Abstract
Clinical and experimental findings have implicated brain α2-adrenoceptors in the regulation of many physiological functions, including sexual activity and stress-related behavior. However, which subtypes of the three α2-adrenoceptors that have now been cloned (α2A, α2B, and α2C) are involved in these controls have yet to be established. Here, we investigated the contribution of α2A-adrenoceptors of the locus ceruleus, the principal source of brain noradrenaline, to exploratory and sexual behaviors. Using administration of antisense oligodeoxynucleotide to inhibit the receptor expression, we found that reductions in brainstem α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA levels and α2-adrenoceptor densities induced by antisense treatment were not accompanied by any changes in the major characteristics of male sexual activity, such as mount latencies and numbers of mounts. However, in sexual behavior tests, antisense-treated male rats had decreased numbers of rearings and thus have higher percentages of behaviors positively correlated with sexual activity. Besides, antisense-treated animals had decreased anxiety in plus-maze tests. The data demonstrate that inhibition of α2A-adrenoceptor expression in the region of the locus ceruleus has an anxiolytic-like effect and facilitates male's attention to female in sexual behavior test.
Pharmacological studies have implicated brain α2-adrenoceptors in the regulation of male sexual behavior (for review, see Bancroft, 1995; Rampin, 1999). Administration of the receptor agonists have been shown to inhibit copulatory activity in male rats (Clark et al., 1985a; Clark, 1991; Benelli et al., 1993), whereas the receptor antagonists enhance their sexual motivation (Clark and Smith, 1984; Clark et al., 1985a,b; Smith et al., 1987; Peters et al., 1988; Sala et al., 1990; Koskinen et al., 1991; Benelli et al., 1993; Tallentire et al., 1996; Spedding et al., 1998). α2-Adrenoceptors have been subdivided into three distinct subtypes: α2A, α2B, and α2C (Bylund et al., 1994). However, which subtypes of the receptors are involved in the control of male sexual behavior have yet to be established.
It has been reported that systemic administration of α2-adrenoceptor antagonists that facilitate sexual behavior are accompanied by an increased noradrenergic transmission in brain regions that receive noradrenergic innervation (Dennis et al., 1987; Szemeredi et al., 1991;Gobert et al., 1997). A majority of noradrenergic neurons are located in the locus ceruleus that project to the cortex and to most subcortical areas. Autoreceptor control of noradrenaline release in the locus ceruleus is mediated by α2A-adrenoceptors (Callado and Stamford, 1999). In the present study, the involvement of the α2A-adrenoceptors of the locus ceruleus in the regulation of male sexual behavior was explored. For this, we examined possible alterations in sexual behavior in sham-operated and castrated male rats after injections of an antisense oligodeoxynucleotide complementary to the α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA into the region of the locus ceruleus. In addition to sexual behavior, anxiety of the animals in the elevated plus-maze test was evaluated after administration of this antisense because the changes in locus ceruleus noradrenergic function appear to influence anxious behavior (Bremner et al., 1996a,b). We also measured the levels of α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA, binding of α2- and β-adrenoceptors, and concentrations of noradrenaline and dopamine in the brain to estimate the specificity of antisense effect on the expression of α2A-adrenoceptors.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals and experimental procedures. Adult male Wistar rats (225–240 gm) were housed under conditions of natural illumination with food and water available ad libitum. All animal use procedures conformed to international European ethical standards (86/609-EEC) and the Russian national instructions for the care and use of laboratory animals. Animals were either bilaterally castrated or underwent a sham operation under ether anesthesia.
Twenty-four days after the surgery, a steel cannula was implanted in the vicinity of the locus ceruleus (9.5 mm caudal to bregma and 5.9 mm below the skull) under Nembutal anesthesia (40 mg/kg, i.p.). Both sham-operated and castrated groups were then subdivided into three subgroups (each subgroup consisted of six animals). Four days after insertion of the cannula, rats of the first subgroups were injected three times continuously for 3 d with 1 nmol with 5μl per day antisense phosphorotioate oligodeoxynucleotide to α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA. The antisense was targeted to the area that bridges the initiation codon (from −11 to + 7, 5′-agcccatgggcgcaaagc-3′). Animals of the two control subgroups received infusions of a phosphorotioate-modified oligodeoxynucleotide containing the same nucleotides as antisense, but in random sequence (5′-gacgacccagtgagcacg-3′), or saline. The sequences of antisense and random have relatively low homology with any of the other known mammalian sequences found in the GenBank database.
