Skip to main content

Umbrella menu

  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
ARTICLE, Behavioral/Systems

Fear and Feeding in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell: Rostrocaudal Segregation of GABA-Elicited Defensive Behavior Versus Eating Behavior

Sheila M. Reynolds and Kent C. Berridge
Journal of Neuroscience 1 May 2001, 21 (9) 3261-3270; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-09-03261.2001
Sheila M. Reynolds
1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kent C. Berridge
1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

This study examined localization of positive versus negative motivational functions mediated by GABA circuits within the accumbens shell. Microinjections of a GABAA agonist (0, 25, 75, and 225 ng/0.5 μl muscimol) in rostral shell sites elicited appetitive increases in eating behavior. In contrast, microinjections in caudal shell sites elicited defensive burying or paw-treading behavior. Rats whose microinjections landed bilaterally outside of the accumbens shell did not display either behavior. Defensive treading elicited by caudal shell muscimol microinjection appeared to be a negative motivated response to threat (similar in parameters and orientation to normal defensive burying of a threatening electrified shock prod). The nucleus accumbens shell thus appears functionally heterogeneous in coding motivational valence. The demonstration that muscimol elicits positive eating behavior from rostral shell versus negative defensive behavior from caudal shell suggests in particular that GABAergic substrates of positive and negative types of motivated behavior in the nucleus accumbens shell are segregated along a rostrocaudal gradient.

  • accumbens shell
  • GABA
  • food intake
  • reward
  • appetite
  • motivation
  • glutamate
  • dopamine
  • fear
  • defense
  • aggression
  • mesolimbic
  • microinjection

GABAergic medium spiny neurons in the nucleus accumbens shell are implicated in the control of appetitive behavior and reward. Regarding eating behavior specifically, robust increases in food intake by rats are elicited by microinjection in the medial accumbens shell of either GABAA or GABAB receptor agonists (or non-NMDA glutamate antagonists) (Maldonado-Irizarry et al., 1995; Stratford and Kelley, 1997; Basso and Kelley, 1999).

Aside from its role in positive appetitive behavior, the accumbens shell has also been implicated in negative motivational states, such as stress, fear, and defensive behavioral responses elicited by noxious or threatening stimuli (Inoue et al., 1994; Beck and Fibiger, 1995; Gray, 1995; Salamone et al., 1997; Berridge et al., 1999; Liberzon et al., 1999). Footshock increases extracellular dopamine in the accumbens shell but not core (Kalivas and Duffy, 1995), and increased accumbens dopamine or DOPAC have also been reported after other noxious stimuli, such as tail pinch, anxiogenic drugs, bright novel environments, or immobilization stress (Thierry et al., 1976;D'Angio et al., 1987; Bertolucci-D'Angio et al., 1990; McCullough and Salamone, 1992; Berridge et al., 1999). Even conditioned stimuli for fear, such as auditory or context cues that have been paired with shock, may produce increases in accumbens dopamine and accumbens Fos expression (Beck and Fibiger, 1995; Young et al., 1998).

Regarding the relationship of accumbens GABA neurotransmission to stress, presentation of a conditioned signal for shock increases GABA levels in the medial accumbens shell (Saul'skaya and Marsden, 1998). Thus, GABA neurotransmission in the accumbens shell may play a role in defensive or fear-related behavior, as well as in positively motivated behavior.

Rodents have evolved a natural defensive reaction, in the form of defensive burying behavior, as a species-specific response to a variety of threatening stimuli (e.g., electric shocks, scorpions, rattlesnakes, etc.) (Owings and Coss, 1977; Wilkie et al., 1979; Bolles and Fanselow, 1980; Pinel et al., 1992; Treit et al., 1993; Rodgers et al., 1997;Londei et al., 1998; Treit et al., 1998). Defensive burying consists of vigorous treading-like movements of the forepaws that splash wood shavings, sand, or similar substrate toward the threatening object, sometimes burying it entirely. Rats emit defensive treading toward an electrified “shock prod” (Fig.1A) (Treit et al., 1981) and toward the food that was paired with LiCl-induced illness (Parker, 1988). Mice emit defensive treading to bury a live scorpion (Londei et al., 1998), and ground squirrels defend their burrow by emitting similar defensive treading and sand kicking toward a rattlesnake during anti-predator mobbing (Fig. 1B) (Owings and Coss, 1977). Anxiolytic drugs reduce defensive treading behavior of rats toward a shock prod (Treit, 1985), as do lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala (Kopchia et al., 1992). Defensive treading therefore appears to constitute a negative motivated reaction to a variety of noxious stimuli that pose a near and immediate threat (Rodgers et al., 1997).

