Abstract
Serotonin modulates afferent synaptic transmission to the lateral giant neurons of crayfish, which are command neurons for escape behavior. Low concentrations, or high concentrations reached gradually, are facilitatory, whereas high concentrations reached rapidly are inhibitory. The modulatory effects rapidly reverse after brief periods of application, whereas longer periods of application are followed by facilitation that persists for hours. These effects of serotonin can be reproduced by models that involve multiple interacting intracellular signaling systems that are each stimulated by serotonin. The dependence of the neuromodulatory effect on dose, rate, and duration of modulator application may be relevant to understanding the effects of natural neuromodulation on behavior and cognition and to the design of drug therapies.
Serotonin, octopamine, and GABA are all known to be present in crayfish and/or lobster nerve cord (Kravitz et al., 1976; Livingstone et al., 1980; Beltz and Kravitz, 1983;Glanzman and Krasne, 1986; Mulloney and Hall, 1990; Schneider et al., 1993) and can modulate the response of the crayfish lateral giant escape command neurons (LGs) to excitatory synaptic input. As a result, they regulate the excitability of escape (Glanzman and Krasne, 1983; Vu and Krasne, 1993; Yeh et al., 1996, 1997). As far as is known, the effect of GABA is always to inhibit and that of octopamine is to facilitate. However, the effect of 5-HT appears not to be fixed but to vary as a function of the animals' past social experience. When 5-HT is applied to the nerve cord of crayfish that have lived for several weeks as social subordinates, its effect is to inhibit EPSPs produced in the LGs by sensory activity, whereas 5-HT exposure to the cord of social dominants usually causes a facilitation of EPSP amplitude (Yeh et al., 1996, 1997). In social isolates, the picture is less clear, with different laboratories obtaining opposite effects (Glanzman and Krasne, 1983; Vu and Krasne, 1993; Yeh et al., 1996, 1997).
We report here that the LGs of social isolates have machinery for both serotonergic facilitation and inhibition and that the modulatory effect produced depends in complex ways on the dose and schedule of 5-HT exposure. These dependencies account for the opposing effects of serotonin obtained in different laboratories. We show that these complex dependencies could result from serotonergic activation of parallel and possibly interacting intracellular signaling cascades. However, whatever their mechanisms, variations in the qualitative form of aminergic neuromodulation as a function of modulator dose, onset characteristics, and exposure duration may have important consequences for pharmacological therapies and ultimately be relevant to comprehending the ways in which neuromodulators are used naturally.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals. Experiments were performed on either adultProcambarus clarkii measuring 8–9 cm rostrum to telson or on juveniles 2–3 cm in length. Crayfish were obtained from Atchalafaya Biological Supply (Raceland, LA) and kept isolated in 14 × 20 cm polyethylene dishes for at least 2 weeks before experimentation. Adults and juveniles gave similar results, and their data were pooled. We avoided use of animals kept in the lab for many months because we believe that they have diminished responsiveness to applied 5-HT.
Experimental procedures. Animals were cooled gradually to 5°C, the abdomen was pinned on Sylgard in crayfish saline, and the nerve cord was exposed dorsally as in Krasne (1969). Thereafter, experiments were at ∼20°C. KCl-filled (3m), ∼10 MΩ recording electrodes were placed in proximal dendrites of LGs, usually in the sixth abdominal ganglion but in the third or fourth in a few experiments, and platinum wire stimulating electrodes were placed on roots 2–4 (root 2 in third and fourth ganglion experiments). Test EPSPs were evoked every 2 min by a 0.2 msec voltage pulse at stimulating electrodes. Exposure to 5-HT was begun when the amplitude of evoked EPSPs, which often drifted up or down after dissection and penetration, stabilized (usually ∼30 min). Electrical activity was stored digitally for later analysis.