Behavioral tests were conducted on days 2 and 3 of the infusion. All animals were decapitated 24 hr after the last injection. Frontal cortex, hippocampus, and brainstem were rapidly dissected out on a cooled plate and frozen in liquid nitrogen. Frontal cortex sample included tissue sections of 1.5-mm-thick cut from the upper surface of the frontal half of the hemispheres. Brainstem sample included pons and medulla oblongata. The levels of α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA (reverse transcription-PCR), numbers of α2- and β-adrenoceptors (radioligand binding of3H-clonidine and3H-dihydroalprenolol), and contents of noradrenaline and dopamine were determined in these regions.
Elevated plus-maze tests. Animals were tested on the elevated plus-maze tests 6 hr after the second injections of drugs between 3:00 P.M. and 4:00 P.M. At the start of the test, rats were placed individually on the center square facing an open arm. During the 5 min test period, the number of entries into both open and closed arms separately and number of rearings were recorded.
Male sexual behavior tests. Sexual behavior tests were conducted 2 hr after the onset of darkness on day 3 of the injections. A receptive female was presented to the male for 20 min. The following measurements were taken: (1) mount latency; (2) number of mounts; (3) number of rearings; (4) number of genital sniffings; (5) number of groomings; and (6) percent of behaviors positively correlated with sexual activity, which was calculated by expressing such behaviors (i.e., number of mounts plus number of groomings) as a percent of the total of scored behaviors (number of mounts plus number of groomings plus number of rearings plus number of genital sniffings).
Analysis of α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA. Total cellular mRNA (2 μm) from the brainstem was isolated by a single step acid guanidinium-phenol-chloroform extraction method (Chomczynski and Sacchi, 1987). Two micrograms of total mRNA were used as template for the first-strand cDNA synthesis in 20 μl reaction volume containing 67 mm Tris-HCl, pH 8.8, 17 mm KCl, 1 mmMnCl2, and 1 mm each of dNTP, 100 ng of oligo-dT primer, 2.5% glycerol, and 5 U of TET-z DNA polymerase, which has reverse transcriptase activity in abundance of Mn2+. Reaction temperatures were 25°C for 10 min for annealing of primer, and 42°C for 1 hr for extension of primers. After that, 5 μl of reaction mixture was increased to 50 μl with 45 μl of PCR mix (67 mm Tris-HCl, pH 8.0, 2.5 mmMnCl2, 0.01 m2-mercaptoethanol, 0.01% Tween 20, 0.2 mm of forward and reverse for α2A-adrenoceptor or β-actin PCR primers, and 2.5 U of Tag polymerase). Oligonucleotide primers were designed from sequences of cytoplasmic β-actin and α2A-adrenoceptor, which are presented in the GenBank database. Oligonucleotides for α2A-adrenoceptor forward primer sequence were 5′-tgcgagatcaacgaccagaag-3′ and reverse-5′-cacgaacgtgaagcgcttctc-3′. The expected size of the amplified fragment was 564 bp. For β-actin, primers were 5′-tccctcatgccatcctgcgt-3′ and 5′-ggaacctctcattgccgata-3′ to produce a 255 bp fragment. Amplifications were performed in a programmable thermal cycler with an initial template denaturation at 95°C for 3 min, annealing at 52°C for 1.5 min, and extension of primers for 2 min at 70°C. This first cycle was followed by 40 amplification cycles of denaturation at 95°C for 0.8 min, annealing at 52°C for 0.8 min, and extension at 70°C for 1 min. The final cycle lasted 10 min at 70°C. In all experiments, the presence of possible contaminants was checked by control reaction in which amplification was performed on samples in which reverse transcriptase was omitted from the reverse transcription reaction mixture. The amplification products were separated on ethidium bromide-stained 1.5% agarose gel. Gene expression level of α2A-adrenoceptors was quantified relatively to β-actin by scanning densitometer (Biodoc II video documentation system; Biometra GmbH, Gottingen, Germany).
