Abstract
This work investigates how the light responses of salamander bipolar cells adapt to changes in temporal contrast: changes in the depth of the temporal fluctuations in light intensity about the mean. Contrast affected the sensitivity of bipolar cells but not of photoreceptors or horizontal cells, suggesting that adaptation occurred in signal transfer from photoreceptors to bipolars. This suggestion was confirmed by recording from photoreceptor–bipolar pairs and observing a direct dependence of the gain of signal transfer on the contrast of the light input. After an increase in contrast, the onset of adaptation in the bipolar cell had a time constant of 1–2 sec, similar to a fast component of contrast adaptation in the light responses of retinal ganglion cells (Kim and Rieke, 2001). Contrast adaptation was mediated by processes in the dendrites of both on andoff bipolars. The functional properties of adaptation differed for the two bipolar types, however, with contrast having a much more pronounced effect on the kinetics of the responses ofoff cells than on cells.
- contrast gain control
- contrast adaptation
- bipolar cell
- adaptation
- temporal contrast
- retinal signal processing
A general problem sensory neurons face is adjusting their operational range to match the range of the input signals they receive. This is particularly clear for vision, in which the mean light intensity and the contrast (the extent of fluctuations in light intensity about the mean) vary substantially in different visual environments. The visual system handles these differences in input signals by adjusting its sensitivity, or adapting. Adaptation includes mechanisms sensitive to the mean light intensity (for review, see Walraven et al., 1990) and the spatial and temporal contrast (for review, see Shapley, 1997; Meister and Berry, 1999). This work examines the contribution of retinal bipolar cells to temporal contrast adaptation.
Contrast adaptation is widely viewed as a cortical phenomena, and there is good evidence that cortical mechanisms contribute (Albrecht et al., 1984; Ohzawa et al. 1985; Sanchez-Vives et al., 2000). Several studies, however, show that changes in temporal contrast affect the sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells in amphibians and mammals (Sakai et al., 1995; Smirnakis et al., 1997), including primate (Chander and Chichilnisky, 1999, 2001). Indeed, without contrast adaptation, many retinal neurons would be easily saturated because of their high contrast gain (Capovilla et al., 1987; Burkhardt and Fahey, 1998,1999). Thus, the retina makes an important contribution to the ability of the visual to adapt to contrast. We know little, however, about where contrast adaptation occurs in the retina or what mechanisms are responsible.
Retinal contrast adaptation shows a range of spatial and temporal properties: the spatial extent of the signal controlling contrast adaptation differs between on and off ganglion cells (Smirnakis et al., 1997); the onset of adaptation after an increase in contrast has several temporal components (Victor, 1987;Sakai et al., 1995; Smirnakis et al., 1997; Kim and Rieke, 2001); and the strength of contrast adaptation differs between on andoff ganglion cells (Chander and Chichilnisky, 1999; Kim and Rieke, 2001). This diversity in the properties of contrast adaptation in the retinal output suggests a corresponding diversity in both the mechanisms mediating contrast adaptation and in the functional roles of each mechanism.
We found previously that contrast adaptation included contributions from spike generation in retinal ganglion cells and from unidentified sites within the retinal circuitry (Kim and Rieke, 2001). The aim of the present work was to identify the sites of contrast adaptation in the retinal circuitry and the mechanisms responsible. The principal findings described here are as follows: (1) the first site of contrast adaptation in the retina is in the dendrites of bipolar cells; (2) this site contributes to the fast-onset component of contrast adaptation seen in retinal ganglion cells (Kim and Rieke, 2001); and (3) the functional properties of contrast adaptation differ betweenon and off bipolars, with contrast exerting a larger effect on the kinetics of the responses of off cells than on cells.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Recording procedures. All experiments used retinas from larval tiger salamanders (Ambystoma tigrinum; from Charles Sullivan, Nashville, TN). Salamanders were dark adapted overnight, and retinas were isolated under infrared light (Kim and Rieke, 2001) following procedures approved by the Administrative Panel on Laboratory Animal Care at the University of Washington.
Light-evoked current and voltage responses of retinal cells were measured in a slice preparation. A piece of retina ∼1 × 2 mm was embedded in low gelling temperature agar (Sigma, St. Louis, MO), immersed in cold HEPES-buffered Ames medium (Sigma), and sliced in 300-μm-thick sections on a vibrating microtome (Leica, Wetzlar, Germany). Slices were transferred to a recording chamber and held in place with a coarse nylon grid glued to a platinum weighting ring. The chamber was placed on the stage of an upright microscope equipped with an infrared viewing system. Slices were continuously superfused with a bicarbonate Ringer's solution containing (in mm): 110 NaCl, 2 KCl, 30 NaHCO3, 1.5 CaCl2, 1.6 MgCl2, and 10 glucose; pH was 7.4 when equilibrated with 5% CO2–95% O2 and osmolarity was 270–275 mOsm. The volume of the recording chamber was ∼300 μl, and the superfusion rate was 1–2 ml/min. All experiments were at 20–22°C.