5-HT application. 5-HT was applied by superfusion in juveniles and by arterial perfusion (as in Mulloney et al., 1987) in adults. LOW concentration was 10−8m (perfusion) or 5 × 10−6m(superfusion), and HIGH concentration was 5 × 10−5m (perfusion) or 10−4m(superfusion). Exposure periods were either SHORT (10 min) or LONG (45 min or 40–60 min in initial experiments). For FAST applications, 5-HT was stepped to full concentration as rapidly as possible (within a few minutes). For SLOW applications, it was dripped so that full concentration was reached in ∼20–30 min (assessed by replacing 5-HT solutions with salt solutions and measuring conductance). 5-HT creatinine sulfate crystals (Research Biochemicals, Natick, MA) were stored with desiccant in a freezer (for not more than a few months) and dissolved just before use in 4 mm HEPES-buffered crayfish Ringer's solution, pH 7.2.
Data analysis. The EPSPs evoked by stimulation of ganglionic roots generally begin with an early depolarizing peak (α) caused by arrival of monosynaptic input and a slightly later peak (β) produced by disynaptic input (Fig.1A,B). Subsequent portions of the response reflect a mixture of polysynaptic excitatory and inhibitory inputs that follow the stimulus. In analyzing our data, we measured membrane potential at the times at which α and β peaks were located at the beginning of the experiment, even when these peaks subsequently shifted somewhat. In some cases, the β peak was not unimodal. We then usually measured the amplitude of the earlier peak. For graphing, EPSP amplitudes were normalized to their value on the last control EPSP before introduction of 5-HT. Although we attempted to set initial EPSP levels so that LG spikes, which interfered with measurement of EPSP amplitudes, would be unlikely, sometimes serotonin increased EPSPs enough to cause spikes. In these cases, we entered into analyses an EPSP amplitude 20% higher than the largest EPSP amplitude of the experiment that was not contaminated by a spike. Mean EPSP amplitudes were calculated for the last 5 pre-serotonin trials, the last 5 serotonin trials (the last three in the case of the SHORT condition), and wash trials 15–19 (30 min after the start of serotonin washout). Statistical analyses were on differences between the mean EPSP amplitude at the time point of interest and the pre-serotonin mean. Statistical tests were two-tailedt tests.
RESULTS
The LG receives converging synaptic input from primary mechanosensory afferents and mechanosensory interneurons (Fig.1A) that produce monosynaptic (α) and disynaptic (β) EPSPs, respectively (Fig. 1B) (Krasne, 1969); LG spikes and reflexive tail flips are usually produced only when the β peak exceeds firing level (Krasne, 1969; Olson and Krasne, 1981), although under some circumstances the α component can also become large enough to trigger spikes and behavior (Edwards et al., 1994).
The effect of rapid application of 5-HT (maximum level reached within 1–2 min) for 10 min or less at a high concentration (the FAST–SHORT–HIGH condition; see Materials and Methods) is shown in Figure 1B1 for an illustrative preparation. Both the α and β components of the LG EPSP declined during the 5-HT but were restored to their baseline values after several minutes of 5-HT washout. This same pattern can be seen to hold for the averaged normalized β component in Figure2, which plots the time course of 5-HT effects for all conditions, superimposed to facilitate comparison. Figure 3A shows mean EPSP amplitudes at the end of 5-HT exposure and after 30 min of washout. The decline of the β component is statistically reliable at the end of the 5-HT exposure (p < 0.05) but not significantly different from baseline after 30 min of wash. As in several conditions of this study, the effects of 5-HT on the α component paralleled those on the β component but were not statistically reliable. However, in two previous studies in which 5-HT exposures comparable with those of the FAST–SHORT–HIGH conditions were examined (Glanzman and Krasne, 1983; Vu and Krasne 1993), the decline of the α component amplitude, although generally smaller than that of the β component, was reliable. Moreover, if the data from the FAST–SHORT–HIGH condition are combined with the identically obtained data from the first 10 min of 5-HT exposure in the FAST–LONG–HIGH condition, the mean percentage decline of the α component at the end of 10 min 5-HT exposure does become statistically significant (p < 0.05).