Analysis of α2- and β-adrenoceptor binding sites. Brain tissues were homogenized in 20 vol of ice-cold 50 mmTris-HCl buffer, pH 7.7, and centrifuged at 20,000 × gfor 15 min. The pellets were rehomogenized in another portion of the buffer and then centrifuged again. The final pellets were resuspended in 140 vol of the same buffer.3H-Clonidine (3.2 nm; 23.2 Ci/mmol), imidazoline with α2-adrenoceptor agonist specificity, or 1.6 nm3H-dihydroalprenolol (3H-DHA) (75 Ci/mmol), the β-adrenoceptor antagonist, were added to 0.8 ml of membrane suspensions, and the samples in final volumes of 1 ml were incubated: for 3H-clonidine binding, 30 min at 25°C in the absence (total binding) or presence (nonspecific binding) of 100 μm noradrenaline (Koch-Light, Haverhill, UK); for 3H-DHA binding, 20 min at 23°C with or without 10 μm propranolol (Sigma, Poole, UK). The reactions were terminated by rapid filtration under a vacuum through glass fiber filters (GF/B; Whatman, Maidstone, UK). Filters were immediately washed three times with 5 ml of ice-cold buffer, and radioactivity was measured in a Delta-300 liquid scintillation counter. Specific binding was determined by subtracting nonspecific binding from total binding and expressed as femtomoles of radioligand bound per milligram of protein (Shishkina and Naumenko, 1995). Determination of protein was performed by the method of Lowry et al. (1951).
Neurochemical assays. The concentrations of noradrenaline and dopamine were determined fluorimetrically (Jacobowitz and Richardson, 1978).
Statistics. Data were analyzed by using two-way ANOVA for effect of castration and treatment. One-way ANOVA was used for treatment effect within each sham-operated or castrated group. Significant differences were identified by Tukey's post hoc t test.
RESULTS
Elevated plus maze
Castration of male rats significantly elevated the numbers of open-arm entries compared with that in sham-operated animals (F(1,30) = 18.150; p< 0.001) (Fig. 1). Treatment of the rats with antisense to α2A-adrenoceptors increased the numbers of open-arm entries compared with random and saline controls (F(2,30) = 12.008; p< 0.001). The effect of treatment was significant for both sham-operated (F(2,15) = 8.903;p < 0.01) and castrated (F(2,15) = 5.572; p < 0.05) rats.
Numbers of entries in the open arms of the plus maze (A), total arm entries (B), and rearings (C) made by sham-operated (SHAM) and castrated (CASTR) males after injections of antisense oligodeoxynucleotide to the α2A-adrenoceptors, oligodeoxynucleotide of a random sequence, or saline into the region of the locus ceruleus. *p < 0.05 versus saline and random.
There were no significant differences among groups in total number of arm entries (castration, F(1,30) = 2.148, NS; treatment, F(2,30) = 0.393, NS) (Fig. 1).
Numbers of rearings (Fig. 1) were significantly elevated with castration (F(1,30) = 4.489;p < 0.05). Antisense treatment had no effect on the numbers of rearings in these tests (F(2,30) = 1.608, NS).
Male sexual behavior tests
Castration of male rats produced a suppression of copulatory behavior as evidenced by drastic increases in mount latencies (F(1,30) = 19.736; p< 0.001) and decreases in numbers of mounts (F(1,30) = 21.357; p< 0.001) (Fig. 2). Antisense treatment did not affect the mount latencies (F(2,30) = 0.890, NS) or numbers of mounts (F(2,30) = 0.154, NS). Although antisense-treated castrated males had shorter mount latencies and greater numbers of mounts than did the control castrated males, none of the differences reached statistical significance.
Mount latencies (A), numbers of mounts (B), rearings (C), and percent of behaviors positively correlated with male sexual activity (number of mounts plus number of groomings) (D) shown by sham-operated (SHAM) and castrated (CASTR) males after injections of antisense oligodeoxynucleotide to the α2A-adrenoceptors, oligodeoxynucleotide of a random sequence, or saline into the region of the locus ceruleus. *p < 0.05 versus saline and random.