Light produced by a light-emitting diode (LED) was focussed on the slice through the bottom of the recording chamber. An LED with a peak output at 470 nm was used to stimulate rods, and one with a peak output at 640 nm was used to stimulate L cones. The light stimuli were spatially uniform and illuminated a circular area 650 μm in diameter centered on the recorded cell. The temporal contrast of the light stimulus was controlled by adding Gaussian fluctuations to the signal controlling the light output of the LED. The Gaussian fluctuations were low-pass filtered at either 10 (for rods) or 30 (for cones) Hz, as noted in the figure legends. The contrast of this stimulus was defined as the SD of the light intensity divided by the mean. Light intensities measured at the preparation are given in the figure legends.
Electrical responses were measured using either perforated-patch or whole-cell recordings and an Axopatch 200B patch-clamp amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA); recording configurations are noted in the figure legends. Patch pipettes were filled with an internal solution containing (in mm): 125 K-aspartate, 10 KCl, 10 HEPES, 5 N-methylglucamine (NMG)–N-hydroxyethylethylenediaminetriacetic acid (HEDTA), 1 CaCl2, 1 ATP, 0.1 GTP, and 0.1 mm calcein, pH was adjusted to 7.2 with NMG-OH (and osmolarity was 260–265 mOsm). For perforated-patch recordings, the pipette solution also contained 1 mg/ml amphotericin-B (solubilized formulation; Sigma), and the pipette tip was filled with amphotericin-free solution. Filled pipettes had resistances of 8–10 MΩ, and the series resistance during recording was 20–40 MΩ. Calcein was included in the pipette solution to permit the morphology of a cell to be visualized under fluorescence at the end of a recording; all bipolar cells reported here had processes in both the inner and outer plexiform layers. on andoff bipolars were distinguished based on the polarity of their responses to 1 sec light increments and decrements (see Fig. 6, insets). In voltage-clamp recordings, bipolar cells were held at −60 mV; in current-clamp recordings, the holding current was between 0 and −50 pA, resulting in a membrane potential near −50 mV. Voltages have not been corrected for junction potentials (approximately −9 to −10 mV for the solutions used).
Data analysis. The effect of contrast on the amplitude and kinetics of the light response of a cell was measured using a static nonlinearity model that provided a relatively simple description of how continuous light inputs were transformed into cellular responses (Sakai et al., 1995; Chichilnisky, 2001). An important aspect of this model is that it separates an instantaneous nonlinearity in the response of a cell (e.g., attributable to saturation or activation of voltage-dependent conductances) from a change in the response characteristics of a cell attributable to adaptation. The model describes the current-to-response transformation as a linear filter followed by a static or time-independent nonlinearity (Fig.1A). Comparing the filter and static nonlinearity for lights of different contrasts proved an effective means of characterizing contrast-dependent changes in the amplitude and kinetics of the light response of a cell. Details of the calculation of the linear filter and static nonlinearity are described by Kim and Rieke (2001).
The linear filter and static nonlinearity were determined from recordings of 5–10 min of the response of a cell to light inputs of a given contrast. Figure 1 shows an example of this analysis for responses of a current-clamped on bipolar cell to a 30% contrast light input. Figure 1B shows the linear filter; convolving this filter with the light input provides the best linear estimate of the voltage response of a cell given the light input (Wiener, 1949; Kim and Rieke, 2001). Thus, the shape of the filter estimates the time course of the response of a cell to a brief light flash at time 0 in the presence of the fluctuating contrast signal. Foron cells such as that in Figure1B, the linear filter measured under current clamp has a positive polarity, reflecting the depolarization produced by an increment in light intensity (Fig. 1B,inset). Systematic differences between the measured voltage and the linear prediction obtained by convolving the light input with the linear filter were used to determine a time-independent nonlinearity in the relationship between light input and the response of a cell: the static nonlinearity in the model. The shape of the static nonlinearity was determined by calculating the average measured response for each value of the linear prediction. Figure 1Cplots the average measured response (y-axis) against the corresponding linear prediction (x-axis). In this cell, as in most bipolars, the nonlinearity was mild and consisted primarily of a gradual decrease in slope for large response amplitudes.
Figure 1D compares a short section of the measured voltage response with the prediction from the linear filter and static nonlinearity in Figure 1, B and C. Although the prediction captures much of the structure in the measured response, there are also clear differences. In principle, these differences could reflect either noise in the measured response or a failure of the model. To distinguish between these possibilities, differences in the individual measured responses to a repeated stimulus were compared with differences between the measured and predicted response. The extent of trial-to-trial fluctuations in the response of a cell was determined by measuring the correlation between the average response and the individual responses to 15–30 repeats of a 20 sec stimulus. In four such experiments, the average correlation was 0.76. This correlation was compared with the average correlation between the response predicted by the static nonlinearity model and the individual measured responses, which was 0.70 in the same four cells. Thus, >90% of the light-dependent structure in the measured response was well predicted by the static nonlinearity model.