Very different results were obtained when the same concentration of serotonin was applied much more slowly, so that the modulator reached its maximum level only after 20–30 min (SLOW–LONG–HIGH condition). Under this condition, facilitation rather than inhibition of both LG EPSP components developed, and the facilitation became persistent, long outlasting removal of the serotonin (Figs.1B2 , 2, 3). In both Figures 1 and 2, the β component amplitude appears to fall during the latter half of the period of 5-HT exposure and to rise again during wash. This was seen in half of the preparations, but it was not statistically reliable across the full population. The α component changes, which were in the same direction as the statistically reliable β component changes, were not statistically reliable (Fig. 3B). However, because increases in the α component in comparable previous work (Yeh et al., 1996,1997) were statistically reliable, we believe that the increases, although insignificant here, are real.
These findings for the FAST–SHORT–HIGH and SLOW–LONG–HIGH conditions duplicate the opposing results of previous studies (Glanzman and Krasne, 1983; Vu and Krasne, 1993; Yeh et al., 1996, 1997) and define the circumstances under which they are obtained.
With slow 5-HT application, EPSP facilitation began when the gradually rising concentration of 5-HT was still quite low (consistent with Yeh et al., 1997). This suggested the possibility that the concentration threshold of facilitation was lower than that needed to cause EPSP reduction. We thus examined the effects of rapid application of a low concentration of 5-HT that was maintained for 45–60 min (FAST–LONG–LOW condition; see Materials and Methods). This procedure produced prompt facilitation of the LG EPSP instead of inhibition, and the facilitation persisted after washout of 5-HT, as in the response to the SLOW–LONG–HIGH application (Figs.1B4 , 2, 3). The increases of both the α and β components at the end of 5-HT application and after 30 min of wash were statistically significant (Fig. 3D).
Because persistent facilitation was seen during washout in both the SLOW–LONG–HIGH and the FAST–LONG–LOW conditions, but not in the FAST–SHORT–HIGH condition, we examined the effect of FAST–LONG–HIGH exposures. Consistent with the findings for the FAST–SHORT–HIGH condition, the initial effect of the 5-HT was inhibitory. This inhibition persisted throughout the ∼45 min period of 5-HT exposure (Figs. 1B3 , 2, 3C; both α and β component reductions statistically significant). However, when the 5-HT was washed out, the inhibition gave way to a persistent facilitation (Figs. 1B3 , 2). This facilitation was statistically reliable for the β but not the α component (Fig. 3C). Thus, persistent facilitation occurred in the aftermath of all LONG exposures, regardless of the initial effect of serotonin (Figs.1B2–B4 ; 2,WASH curves; 3B–D, open bars). In many of the present experiments, facilitation persisted for >1 hr without decline, and in a previous report using slow application (Yeh et al., 1997), facilitation of both α and β were routinely seen to persist throughout 1 hr of wash and in some cases seen to persist unabated during 5 hr of wash.
The LGs became depolarized during exposure to 5-HT in all conditions and all experiments and by approximately the same amount in each condition (mean ± SD, 2.7 ± 1.3). Depolarization also occurred during facilitation in Yeh et al. (1997) and during inhibition produced by exposures, comparable with our FAST, HIGH conditions (Vu and Krasne, 1993).
DISCUSSION
These experiments have uncovered two distinct forms of serotonergic modulation affecting the LGs in socially isolated crayfish, a facilitation that is produced by low concentrations of 5-HT (“low-threshold facilitation”) and an inhibition that is produced by high 5-HT concentrations (“high-threshold inhibition”), both of which are associated with depolarization of the LGs.