Castration of animals significantly increase the number of rearings (F(1,30) = 10.968;p < 0.01) (Fig. 2) and genital sniffings (F(1,30) = 7.134; p < 0.001) (data not shown) in sexual behavior tests. Administration of antisense significantly reduced both rearing (F(2,30) = 10. 416; p< 0.001) and sniffing (F(2,30) = 3.43; p < 0.05) numbers. Numbers of groomings that were significantly reduced after castration (F(1,30) = 19.833; p< 0.001) were not influenced by antisense treatment (F(2,30) = 0.062, NS) (data not shown).
The percent of the behaviors positively correlated with sexual activity (number of mounts plus number of groomings) were higher in sham-operated animals than in castrated males (F(1,30) = 28.100; p< 0.001) (Fig. 2). There were marginal elevations in the percentages of these behaviors after antisense treatment (F(2,30) = 3.208; p < 0.06). The elevation reached significance in castrated rats (F(2,15) = 3.770; p < 0.05).
Expression of α2A-adrenoceptors in the brainstem
The levels of α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA in the brainstem (Fig.3) were increased by castration (F(1,12) = 8.001; p < 0.05) and decreased by administration of antisense into the region of locus ceruleus (F(2,12) = 17.301;p < 0.001).
Levels of α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA (A) and 3H-clonidine binding (B) in the brainstem of sham-operated (SHAM) and castrated (CASTR) males after injections of antisense oligodeoxynucleotide to the α2A-adrenoceptors, oligodeoxynucleotide of a random sequence, or saline into the region of the locus ceruleus. *p < 0.01 versus saline and random.
The densities of brainstem α2-adrenoceptors (Fig. 3), assessed by 3H-clonidine binding, were not changed in castrated males (F(1,30) = 0.036, NS). Marginally significant decreases in3H-clonidine binding were found in this brain region after antisense treatment (F(2,30) = 3.075; p < 0.07).
3H-clonidine binding in the frontal cortex and hippocampus
The densities of 3H-clonidine binding sites in the frontal cortex were significantly increased after castration (F(1,30) = 5.471;p < 0.05) (Fig. 4). Administration of antisense into the region of the locus ceruleus did not affect the densities of binding sites in the frontal cortex (F(2,30) = 0.760, NS).
Binding of 3H-clonidine in the frontal cortex (A) and hippocampus (B) of sham-operated (SHAM) and castrated (CASTR) males after injections of antisense oligodeoxynucleotide to the α2A-adrenoceptors, oligodeoxynucleotide of a random sequence, or saline into the region of the locus ceruleus. *p < 0.05 versus saline and random.
In the hippocampus (Fig. 4), castration did not modify specific3H-clonidine binding (F(1,30) = 0.042, NS). Treatment with antisense induced a significant reduction in the binding site densities in this brain region (F(2,30) = 6.575;p < 0.01).
Concentrations of noradrenaline and dopamine, and 3H-DHA binding in the frontal cortex
No significant changes were found in the concentrations of noradrenaline in the frontal cortex of rats after castration (F(1,30) = 1.519, NS). Antisense treatment caused a significant decrease in cortical noradrenaline concentrations (F(2,30) = 5.679;p < 0.01) (Fig. 5).
Cortical noradrenaline (A) and 3H-DHA binding (B) in sham-operated (SHAM) and castrated (CASTR) males after injections of antisense oligodeoxynucleotide to the α2A-adrenoceptors, oligodeoxynucleotide of a random sequence, or saline into the region of the locus ceruleus. *p < 0.05 versus random.
There was no effect of castration (F(1,30) = 0.018, NS) or antisense treatment (F(2,30) = 0.028, NS) on the concentrations of dopamine in the frontal cortex (data not shown).
A marginally significant elevation in the binding of3H-DHA to cortical β-adrenoceptors (Fig.5) was seen for the castrated animals (F(1,30) = 3.293; p < 0.08). Antisense treatment led to a marginally significant reduction in3H-DHA binding in the frontal cortex (F(2,30) = 3.225; p < 0.06).