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Examples of defensive treading or burying behavior elicited from rodent species by threatening stimuli or by accumbens GABA activation. A, Normal rat defensively burying electrified shock prod that shocked it.B, Belding's ground squirrel defending maternal burrow against a predatory rattlesnake in the wild (reprinted from Owings and Morton, 1998 with permission). C, Rat emitting defensive treading of the type elicited by muscimol microinjection at caudal sites in accumbens shell. Note the midair substrate in front of rat, thrown up by defensive treading movements inA and C.

In this study, we compared eating versus defensive treading behavior elicited by GABAA receptor activation after muscimol microinjection in the accumbens shell. Our goal was to examine the GABAergic localization of appetitive and defensive motivational function within accumbens shell.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Experiment 1: segregation of accumbens shell sites for elicitation of eating behavior versus defensive treading behavior

Subjects

A total of 60 male and female Sprague Dawley rats (280–320 gm at the time of surgery) were group-housed (∼21°C; 12 hr light/dark cycle) with ad libitum food (Purina Rat Chow) and water (tap water) available.

Surgery

Rats were pretreated with 0.1 ml of atropine sulfate and anesthetized with a mixture of ketamine HCl and xylazine (80 and 5 mg/kg, respectively) and placed in a David Kopf Instruments (Tujunga, CA) sterotaxic apparatus with the incisor bar set at 5.0 mm above interaural zero to avoid the lateral ventricles. Chronic microinjection guide cannulas (23 gauge) were implanted bilaterally 2.0 mm above the intended target. Accumbens shell targets differed mainly in anteroposterior (AP) values (between +2.1 and +3.6 mm anterior to bregma), which progressed through the shell in 0.3 mm increments, with only minor alterations in mediolateral (ML) coordinates (±0.8 to ±1.2) and dorsoventral (DV) coordinates (−5.5 to −5.8 below surface) to accommodate the changing contours of the accumbens shell (Table 1). Microinjection guide cannulas were anchored to the skull with screws and acrylic cement. A stainless steel obturator was inserted into each guide cannula to help prevent occlusions. Each rat received prophylactic penicillin (aquacillin; 45,000 U, i.m.) after surgery. At least 7 d were allowed for recovery before the beginning of behavioral testing.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Sterotaxic surgical coordinates for experiment 1

Drugs and intracerebral injections

Muscimol (Sigma, St. Louis, MO) was dissolved in sterile 0.15m saline, which was also used for vehicle control microinjections (0.5 μl). For the first experiment, a dose of 75 ng/side muscimol (resulting in a total dose of 150 ng/animal) was chosen because it was intermediate between the two most effective doses (50 and 100 ng) for eliciting eating as reported by Stratford and Kelley (1997). Microinjections were made with a stainless steel injector cannula (29 gauge), extending 2.0 mm beyond the ventral tip of the guide, and attached to a syringe pump via PE-20 tubing. The animals were gently hand-held and bilaterally infused with a volume of 0.5 μl at a rate of 0.30 μl/min. Animals received muscimol and vehicle microinjections in a counterbalanced order with 48 hr rest between injections. After infusion, the injectors remained in place for an additional 60 sec to allow for drug diffusion before the obturators were replaced, and the animal was placed immediately in the test chamber. For this and subsequent experiments, animals were habituated to the test chambers for 4 consecutive days before the beginning of behavioral testing and received a vehicle microinjection on the final day of habituation.

Behavioral testing

The transparent test chambers (23 × 20 × 45 cm) contained wood shavings spread 2.0 cm in depth evenly across the chamber floor. A preweighed amount of food (Purina Rat Chow pellets) was placed on the chamber floor, and a tap water spout was available during each 60 min test session.

Food and water intake were recorded by both weight and duration of eating or drinking behavior. The behavior of each rat was videotaped for detailed off-line behavioral analysis. The videotapes were subsequently scored by an experimenter blind to drug treatment and analyzed for (1) time spent eating (defined as the amount of time the animal's mouth was either touching a food pellet or chewing), (2) drinking (amount of time a rat's tongue was in contact with the water spout), (3) paw treading (defined as rapidly repeated forward-and-backward movements of either a single forepaw or rapidly alternating bilateral forward-and-backward movements of both forepaws, which shoved or sprayed the wood shavings forward, (4) head covering (burying the head underneath the wood shavings covering the snout and eyes), (5) grooming (defined as paw strokes over the face or licking of the paws or body), (6) general locomotion (front-to-back cage crossings), and (7) resting (tucking head against chest without movement for >5 sec). Duration of each behavior was scored in terms of seconds spent engaged in it by each rat.