The fact that 5-HT effects were generally more pronounced for the β component than for the monosynaptic α component might indicate that the effects of 5-HT on the β component are in part attributable to modulation of the first-order synapse of the disynaptic pathway. However, inhibition of the α component was statistically significant at both 10 and 45–60 min after rapid introduction of a high concentration of 5-HT, and facilitation of the monosynaptic response was statistically reliable during exposure to low concentrations of 5-HT. Reliable facilitation and inhibition of monosynaptic (α) input to the LGs have also been seen in previous studies (Glanzman and Krasne, 1983; Vu and Krasne, 1993; Yeh et al., 1996, 1997). Additionally, the fact that 5-HT altered the membrane potential of the LGs in both this and previous studies and that LG input conductance increased in association with the inhibition (Vu and Krasne, 1993) and decreased in association with the facilitation (Yeh et al., 1997) supports the view that both facilitatory and inhibitory effects of 5-HT in isolates are attributable at least in part to modulation directly at the level of the LG and/or synapses directly on it.
The same SLOW–LONG–HIGH application of serotonin that here and in previous studies (Yeh et al., 1996, 1997) produced facilitation in social isolates produces inhibition of the response of the LG in social subordinates (Yeh et al., 1997). Because the inhibition studied in subordinates, unlike the high-threshold inhibition studied here, is associated with LG hyperpolarization, it appears to be a distinct type of serotonergic modulation of LG, different from either of those observed here.
The present experiments also add to the complexity of serotonergic modulation of LG in that directly opposite effects of serotonin are obtained not only in animals with different social histories but also in animals of a given history when different patterns of serotonin application are used. It remains to be determined whether the effect of serotonin on the LGs in subordinates is also sensitive to the pattern of application.
Possible mechanisms
The cellular mechanisms that cause the modulatory effects of serotonin to depend on the concentration, application rate, and duration of exposure are unknown, but some preliminary conclusions may be drawn. The results shown here suggest that 5-HT exposure can activate two parallel intracellular signaling pathways, a lower-threshold facilitatory pathway, and a higher-threshold inhibitory pathway. These pathways might be activated by either different 5-HT receptors or different levels of a common initial second messenger (as in Artola and Singer, 1993; Lisman, 1994; Bear, 1995).
The production of inhibition when higher concentrations of 5-HT are introduced rapidly but facilitation when the same concentrations are reached slowly suggests that intracellular signals generating one form of modulation may interfere with early steps involved in producing the other, as shown in Figure4A1 . If such mutual suppression operated, then whichever modulatory pathway became active first would prevent development of the other form of modulation and, thus, would dominate. With gradual application of 5-HT, the lower-threshold facilitatory pathway would dominate because it would be engaged first. On the other hand, if the high-threshold inhibitory pathway had a faster response time than the facilitatory signaling pathway, then sudden increases of 5-HT concentration to high levels would cause inhibition rather than facilitation. This is by no means the only possible explanation of the application rate sensitivity we observed, but it was the most plausible one we could think of.
Serotonergic facilitation that lasts for hours after prolonged but not after brief 5-HT exposures is reminiscent of a similar effect at sensorimotor synapses of Aplysia. There, hours long facilitation can result from either translation-dependent (but transcription-independent) processes (Ghirardi et al., 1995;Mauelshagen et al., 1996, 1998) or entirely post-translational processes (Ghirardi et al., 1995). The exact mechanisms operating either in Aplysia or here are unknown, and there are many possibilities. For example, such properties can in principle arise from mutual facilitation between signaling pathways, as illustrated by modeling of the interactive properties of mitogen-activated protein (MAP) kinase and protein kinase C (Bhalla and Iyengar, 1999). Another possibility is that the process that normally destroys facilitatory precursors gradually inactivates and allows the remaining precursors and resultant facilitation to persist, as shown in Figure 4B. The mechanisms by which G-protein-coupled receptors adapt (Ferguson and Caron, 1998; Lefkowitz, 1998) provides a possible mechanism, although at a shorter time scale, for such inactivation.