DISCUSSION
The present results showed that administration of antisense to α2A-adrenoceptors into the region of locus ceruleus decreased the level of α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA and the number of α2-adrenoceptors in the brainstem. However, the reduction of the receptor density was only marginal, whereas that of mRNA level was highly significant. One possible reason for this difference is the ability of3H-clonidine to label nonadrenoceptor imidazoline binding sites, in addition to α2-adrenoceptors (Ernsberger et al., 1987). A similar 20–30% decrement in binding site densities in brain regions close to the side of the intracerebroventricular infusion of antisense to α2D-adrenoceptors has been reported by Robinson et al. (1999) with the other non-subtype selective radioligand 3H-RX821002.
Treatment with antisense caused reduction in expression of brainstem α2-adrenoceptors in both sham-operated and castrated rats. However, castration itself affected the receptor expression in brain regions. The levels of α2-adrenoceptor mRNA in the brainstem and the densities of α2-adrenoceptors in the frontal cortex were increased in castrated animals 4 weeks after the operation compared with sham-operated males. This is consistent with the previously published report that demonstrated increases in 3H-clonidine binding in the cortex 2 and 3 weeks after castration (Shishkina et al., 1997). Although the castrated group started out with a higher level of α2A-adrenoceptor mRNA, antisense treatment lowered it to the same level as the sham-operated antisense-treated animals. Effects of antisense on behavioral assessments in sexual behavioral tests were also more pronounced in castrated rats.
Male sexual behavior depends on testicular hormones (Damassa et al., 1977; Clark et al., 1988; Shishkina et al., 1993). Results from the present and previous studies indicate that α2-adrenoceptors appear to be sensitive to their regulatory influences (Shishkina and Naumenko, 1995; Shishkina et al., 1997). These observations suggest that the stimulatory effects of testosterone on sexual behavior may be attributable to alterations in brain α2-adrenoceptor density. Moreover, ability of the α2-adrenoceptor antagonist yohimbine to facilitate sexual activity in long-term castrated male rats indicate that the α2-adrenergic regulation of sexual behavior is downstream from the effect of testosterone (Clark et al., 1985b).
The locus ceruleus noradrenergic cell group is involved in the formation of the arousal system (Marrocco et al., 1994). However, α2A-adrenoceptors of the locus ceruleus are not important in the control of sexual arousal. Reduction of the α2A-adrenoceptor expression induced by antisense treatment was not accompanied by changes in major characteristics of male sexual activity, such as mount latency and number of mounts. Thus, it seems likely that stimulatory effects of systemic injection of α2-adrenoceptor antagonists on male sexual behavior (Clark et al., 1984, 1985a,b; Smith et al., 1987;Peters et al., 1988; Sala et al., 1990; Koskinen et al., 1991; Benelli et al., 1993; Tallentire et al., 1996; Spedding et al., 1998) occur via other brain regions. For example, Clark (1991) has reported that administration of yohimbine into the medial preoptic area attenuated the inhibitory effect of systemically administered clonidine on sexual activity. At the same time, because in sexual behavior tests genital sniffing in castrated males and rearing in rats of both sham-operated and castrated groups were declined after antisense treatment, percent of behaviors positively correlated with sexual activity (number of mounts plus number of groomings) was increased in these animals. It is important to note that the inhibitory effect of antisense on rearing is specific for the sexual behavior test because, in the other behavioral test, the plus maze, number of rearings did not differ between control and antisense-treated rats. Together, these findings suggest that antisense treatment resulted in a facilitatory effect on male's attention to female but, in general, did not affect copulatory behavior in males.
Antisense administration also modified the function of the noradrenergic system in the target brain regions of the locus ceruleus, such as the frontal cortex and hippocampus. Thus, a significant fall in α2-adrenoceptor densities in the hippocampus was observed after the treatment. This is consistent with the finding that the majority of hippocampal α2A-adrenoceptors appear to be associated with fine axons and presynaptically located (Milner et al., 1998). In contrast to the hippocampus, we did not observe any effect of antisense on the α2-adrenoceptor densities in the frontal cortex. The presence of relatively large number of α2-adrenoceptors located postsynaptically and on noncatecholaminergic terminals (Heal et al., 1993; Aoki et al., 1994; Venkatesan et al., 1996) could mask the possible effect of antisense on cortical receptors, which are imported into this region from the brainstem. In the frontal cortex, concentrations of noradrenaline were decreased in antisense-treated animals. This effect can be explained by an enhancement in neuronal firing and release of neurotransmitter. Such a possibility is supported by the finding that infusion of preferential α2A-adrenoceptor antagonist into the locus ceruleus enhanced release of noradrenaline in the cortex (Mateo and Meana, 1999). Because long-term exposure to agonist can change the expression of different adrenoceptors, the tendency for the decrease in cortical β-adrenoceptor density after antisense treatment may reflect downregulation of this receptor population by continued noradrenaline stimulation (Hosoda et al., 1994, 1995).