The model of Figure 1 was used to study contrast adaptation by comparing linear filters and static nonlinearities measured for light inputs of two contrasts. In all cells analyzed, the effect of contrast could be restricted to changes in the linear filter, greatly simplifying interpretation of the results. For a given contrast, the linear filter and static nonlinearity are unique up to a single scale factor. Thus, both the y-axis scaling of the filter in Figure 1B and the x-axis scaling of the static nonlinearity in Figure 1C can be multiplied by a factor α without changing the prediction of the model, because the rescaling of the filter amplitude is offset by the change in the static nonlinearity. When linear filters and static nonlinearities for two contrasts were compared, α was chosen to produce the best overlap of the static nonlinearities (Chichilnisky, 2001; Kim and Rieke, 2001). For example, Figure 3D–F shows nonlinearities for a cone, horizontal cell, and off bipolar cell measured at 10 and 30% contrast. In each case, α was chosen to cause the static nonlinearities to overlap and thus restrict the effect of contrast to changes in the linear filter. A similar scaling was used each time contrast adaptation was quantified using the static nonlinearity model. Thus, the transformation of light inputs into cellular responses was described as a contrast-dependent linear filter followed by a contrast-independent static nonlinearity.
RESULTS
The experiments described below indicate that signal transfer from rods and cones to bipolar cells provides the first site of contrast adaptation in the retina. The onset and offset of this adaptation were relatively rapid and contributed to a fast-onset component of contrast adaptation in retinal ganglion cells. Contrast adaptation differed both functionally and mechanistically inon and off bipolars.
Bipolar cells provide the first site of contrast adaptation
Two results indicate that the light responses of bipolar cells adapt to the contrast of the light input but that those of photoreceptors and horizontal cells do not. First, after an increase in contrast, the amplitude of the light response of a bipolar immediately increased and then gradually declined, suggesting a time-dependent change in sensitivity induced by the contrast change. Second, the steady-state sensitivity of the light responses of bipolar cells decreased after an increase in contrast. Both effects were small or absent in photoreceptors and horizontal cells.
Response time course after a change in contrast
A signature of adaptation is a change in sensitivity over time after a change in the input signal. Thus, contrast adaptation should cause gradual changes in the amplitude of the response of a cell after a change in contrast. To test for such changes, a randomly fluctuating light stimulus was switched between 10 and 30% contrast every 20 sec while recording the resulting response in a photoreceptor, horizontal cell, or bipolar cell. Figure2A–C shows voltage recordings from a cone (A), horizontal cell (B), and OFF bipolar cell (C) to a single cycle of this alternating contrast signal. The timing of the contrast change is shown in the stimulus trace at the bottomof the figure.
The amplitude of the light responses of photoreceptors and horizontal cells changed quickly after an increase (0 sec) or decrease (20 sec) in contrast and did not show time-dependent structure that would suggest the cells were adapting. The absence of time-dependent structure was captured more clearly by measuring the time-dependent variance across 15–30 cycles of the contrast signal. Independent stimuli were used in each cycle, and thus the variance measured the strength of the response of a cell to the fluctuating stimulus. In both photoreceptors (Fig.2D) and horizontal cells (Fig. 2E), the variance reached a steady-state level within ∼0.2 sec after a change in contrast and maintained this level throughout the 20 sec duration of the contrast signal. A similar lack of time dependence was observed in the responses of four rods, eight cones, and six horizontal cells.
The responses of bipolar cells, on the other hand, did show time-dependent structure. After an increase in contrast, the amplitude (Fig. 2C) and the variance (Fig. 2F) of the response increased rapidly and then declined to a steady level; after a decrease in contrast, the variance fell to an initial minimum and then gradually increased. A similar time dependence was observed in nine bipolar cells. These results suggest that bipolar cells adapt to temporal contrast.
Contrast-dependent changes in sensitivity
The static nonlinearity model (see Materials and Methods) (Fig. 1) was used to determine how contrast affected the gain and kinetics of the responses of photoreceptors, horizontal cells, and bipolar cells. This model predicts the response of a cell to continuous light inputs of a particular contrast by passing the light intensity through a linear filter followed by a time-independent or static nonlinearity (Fig. 1). The linear filter (Fig.3A) estimates the time course of the response of a cell to a brief light flash in the presence of a fluctuating contrast signal. Thus, for the cone in Figure3A, the filter predicts that the cell would respond to a brief light flash at time 0 with a hyperpolarization lasting ∼0.2 sec. The static nonlinearity (Fig. 3D) describes the relationship between the output of this linear filter and the measured response. In general, this relationship is nonlinear, reflecting processes such as saturation or activation of cellular conductances. This model provided a relatively compact description (a single filter and time-independent nonlinearity) of how the cell responded to light inputs of a particular contrast. A key aspect of this model is that it separates instantaneous nonlinearities in the response of a cell from changes in sensitivity attributable to adaptation.