Ability of mechanisms to account for findings
To show that signaling pathway interactions really could cause the phenomena we observed, we constructed a specific computational model (Fig. 5A) that combined the mutual suppression and the inactivation processes just discussed; the quantitative details of the model are given in . The model consists of interacting facilitatory (F) and inhibitory (I) pathways activated by serotonin (S). Each pathway has three reaction steps in which an initial product (Fo orIo) increases as a sigmoidal function of log S and is converted to secondary and tertiary products (F1,F2, orI1,I2). Products increase at a rate proportional to the amount of precursor (Fig. 5A,processes 3, 7, 10, 11) and are broken down at a rate proportional to their own amount (Fig.5A, processes 5, 9, 13,14), and where necessary, feedback inhibition was used to avoid run-away growth of products (Fig. 5A,processes 4, 8). Whichever form of modulation becomes established first, be it facilitation or inhibition, dominates because product F2 suppresses the production of I2 and conversely (Fig.5A, process 12; compare with Fig.4A). Facilitation persists after extended serotonin exposure because breakdown of F1 (Fig.5A, process 5) inactivates (Fig. 5A,process 6; compare with Fig. 4B). The final products (F2 andI2) determine multiplicative factors that adjust EPSP amplitude; the facilitatory multiplier was constructed to increase the EPSP by a percentage proportional to the amount of facilitatory product and correspondingly for the inhibitory multiplier and the final inhibitory product. EPSP amplitudes predicted by the model under the four different conditions of 5-HT application are shown in Figure 5B.
The FAST–SHORT–HIGH 5-HT stimulus (Fig.5B1 ) is inhibitory because the early, rapid response of the inhibitory pathway suppresses production ofF2. Facilitation is not activated long enough for process 5 (Fig. 5A) to inactivate; therefore,F1 does not become self-sustaining and consequently facilitation fails to develop during wash.
The gradual increase in 5-HT concentration during the SLOW–LONG–HIGH condition (Fig. 5B2 ) promotes facilitation because the low-threshold facilitatory response (F2) develops first andF2 laterally inhibits the production of I2.F1 becomes self-sustaining because its breakdown inactivates; thus, facilitation persists during wash. The trend in the biological data for facilitation to diminish after extended exposure to 5-HT and to rise further during wash was generated by introducing feedforward inhibition of the effects ofF2 byI1 (Fig. 5A, process 15).
The FAST–LONG–HIGH stimulus (Fig. 5B3 ) causes inhibition for the same reasons as does the FAST–SHORT–HIGH stimulus. F1 becomes self-sustaining because F1 is elevated long enough for the breakdown reaction (Fig. 5A, process 5) to inactivate. Therefore, F2 is produced once inhibition of its production byI2 (Fig. 5A, process 12) ceases, with the consequence that facilitation becomes manifest during washout of 5-HT.
The FAST–LONG–LOW stimulus (Fig. 5B4) activates the lower-threshold facilitatory pathway for long enough to result in self-sustaining elevation of F1, but it fails to activate the higher-threshold inhibitory pathway. Thus, persistent facilitation is produced.