Of the brain regions that have been investigated, noradrenergic neurotransmission or α2A-autoreceptors in the frontal cortex may be involved in regulating behaviors in sexual tests by altering the male's reactivity to female stimuli. Thus, adrenergic pathways innervating the cortex play a role in cognito-attentional processes (Marrocco et al., 1994), and it has been shown that male rats with lesions of the cerebral cortex near the midline in the frontal region had very long mount latencies (Agmo and Villalpando, 1995). The involvement of cortical α2-adrenoceptors in sexual behavior is also supported by changes in their expression after castration and copulation (Rago et al., 1992).
The results obtained with the plus maze showed anxiolytic-like effects of long-term castration and antisense administration. Effect of castration may be attributable to the decreased level of testosterone. Thus, it was shown that adult male rats made a lower percentage of entries in the open arms than females. Besides, newborn castration led to an adult male behavioral pattern that resembles that of the female (Lucion et al., 1996). A hypothesis to explain the anxiolytic effect of antisense may be an increase of noradrenergic neurons firing and elevation of synaptic noradrenaline release. This hypothesis is supported by studies showing a decrease in anxiety-related behavior after direct stimulation of the locus ceruleus (Weiss et al., 1994). This is also consistent with the finding that noradrenergic neuronal integrity is required for the anxiolytic-like effects of chronic antidepressant treatment (Fontana et al., 1999). However, we have no comparable data pointing to the α2A-adrenoceptor mechanism whereby novelty leads to anxiety. Pharmacological results reported with α2-adrenoceptor antagonists are quite inconsistent. They span the full range of effects from anxiogenesis (Handley and Mithani, 1984) to anxiolysis (La Marca and Dunn, 1994; Cole et al., 1995). This is discussed in relation to the well known action of the used antagonists on monoaminergic receptors other than α2-adrenoceptors. Nevertheless, our results are consistent with the view that agents with selective antagonism at the α2-adrenoceptor may be anxiolytic, whereas agents with less specificity at this adrenoceptors are not anxiolytic (La Marca and Dunn, 1994; Cole et al., 1995). The divergence in the antagonist effects may be also attributable to the multiplicity of α2-adrenoceptor subtypes and the lack of subtype-specific ligands. The present study provides evidence that specific reduction of the α2A-adrenoceptors in the brainstem exerts anxiolytic-like effects in sham-operated and castrated male rats.
Together, the results of the present study could be also interpreted as that administration of antisense against α2A-adrenoceptors into the region of the locus ceruleus caused anxiolytic-like effect, resulting in an increase in male's attention to the female in the sexual behavior test. This interpretation is supported by the finding that the anxiogenic agent RS-30199 has been shown to fully inhibit the facilitation of male sexual behavior in rats caused by the α2-adrenoceptor antagonist delequamine (Spedding et al., 1998). Moreover, Rowland et al. (1997) found no effect of yohimbine on most aspects of sexual response in sexually functional men. Facilitation of sexual activity under yohimbine in men with erectile problems has been suggested to relate with yohimbic effects on psychological factors that modulate overall sexual response rather than a more selective activation of erectile response.
In conclusion, we have shown that inhibition of α2A-adrenoceptor expression in the region of the locus ceruleus has an anxiolytic-like effect and facilitates male's attention to female in sexual behavior test.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Russian Fund for Basic Research Grants N 98-04-49651 and N 99-04-50022.
Correspondence should be addressed to N. N. Dygalo, Institute of Cytology and Genetics, Novosibirsk 630090, Russia. E-mail:dygalo{at}bionet.nsc.ru.