Contrast-dependent changes in the amplitude and kinetics of the response of a cell were measured by comparing filters and nonlinearities for different contrast inputs. High- and low-contrast stimuli were interleaved to ensure that adaptation was reversible. A section of record after each change in contrast was discarded to permit the response of a cell to reach steady state. Figure3A–C shows linear filters measured at 10 and 30% contrast for the cone, horizontal cell, and OFF bipolar cell from Figure 2. Figure 3D–F plots the corresponding static nonlinearities. In each cell, the nonlinearities had similar shapes at high and low contrast and thus did not contribute to contrast-dependent changes in sensitivity (see Materials and Methods). A similar contrast independence of the shape of the static nonlinearity was found in all of the photoreceptors, horizontal cells, and bipolar cells studied. This greatly simplified characterization of contrast adaptation as the effect of contrast on the response of a cell was restricted to changes in the linear filter. In subsequent figures, the static nonlinearities are not shown, and the linear filters at high and low contrast are scaled by a common factor so that the filter at low contrast has unit amplitude.
Linear filters measured at 10 and 30% contrast were nearly identical in cones (Fig. 3A) and horizontal cells (Fig. 3B) but differed considerably in bipolar cells (Fig. 3C). The ratio of the amplitude of the filter at high and low contrast was 0.98 ± 0.04 (mean ± SEM) in four rods, 1.01 ± 0.04 in eight cones, 0.96 ± 0.03 in six horizontal cells, and 0.79 ± 0.02 in 42 bipolar cells. Contrast had a similar effect on the light responses of current- and voltage-clamped bipolar cells (see Fig. 8); thus, in most experiments, cells were voltage clamped to minimize the effects of voltage-activated conductances. Experiments like those illustrated in Figure 3 show that the sensitivity of photoreceptors and horizontal cells changed little after a change in contrast, whereas the sensitivity of bipolar cells changed substantially.
Contrast affects the gain of photoreceptor–bipolar signal transfer
The experiments of Figures 2 and 3 indicate that bipolar cells, but not photoreceptors or horizontal cells, adapt to temporal contrast, suggesting that contrast affects the gain of signal transfer from photoreceptors to bipolar cells. Simultaneous recordings from photoreceptors and bipolar cells provided direct evidence for this conclusion by bypassing the phototransduction process and measuring the gain of signal transfer directly.
The gain of signal transfer was measured by injecting a depolarizing current pulse into a photoreceptor and measuring the resulting postsynaptic response in a bipolar cell. This experiment was repeated in the presence and absence of a 25% contrast light input, and the gain of signal transfer was compared in the two conditions. No attempt was made to disrupt gap junctions, so the bipolar response represents changes in transmitter release from a collection of electrically coupled photoreceptors (Schwartz, 1976; Attwell et al., 1984). A similar spread of signals among coupled photoreceptors occurs under normal conditions in the retina.
Figure 4A shows results from a cone–on bipolar pair in which the cone was current clamped and the bipolar was voltage clamped. Throughout the experiment, the slice was exposed to bright 640 nm light. Changing the holding current of the cone from −50 to +50 pA for 20 msec depolarized the cone and produced a postsynaptic response in the bipolar cell. Sets of 20 such current pulses delivered in the presence and absence of a 25% contrast light input were interleaved. The bottom panelof Figure 4A shows average voltage changes in the cone in response to the current pulse. Responses in the presence and absence of the contrast stimulus are superimposed. Neither the mean cone voltage nor the voltage change produced by the current pulse was affected by the contrast signal. The top panel of Figure4A shows the response of the bipolar to current injected into the cone. The bipolar response was smaller in the presence of the contrast signal than in its absence, indicating a contrast-dependent change in the gain of signal transfer.
Figure 4B shows results from a similar experiment on a rod–off bipolar pair. In this case, the experiment was performed in the presence of 470 nm light, which produced responses in rods but was too dim to elicit responses in cones. The bottom panel of Figure 4B shows the average voltage response in the rod when the holding current was changed from −50 to +50 pA for 100 msec. Responses in the presence and absence of a 25% contrast light input are superimposed. Again, the contrast stimulus had little or no effect on either the mean rod voltage or the voltage response to the current pulse. The top panel in Figure 4B shows the bipolar response to current injected into the rod. As for the cone–bipolar pair, the bipolar response was smaller in the presence of the contrast signal. Contrast also appeared to speed the kinetics of signal transfer from the rod to the off bipolar, as observed for the light responses of off bipolars (see Fig. 6).
A similar contrast dependence of the gain of signal transfer was observed in four cone–bipolar and six rod–bipolar pairs. The contrast dependence of signal transfer could be produced by either presynaptic or postsynaptic mechanisms. Because horizontal cells and bipolar cells both receive direct input from photoreceptors, the lack of contrast adaptation in horizontal cells (Fig. 3B) suggests that the changes in signal transfer are not a property of presynaptic mechanisms in the photoreceptor synaptic terminal but are generated postsynaptically in the bipolar cell. Additional evidence for this conclusion is provided by the experiments of Figure 10 described below.