The signaling pathways mediating the effects of serotonin on the LG are not yet known. Thus, attempts to suggest specific signaling pathways and interactions that could implement this model, which is itself only one of a number that could reproduce our findings, would be premature. However, it should be pointed out that the model is not unrealistic in terms of types of mechanisms that are known to operate in cellular signaling pathways. Thus, F1 (orI1) in the model might plausibly correspond to a second messenger, such as cAMP or diacylglycerol, and F2 (orI2) to the activated form of the associated protein kinase of the second messenger. The activated kinases might then alter membrane conductances by directly phosphorylating channel proteins, the decreased conductance and depolarization associated with low-threshold facilitation being consistent with closure of a K+ channel and the increased conductance and depolarization associated with high-threshold inhibition being consistent with opening of a Cl+ channel. The suppression ofF2 production byI2 would then result if the activated kinase corresponding to I2 were to inhibit the activation of the kinase corresponding toF2 by phosphorylating suitable sites on its regulatory region and conversely (as in inhibition of MAP kinase and CaM kinase kinase activation by PKA) (D'Angelo et al., 1997;Wayman et al., 1997; Soderling, 1999). TheF1 persistence postulated in the model could be attributable to inactivation of an enzymatic reaction breaking down the second messenger (see above) or to some other mechanism leading to prolonged elevation of second-messenger levels such as those responsible for prolonged elevation of DAG in some systems (Nishizuka, 1995). Given this set of proposals, feedforward inhibition (Fig. 5A, process 15) would have to be explained by postulating that the second messenger corresponding toI1 causes a reduction in the extent to which the phosphorylation caused by facilitatory kinase closes the K+ channel. Because some second messengers can directly gate ion channels (Yau, 1994), this effect might be direct.
Alternatively, F1 andI1 could correspond to two different activated protein kinases and F2 andI2 to the sets of phosphorylated proteins that are the targets of the kinases. Then, if the activated kinase corresponding to I1 were to inhibit the catalytic action of the activated kinase corresponding toF2 by phosphorylating it, and conversely, the mutual inhibition postulated by the model would be accomplished. The persistence of F1would here be equivalent to the sustained activation of the kinase to which it corresponds; sustained activation of PKA in fact occurs inAplysia after suitably extended periods of serotonin application (Muller and Carew, 1998). Under this set of assumptions, feedforward inhibition would have to be explained by assuming that the activated kinase corresponding to I1reduces the extent to which the phosphorylation of the K+ channel catalyzed by the facilitatory kinase effectively closes the K+ channel; the I1 kinase could do this by directly phosphorylating the K+ channel at a suitable regulatory site.
More specific and more complete hypotheses must await identification of the signaling pathways recruited in the LGs by 5-HT. However, whatever the actual mechanisms, our simulations show that the dependence of the effect of 5-HT on dose, rate, and duration of 5-HT application may in principle be understood in terms of complex interactions between second messengers and enzymes in intracellular signaling systems. Interactions of this kind provide a capability for logical operations, memory, and temporal pattern generation based on intracellular biochemical networks rather than on multicellular neural networks (Katz, 1999; Weng et al., 1999).
Functional importance
These different application regimens may mimic the different ways that naturally released 5-HT can reach the LG neuron. A serotonergic neuron has been found that projects caudally along the dorsal aspect of the LGs and has terminal varicosities in apparent contact (at the light microscopic level) with the initial dendritic segment of the LG in each abdominal ganglion (Yeh et al., 1997). Activation of this serotonergic neuron could cause the concentration of serotonin in the vicinity of LG to rise quickly to high levels, mimicking the FAST–HIGH application regimen. Serotonin is also a hormone in the blood of crayfish and lobsters (Beltz and Kravitz, 1983); the lack of a blood–brain barrier allows serotonin in the blood to reach the LG, but it presumably does so more slowly and at lower concentrations than with synaptic release, perhaps mimicking the SLOW and LOW application regimens of this study.
The functional significance of serotonergic modulation of LG excitation is not yet clear. However, because injected serotonin seems to have effects on social behavior (Huber et al., 1997) and because its effects on the LGs are social experience dependent, it seems likely that the modulation is important during agonistic interactions. There also is evidence for both increases in medial giant-mediated escape (Herberholz et al., 2001) and decreases of lateral giant-mediated escape (Krasne et al., 1997) during social interactions of freely behaving crayfish, although a role for serotonin in such modulation has not been established.