Environmental influence

We noticed in pilot experiments that our magnitude of food intake elicited after muscimol injections was slightly less than that reported by Kelley and colleagues after equivalent doses (Stratford and Kelley, 1997; Basso and Kelley, 1999). We surmised that extraneous environmental stimuli, such as wood shavings, might exert an inhibitory influence on elicited eating because our animals had always been tested for food intake in chambers containing both wood shavings and food, whereas previous studies by Kelley and colleagues had tested intake with only food present (no shavings). To test this possibility, the effects of muscimol infusions on food intake were compared in two environmental stimulus conditions: food and wood shavings both present versus food only present (shavings absent). A waterspout was always available during testing.

Histology

After behavioral testing, rats were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital, microinjected with ink for anatomical localization of injection sites, and perfused transcardially with buffered saline, followed by a buffered 4% paraformaldehyde solution. The brains were removed, post-fixed, sectioned (40 μm), mounted on slides, and stained with cresyl violet. Cannula placements were mapped onto the corresponding atlas drawings of Paxinos and Watson (1986). The data from animals with cannula placements falling outside the accumbens shell were considered separately in the statistical analysis.

Statistical analysis

Each behavior was initially examined with a two-way repeated measures ANOVA (drug × anatomical level) with one factor (drug) repetition. When significant main effects were found, additional analysis was performed with one-way repeated measures ANOVA withpost hoc comparisons conducted by Bonferroni test.

Experiment 2: dose–response effects for elicited eating versus defensive treading behavior

Surgery

Twenty-five female Sprague Dawley rats were implanted bilaterally with chronic indwelling cannulas in the medial accumbens shell as in experiment 1. Fifteen rats received cannulas aimed at the rostral shell (AP, +3.1; ML, ±0.8; DV, −5.8), and 10 rats received cannulas aimed at the caudal shell (AP, +2.1; ML, ±1.2; DV, −5.6).

Experimental design

Each animal received bilateral injections of 0, 25, 75, or 225 ng of muscimol (dissolved in 0.5 μl of saline) before testing. The order of doses was administered across rats in a counterbalanced order. Testing of eating behavior and of defensive treading behavior and all analysis and histology procedures were as described above.

Statistical analysis

Effects of muscimol doses on each behavior at each anatomical level were examined by one-way repeated measures ANOVA, followed by multiple comparisons conducted with Bonferroni tests.

Experiment 3: comparison of muscimol-elicited treading with “real defensive treading” elicited by electric shock prod

To determine whether the paw-treading behavior elicited by GABA receptor activation in the caudal shell reflects a motivated response to threat similar to natural defensive treading, we compared motor and orientation parameters of muscimol-elicited treading with those of normal defensive treading behavior elicited from undrugged rats by encounter with an actual threatening object, namely, an electrified shock prod (Treit et al., 1981).

Experimental design

Eight female Sprague Dawley rats were habituated to the 29 × 39 × 39 cm Plexiglas testing chamber containing 5.0 cm of wood shavings on the floor, for 4 consecutive days. On the fifth day, undrugged animals were placed into the chambers, and an electrified metal shock prod (9 cm, 1 mA) was inserted into the front of the chamber at a height of 2 cm (Treit et al., 1981). Rats were allowed to explore the chamber and to touch or avoid the prod as they chose for 30 min while behavior was videotaped for later analysis. A separate group of eight female rats received bilateral muscimol microinjections into caudal accumbens shell (75 ng/0.5 μl). These rats were selected from experiment 1 on the basis of having shown vigorous defensive treading elicited by shell GABA receptor activation as described above. The rats that received muscimol microinjections were tested in similar chambers with wood shavings on the floor (but without the shock prod). The videotaped behavior of both groups was subjected to identical video analysis.

Videotape analysis

Orientation toward chamber and external stimuli. The orientation of defensive treading behavior was scored in terms of whether the spray of shavings was directed against the front, sides, or back of the cage using a 360° circle in which 0° represented a radial line from the midline of the cage front (the shock prod was always inserted in the front; the video camera, experimenter, and light source were also visible beyond the transparent front wall).

Mound construction. The number, size (height and length), shape, and location of mounds constructed during treading behavior were measured by video analysis. A mound was defined as a pile of shavings >1 cm in height constructed as a consequence of the rat's treading movements. The physical parameters of the mound were calculated by comparing the measured video image size with measures of standard mounds of known size, and mound parameters were plotted to show their position in a map diagram of the test chamber.