Properties of contrast adaptation in bipolar cells
Time course of onset and offset
The onset and offset of contrast adaptation in the retina occur on several time scales (Victor, 1987; Sakai et al., 1995; Smirnakis et al., 1997; Kim and Rieke, 2001). For example, the onset of contrast adaptation in salamander retinal ganglion cells includes components with ∼1 and ∼10 sec time constants (Kim and Rieke, 2001). To determine the kinetics of the onset and offset of contrast adaptation in bipolar cells, the contrast was switched between 10 and 30% periodically, and the time-dependent variance of the resulting bipolar response was measured. Figure 5 shows the variance in the current response of a voltage-clamped offbipolar cell for contrast switches every 10 sec. The changes in the variance after increases (t = 0) and decreases (t = 10 sec) in contrast were fit by single exponentials (Fig. 5, smooth curves). The time constants for the onset and offset of contrast adaptation in this cell were 2.1 and 6.2 sec. In nine cells, the onset of contrast adaptation had a time constant of 1.8 ± 0.3 sec (mean ± SEM), and the offset had a time constant of 4.7 ± 0.8 sec. The input currents to ganglion cells exhibited a similar asymmetry between the kinetics of the onset and offset of contrast adaptation (Kim and Rieke, 2001). Bipolar cells, however, showed no evidence for a slower temporal component after an increase in contrast like that seen in ganglion cells. Thus, bipolar cells contribute to the fast-onset component of contrast adaptation observed in the ganglion cells.
on and off bipolars adapt differently to contrast
Contrast adaptation differed in on and offbipolar cells. Although contrast affected the amplitude of the light responses in both types of bipolar cell, the effect of contrast on the response kinetics was pronounced in off bipolars and small or absent in on bipolars. on andoff bipolars were distinguished based on their responses to 1 sec light increments. Under voltage clamp, a light increment generated an outward current in an off bipolar (Fig.6A, inset) and an inward current in an on bipolar (Fig.6B, inset). No attempt was made to divideon and off bipolars into subtypes (Burkhardt and Fahey, 1998; Wu et al., 2000).
Contrast adaptation was measured by presenting 10 and 30% contrast inputs and characterizing the amplitude and kinetics of the light response of a cell using the static nonlinearity model. Figure 6 shows linear filters for an off bipolar (Fig.6A) and an on bipolar (Fig.6B), both measured under voltage clamp. The static nonlinearities did not change with contrast (data not shown), and thus contrast adaptation was restricted to changes in the linear filters. In the off bipolar, increasing the contrast from 10 to 30% decreased the amplitude and the time-to-peak of the filter. In the on bipolar, increasing the contrast from 10 to 30% produced a clear change in amplitude but little or no change in the time-to-peak. Figure 6C summarizes measurements of the amplitude of the filter at high contrast relative to that at low contrast for 17 off bipolars and 25on bipolars, all measured under voltage clamp. Contrast had a slightly larger effect on the amplitude of the light responses of off bipolars thanon bipolars. Figure 6Dsummarizes measurements of the time-to-peak of the linear filter at high contrast relative to that at low contrast. The change in response kinetics was clear in off bipolars and small or absent in on bipolars. A similar asymmetry between the effect of contrast on the responses ofon and off bipolars was observed in current-clamp recordings. This asymmetry contributes to anon–off asymmetry in the light responses of salamander retinal ganglion cells (Chander and Chichilnisky, 1999; Kim and Rieke, 2001) (see Discussion).
Contrast adaptation affects rod- and cone-mediated responses
Contrast affected the amplitude and kinetics of the bipolar responses over a wide range of light intensities. Thus, contrast adaptation acted on signals from both rod and cone photoreceptors. Rod-dominated responses were studied using 470 nm light at mean intensities of 4–40 photons μm−2sec−1, too dim to produce responses in the cones. Cone-dominated responses were studied using 640 nm light at mean intensities of 3,000–80,000 photons μm−2sec−1, light levels sufficient to saturate the rods. Contrast adaptation was studied by delivering 10 and 30% light inputs and measuring steady-state sensitivity using the static nonlinearity model. Figure 7 shows linear filters measured for rod- (Fig. 7A) and cone- (Fig.7B) dominated responses in a voltage-clampedoff bipolar cell. The corresponding static nonlinearities overlapped and hence did not contribute to contrast adaptation (data not shown). Rod-mediated responses were slower than cone-mediated responses, as expected from the slower kinetics of the photoreceptor responses themselves. However, increasing the contrast of the light input from 10 to 30% decreased the amplitude and the time-to-peak of the filter for both rod- and cone-mediated signals. Contrast affected the sensitivity of the bipolar cell responses for mean light levels as low as 4 photons μm−2sec−1 (three cells) and as high as 80,000 photons μm−2sec−1 (six cells). This indicates that contrast adaptation in photoreceptor–bipolar signal transfer is a general property of how signals are processed by the retinal circuitry.