The factors determining 5-HT exposure pattern in crayfish are not unique. Cortical targets located close to the release sites of serotonergic neurons projecting from the raphe nuclei might be expected to experience potentially large, rapidly changing levels of 5-HT exposure, whereas more distant targets would experience smaller, slower changes (Stamford et al., 2000). If similar to crayfish, these proximal and distal targets might be modulated very differently. Understanding how intracellular signaling systems are tailored to respond to different aspects of the 5-HT signal may guide both the study of the cognitive and behavioral responses to natural neuromodulation and the design of drug therapies.
Appendix
This appendix provides a mathematical characterization of the model described in Discussion and diagramed in Figure 5A. Variable names correspond to those of the figure, and equation numbers correspond to the numbered processes of the figure.
Serotonin at concentration S produces initial intracellular facilitatory and inhibitory factors,F0 andI0, that are sigmoidal functions of log S. Specifically, Equation 1where A = 2, logS0 = −8, andz0 is chosen so thatF0 = 0 when log S = −10,
and Equation 2where A = 2, logS0 = −5, andz0 is chosen so thatI0 = 0 when log S = −10.
F1 is produced at a rate proportional to F0 but is attenuated by a factor,repiF, because of endpoint inhibition, which is constructed to keepF1 from exceeding unity even when breakdown of F1 is totally inactivated (see below). Thus, Equation 3where Equation 4F1 breaks down at a rate proportional to its concentration, but the rate of degradation is attenuated by an inactivation factor,ri, that declines as breakdown occurs. This inactivation of F1 breakdown is responsible for the persistence of F1when 5-HT is removed after long exposures. Thus, Equation 5where Equation 6Taking into account both production and breakdown, Production and breakdown of I1are computed similarly exceptthat breakdown ofI1 does not inactivate. Thus, Equation 7where endpoint inhibition is given by Equation 8which prevents I1 from exceeding unity, and Equation 9Taking into account both production and breakdown, F2 and I2 are each produced at rates proportional to their precursors, but production of each intermediary is inhibited by the other (mutual inhibition,rmi). Thus, Equation 10and Equation 11where the mutual inhibition attenuation factor is given by Equation 12with the argument, x, being eitherI2 orF2.
As above, breakdown of F2 andI2 are given by Equation 13 Equation 14Hence, Finally, EPSP amplitudes are assumed to be changed by factors proportional to final facilitatory and inhibitory products. Thus, However, in approximately half of our experiments under the SLOW–LONG–HIGH condition and in the mean curves of Figure 2, the elevated β component of the EPSP fell somewhat as 5-HT concentration increased and then grew again as 5-HT was washed out. Although this effect was not statistically reliable, it was sufficiently common (and clear in some individual experiments) that we wanted our model to be able to emulate it. It could not be explained by production of small amounts of I2 as the slowly rising 5-HT concentration grew, because mutual inhibition necessarily drivesI2 to zero when 5-HT concentration rises slowly. Therefore, to account for this feature of the data, we assumed that I1 attenuatesF2-caused facilitation by a factor,rffi (feedforward inhibition) via the dashed pathway of Figure 5. Thus, where Equation 15Simulations were done with this feedforward inhibition operative. When it is omitted, the prewash dip and postwash rise of EPSP amplitude seen in Figure 5B2 do not occur.
For computations, 4000 updates were taken to correspond to 30 min of real time or 0.45 sec per update. For HIGH conditions, log Swas set to −3, and for LOW to −7. FAST applications were simulated by steps of log S from −10 to final concentration, whereas for SLOW application log S was started at −10 and altered over time according to Δlog S/Δt = 0.008 (logSfinal − log S). LONG was equal to 4000 iterations and SHORT to 1000, corresponding nominally to 30 and 7.5 min of 5-HT application, respectively. Parameters used in simulations were as follows:
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants NS8108 (to F.B.K.) and NS26547 (to D.H.E.). We thank Ronald Harris-Warrick for helpful discussions and Paul Katz for suggestions on an early version of this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Frank Krasne, Department of Psychology, University of California, 1285 Franz Hall, Box 951963, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563. E-mail: krasne{at}psych.ucla.edu.