, after sufficient training, word-like stimuli (i.e., reversed words or word stems) come to automatically evoke a lexical representation.
Anterior temporal cortex
Prominent activation was also observed in the anterior temporal lobe, with an onset latency longer than Wernicke's or posteroventral temporal areas. Like Broca's area, anterior temporal activation decreased in response to repeated word stems. These repetition-induced decreases were maximal from ∼365 to 500 msec, corresponding in location and timing to a previous MEG–fMRI study of repetition priming to visually presented words (Dale et al., 2000). Furthermore, anteroventral temporal iERP responses to words are also sensitive to repetition (Smith et al., 1986; Halgren et al., 1994a) and semantic priming (Nobre et al., 1994). The repetition-induced decline in Broca's area has been observed previously with PET and fMRI (for review, see Buckner et al., 2000), as well as with iERP.
Frontotemporal activation at ∼400 msec in language tasks appears to correspond to the scalp-recorded, event-related potential component termed the N400 (Dale et al., 2000). The N400 is evoked by potentially meaningful stimuli, such as words and faces, and is modulated by the “ease” of integrating the stimulus with a cognitive context (for review, see Rugg, 1987; Kutas and Van Petten, 1988; Halgren, 1990). The amplitude of this response is attenuated by stimulus repetition, as was the anteroventral temporal and posteroventral prefrontal activation in the current study. These studies suggest that the N400, and thus the associated anteroventral temporal activation, embody supramodal cognitive integration at the semantic level (Damasio et al., 1996).
Spatiotemporal integration in visual language processing
In summary, the current study found a progression of activation during word-stem completion, beginning bilaterally in the occipital pole and then shifting to ventral occipital cortex. Activity quickly lateralized to the left anterior occipital cortex and then spread to ventral and dorsal posterior temporal areas, followed by anterior temporal and finally posteroventral prefrontal cortex. In the final stage, there was sustained coactivation of all of the above areas, with the exception of posterior occipital cortex. This sustained activation may permit multiple sources of lexical information (i.e., phonemic, orthographic, and semantic) to converge (possibly within Broca's area) and constrain verbal response selection.
In the current study, the sustained activation to novel word stems was greatly abbreviated and attenuated when they were repeated. That is, the early repetition-induced increase in posteroventral temporal cortex was followed by a widespread decrease in frontotemporal cortex. It is tempting to infer that repeated presentation allows word stems to directly activate lexical representations without involving widespread integrative mechanisms.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the United States Public Health Service [National Institutes of Health (NIH) Grant NS18741], by the National Foundation for Functional Brain Imaging (Department of Energy Grant DE-FG03-99ER62764), and by NIH Grant MH57506. We thank Bruce Fischl, Arthur Liu, Thomas Witzel, Jeff Lewine, Bruce Rosen, and David Caplan.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Eric Halgren, Massachusetts General Hospital, Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Center, Room 2301, Building 149, 13th Street, Charlestown, MA 02129. E-mail:halgren{at}nmr.mgh.harvard.edu.