Mechanism of contrast adaptation in bipolar cells
The effect of contrast on the sensitivity of bipolar cells and lack of an effect on the sensitivities of photoreceptors and horizontal cells (Fig. 3) indicates that contrast adaptation is mediated after signals are transferred to the bipolar cell. The experiments described below tested several possible mechanisms: (1) voltage-activated conductances in the bipolar soma (Mao et al., 1998); (2) amacrine feedback to the bipolar synaptic terminal (Maguire et al., 1989); (3) horizontal cell input to the bipolar dendrites (Mangel, 1991); and (4) Ca2+-dependent mechanisms in the bipolar dendrites (Shiells and Falk, 1999; Nawy, 2000).
Voltage-activated conductances in bipolar soma make a minimal contribution
If voltage-activated conductances in the bipolar soma make a substantial contribution to contrast adaptation, the effect of contrast on the light response of a bipolar should differ when the bipolar voltage is free to change or held constant. The extent of contrast adaptation under current and voltage clamp was measured using the static nonlinearity model as in Figures 1 and 3. Figure8A shows linear filters at 10 and 30% contrast for a voltage-clamped offbipolar cell. Contrast affected the amplitude and kinetics of the filter. Figure 8B shows linear filters for current-clamp responses from the same cell. Contrast again affected the amplitude and kinetics of the filter, and the magnitude of these effects was similar to that observed under voltage clamp.
The extent of contrast adaptation with the membrane voltage clamped or free to change was compared in seven off bipolars and fiveon bipolars; collected results are shown in Figure 8,C and D. Figure 8C plots the change in the amplitude of the linear filter measured under voltage clamp against that measured under current clamp, with each pointrepresenting measurements from a single cell as in Figure 8,A and B. Figure 8D compares the change in time-to-peak of the filters measured under voltage and current clamp. In each case, the points cluster near the line of identity, indicating that the effect of contrast on the amplitude and kinetics of the light responses of bipolar cells was similar under current and voltage clamp. These experiments show that voltage-activated conductances in the bipolar soma make little or no contribution to contrast adaptation.
Contrast adaptation persists without amacrine feedback
To test whether the effect of contrast could be attributable to amacrine feedback to the bipolar synaptic terminal, contrast adaptation was measured with the amacrine feedback working normally and with it suppressed. Amacrine feedback was suppressed with picrotoxin and strychnine, inhibitors of the GABA (Maguire et al., 1989) and glycine (Maple and Wu, 1998; Cook et al., 2000) receptors on the bipolar synaptic terminal. A substantial increase in the amplitude of the light responses of amacrine and ganglion cells confirmed that that picrotoxin and strychnine were effective in altering inhibition from amacrine cells (data not shown).
The effect of contrast on the amplitude and kinetics of the light response of a bipolar persisted in the presence of picrotoxin and strychnine. Figure 9, A andB, compares the extent of contrast adaptation in a voltage-clamped off bipolar cell superfused with normal Ringer's solution (Fig. 9A) or with Ringer's solution containing 150 μm picrotoxin and 5 μm strychnine (Fig. 9B). Picrotoxin and strychnine did not substantially alter the effect of contrast. Results from 13 bipolars in which contrast adaptation was measured with and without amacrine feedback are summarized in Figure 9C, which plots the effect of contrast on the amplitude of the response of a cell in picrotoxin and strychnine (y-axis) against that in Ringer's solution (x-axis). Each pointrepresents measurements on a single cell. The points cluster around the line of identity, indicating that contrast adaptation was similar in the two cases. The average amplitude of the filter at high contrast relative to that at low contrast was 0.80 ± 0.02 (mean ± SEM) in normal Ringer's solution and 0.82 ± 0.02 with amacrine feedback suppressed. Thus, contrast adaptation was affected little when amacrine feedback to the bipolar terminal was suppressed, and most or all of the adaptation was attributable to other mechanisms.
Contrast adaptation persists when horizontal inputs to bipolars are suppressed
To test whether the effect of contrast could be attributable to horizontal cell input to the bipolar dendrites, contrast adaptation was compared before and after suppressing the horizontal–bipolar synapse with bicuculline, an inhibitor of GABA receptors on the bipolar dendrites. The effect of contrast was essentially unchanged by suppression of horizontal input to bipolars.
Figure 9, D and E, compares the extent of contrast adaptation of a voltage-clamped offbipolar cell superfused with normal Ringer's solution (Fig.9D) or with Ringer's solution containing 5 μm bicuculline (Fig. 9E). Contrast adaptation was similar in the two conditions. Figure 9Fcollects results from seven such experiments, plotting the effect of contrast on the amplitude of the response of a cell in bicuculline (y-axis) against that in Ringer's solution (x-axis). Contrast adaptation was essentially unchanged in the presence of bicuculline. The average amplitude of the filter at high contrast relative to that at low contrast in these seven cells was 0.80 ± 0.05 (mean ± SEM) in both Ringer's solution and bicuculline. Thus, horizontal cell input to bipolars does not make a substantial contribution to contrast adaptation.
Ca2+ buffers eliminate contrast adaptation inoff bipolars
The experiments of Figure 9 show that contrast adaptation persists when a bipolar cell is voltage clamped and amacrine feedback to the bipolar terminal is suppressed. Under these conditions, the voltage in the axon terminal should not change, and thus conductances in the axon terminal do not contribute significantly to contrast adaptation. The similarity of contrast adaptation under current and voltage clamp (Fig.8) indicates that conductances in the soma do not make significant contributions. Thus, Figures 8 and 9 indicate that most of the contrast adaptation in the light response of a bipolar is generated in its dendrites.
Several studies have identified Ca2+-dependent gain controls in the dendrites of on bipolar cells (Shiells and Falk, 1999;Nawy, 2000). To test whether such a mechanism might contribute to contrast adaptation, changes in Ca2+ were suppressed by dialyzing a bipolar cell with a high concentration of Ca2+ buffer and measuring the consequences for contrast adaptation. The Ca2+ buffer and total Ca2+ in the dialyzing solution were increased by the same factor to keep the free Ca2+ concentration constant. HEDTA and diBromoBAPTA (Br2BAPTA) were used as Ca2+ buffers because they have a relatively low affinity for Ca2+ and thus are not likely to become fully Ca2+ bound.
Increasing the Ca2+ buffer concentration eliminated contrast adaptation in off, but noton, bipolars. The effect of contrast on the gain and kinetics of the light response of a cell was measured using the static nonlinearity model (Fig. 1). Figure10A shows linear filters at 10 and 30% contrast for a voltage-clampedoff bipolar dialyzed with a solution containing 1 mm HEDTA. As for perforated-patch recordings, contrast affected the amplitude and kinetics of the filter. Figure10B shows filters for a voltage-clampedoff bipolar dialyzed with a solution containing 10 mm HEDTA. In this case, contrast had little or no effect on the amplitude or kinetics of the filter. Results fromoff bipolars dialyzed with high and low concentrations of Ca2+ buffers are summarized in Figure 10C and compared with the extent of contrast adaptation in cells with native Ca2+ buffers (perforated-patch recordings). All cells in which Ca2+changes were suppressed with high Ca2+buffer concentrations showed essentially no contrast adaptation.
Contrast adaptation in on bipolars had little or no dependence on the Ca2+ buffer concentration. Figure 10D shows linear filters at 10 and 30% contrast for a voltage-clamped onbipolar dialyzed with a solution containing 1 mmHEDTA. Figure 10E shows filters for a voltage-clampedon bipolar dialyzed with a solution containing 10 mm HEDTA. In both cases, the sensitivity of the light response of a cell decreased with increases in contrast. Figure10F collects results from onbipolars dialyzed with high and low concentrations of Ca2+ buffer and with native buffers intact. Contrast adaptation was similar in all three conditions. Contrast adaptation also persisted in voltage-clampedon bipolar cells dialyzed with a solution containing 5 mm Br2BAPTA (data not shown). In eight cells dialyzed with Br2BAPTA, the amplitude of the linear filter for 30% contrast inputs relative to 10% contrast inputs was 0.81 ± 0.04 (mean ± SEM), again similar to the extent of contrast adaptation with the native Ca2+ buffers intact.
The results summarized in Figure 10 indicate that Ca2+-dependent mechanisms in the bipolar dendrites contribute to contrast adaptation in off, but noton, bipolars. This difference may help explain whyoff bipolar cells adapt more strongly to contrast changes than on bipolars (Fig. 6).
DISCUSSION
The experiments described here lead to three conclusions about contrast adaptation in salamander bipolar cells: (1) bipolar cells, but not horizontal cells or photoreceptors, adapt to temporal contrast; (2) contrast adaptation in the bipolars has a relatively fast onset and offset; and (3) the functional properties of contrast adaptation inon and off bipolars differ. The properties of contrast adaptation in salamander bipolar cells are discussed below and compared with adaptation in retinal ganglion cells.
Contrast gain and contrast adaptation
Contrast adaptation in signal transfer from photoreceptors to bipolars serves an important role in protecting against saturation. For contrast steps about a steady light level, the contrast gain of salamander bipolar cells is 5–10 times higher than that of cones (Burkhardt and Fahey, 1998) (Figs. 2, 3). Thus, without adaptation, fluctuating light inputs with a contrast of 5–10% would lead to significant saturation of the bipolar responses; such saturation would compromise encoding of visual inputs. Contrast adaptation may be a general feature of signal processing in bipolar cells. In rabbit, altering the contrast in the surround of the receptive field of a ganglion cell does not affect the gain of the receptive field center, suggesting that mammalian bipolar cells, which provide the receptive field center, also adapt to contrast (Brown and Masland, 2001).
Time course of onset and offset in bipolar and ganglion cells
After an increase in contrast, the onset of adaptation in the currents measured at the ganglion cell soma included fast (1–2 sec) and slow (10–20 sec) components (Kim and Rieke, 2001), suggesting that at least two distinct mechanisms contribute. The fast component in the ganglion cell currents accounted for a 20–30% reduction in the sensitivity when the contrast was increased by a factor of three (Kim and Rieke, 2001). In bipolar cells, the onset of contrast adaptation was restricted to a fast (1–2 sec) component. A threefold increase in contrast reduced the bipolar sensitivity by 20–25%, similar to the reduction in the sensitivity of a ganglion cell attributable to the fast-onset component of contrast adaptation. Thus, adaptation in the bipolar cells can account for most or all of the fast-onset contrast adaptation seen in the currents at the ganglion cell soma. Bipolar cells did not show a substantial slow component of contrast adaptation, and hence this component must originate at a later stage in retinal processing.
The different temporal components of contrast adaptation likely play different functional roles in retinal signal processing. The slow onset form of contrast adaptation observed in the responses of retinal ganglion cells (Smirnakis et al., 1997) is well suited to dynamically adjust visual sensitivity to match the temporal and spatial structure of the light inputs. The fast-onset component of contrast adaptation seen in the bipolar cells and ganglion cells is likely to shape responses to single visual objects, e.g., objects moving through the receptive field of a cell (Berry et al., 1999). The fast-onset component of contrast adaptation in the output spike trains of a ganglion cell contains approximately equal contributions from adaptation in the bipolar cells and adaptation in spike generation in the ganglion cell itself (Kim and Rieke, 2001).
The onset and offset of contrast adaptation follow different time courses in both bipolars (Fig. 5) and ganglion cells (Smirnakis et al., 1997; Brown and Masland, 2001), with the offset of adaptation proceeding more slowly than the onset. The asymmetry between the time course of the onset and offset is in agreement with theoretical arguments about the ease with which increases and decreases in contrast can be detected from the response of a cell (DeWeese and Zador, 1998).
Functional and mechanistic differences between on andoff bipolars
The functional properties of contrast adaptation differed foron and off bipolar cells (Fig. 6). Increases in contrast had a similar effect on the amplitude of the light responses in on and off cells. Changes in contrast, however, had a much more pronounced effect on the kinetics of the response of off bipolars than on bipolars. In salamander retinal ganglion cells, changes in contrast had a greater effect on both the amplitude and kinetics of the light response inoff cells than on cells (Chander and Chichilnisky, 1999, 2001; Kim and Rieke, 2001). Thus, some but not all of the on–off asymmetry in the ganglion cells can be accounted for by the asymmetry in the bipolars.
In addition to the difference in functional properties, the mechanisms mediating contrast adaptation in on and offbipolars differed (Fig. 10). In both cell types, contrast adaptation was mediated by mechanisms in the dendrites. In offbipolars, suppressing changes in Ca2+eliminated contrast adaptation, indicating that a Ca2+-dependent gain control in the dendrites was responsible. In on bipolars, suppressing Ca2+ changes had little or no effect on contrast adaptation. Adaptation in on bipolars could be mediated by desensitization of glutamate receptors or regulation of the second-messenger cascade coupling the receptors to channels (Nawy, 1999).
Mean and contrast adaptation in the retina
The output signals of retinal ganglion cells adapt to both the mean light intensity (for review, see Walraven et al., 1990) and the temporal contrast (for review, see Shapley, 1997; Meister and Berry, 1999). The extent to which mean and contrast adaptation operate independently has an important bearing on how vision adjusts to changes in the statistics of the light inputs.
At high light levels, the mean and contrast of a visual scene are primarily independent, with the mean determined primarily by the illuminating light source and the contrast by the distribution of reflectances in the scene. At low light levels, the mean and contrast of the light inputs cannot change independently because of quantal fluctuations in the incident photons. Indeed, the form of adaptation operating at the lowest light intensity is mediated by mechanisms in the retinal circuitry controlled by quantal fluctuations in the light input (Donner et al., 1990), likely the same mechanisms causing contrast adaptation at higher light levels.
Several observations suggest that adaptation to the mean light intensity and the fluctuations about the mean are primarily independent in the outer retina. First, photoreceptors contribute to adaptation to the mean light level (for review, see Walraven et al., 1990; Koutalos and Yau, 1996) but not to contrast adaptation (Fig. 3) (Sakai et al., 1995; Smirnakis et al., 1997). Second, Ca2+-dependent mechanisms in the dendrites of on bipolar cells contribute to mean but not contrast adaptation (Shiells and Falk, 1999) (Fig. 10). Third, results described here suggest that signal transfer from photoreceptors to bipolars can be described as a contrast-dependent linear filter in the bipolar dendrites followed by a static nonlinearity (Fig. 3). Consistent with this description, the static nonlinearity was much less pronounced in voltage-clamp experiments than current-clamp experiments and thus appeared to be dominated by voltage-dependent conductances in the bipolar soma. This indicates that contrast adaptation must be produced by a change in the fluctuations of the bipolar response rather than a change in the mean response.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant EY-11850 and the McKnight Foundation. I thank Cecilia Armstrong, Divya Chander, E. J. Chichilnisky, Greg Field, Josh Gold, Kerry Kim, and Maria McKinley for helpful discussions and Eric Martinson for excellent technical assistance.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Fred Rieke at the above address. E-mail: rieke{at}u.washington.edu.