Movement parameters. Six motor parameters of treading movements were analyzed frame-by-frame (30 frames per second) or in slow motion ( Embedded Image-½ actual speed): (1) cycle duration was the interval between forepaw extension and retraction to the point of origin (in milliseconds); (2) bout duration was duration of a series of paw-treading strokes without >1 sec pause; (3) number of cycles per bout was the number of forelimb extension–retraction cycles performed within each bout; (4) limb extension was length of maximal extension of a paw from the point of origin (determined from the video screen by first measuring the video image length from the rat's nose tip to its ear and using that to calculate the movement extension length); (5) unilateral versus bilateral paw use was percentage of treading bouts performed with one forelimb only compared with percentage performed with both forelimbs; and (6) direction of forelimb strokes was direction of forelimb strokes relative to the rat's midline and classified as midline movement (0°) or lateral movement (>45°).

Statistical analysis

Direction of each type of paw-treading behavior was examined with one-way repeated measures ANOVA. Size of mounds constructed by treading behavior was assessed with Mann–Whitney Rank Sum test. Differences in movement parameters between shock prod and muscimol-induced treading were examined with Mann–Whitney Rank Sum test.

RESULTS

Experiment 1: segregation of accumbens shell sites for elicitation of eating behavior versus defensive treading behavior

Eating behavior elicited by GABA agonist: rostral shell localization

For the purpose of initial analysis, the shell was divided into three rostrocaudal levels: a rostral level (2.2–1.6 mm anterior to bregma; ∼40% of the shell; n = 19), a middle level (1.4–1.2 mm anterior to bregma; ∼20%; n = 6), and a caudal level (1.0–0.4 mm anterior to bregma; ∼40%;n = 23). Drinking, grooming, locomotion, and resting behaviors were not altered by muscimol microinjection at any level. Robust eating behavior was elicited by muscimol microinjection (75 ng) into the medial accumbens shell but only at rostral and middle sites. A two-way ANOVA that compared rostrocaudal site (three levels) against drug condition (muscimol versus vehicle) found a main effect of rostrocaudal site (F(2,101) = 8.75;p < 0.001), a main effect of drug versus vehicle (ANOVA; F(1,101) = 10.32;p < 0.002), and a significant drug by site interaction (ANOVA; drug × level, F(2,101) = 29.73; p < 0.001) (Fig.2). A one-way ANOVA comparing rostrocaudal levels indicated that muscimol elicited greater food intake when infused into either the rostral or middle shell levels compared with the caudal shell level (for muscimol rostral vs caudal,p < 0.001; middle vs caudal, p < 0.002).

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Mean ± SEM food intake and time spent paw treading after vehicle or 75 ng muscimol injections into different regions of the medial accumbens shell. A, Muscimol increased eating behavior compared with vehicle when microinjected into the rostral shell but decreased food intake below vehicle when injected into the caudal shell (cumulative total over 60 min trial).B, Paw-treading behavior was markedly increased by muscimol injections compared with vehicle injections in the caudal accumbens shell and only slightly increased by rostral muscimol infusions. *p < 0.002; **p < 0.001, significant muscimol compared with vehicle effect.

Muscimol microinjection at sites in the rostral one-third of the shell (2.2–1.6 mm anterior to bregma) most dramatically increased eating over vehicle-elicited baseline intake (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; drug, F(1,39) = 49.82; p< 0.001) (Fig. 3). Food intake was approximately doubled after muscimol compared with vehicle injections at every site within the rostral level: 2.20 mm anterior to bregma (ANOVA; drug, F(1,11) = 18.62;p < 0.01), 1.70 mm (ANOVA; drug,F(1,13) = 11.70; p < 0.02), and 1.60 mm (ANOVA; drug, F(1,15) = 28.05;p < 0.002) (Fig. 3). At sites within the middle AP level (1.4–1.2 mm anterior to bregma), muscimol produced only a marginally significant increase in food intake over vehicle (ANOVA; drug, F(1,11) = 5.60; p = 0.06). In contrast, muscimol administration into the caudal level of the accumbens shell (1.0–0.4 mm anterior to bregma) actually caused a significant decrease of food intake to ∼50% of vehicle control amounts (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; drug,F(1,51) = 15.60; p < 0.001). Food intake was suppressed by muscimol compared with vehicle at two caudal sites, corresponding to 1.00 mm anterior to bregma (ANOVA; drug, F(1,21) = 18.47;p < 0.002) and 0.48 mm (ANOVA; drug,F(1,5) = 24.75; p < 0.04) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Mean ± SEM food intake (top row) and time spent paw treading (bottom row) after vehicle or muscimol injections at each rostrocaudal level of the accumbens shell. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001 muscimol compared with vehicle.

Environmental modulation of eating

Comparison of food intake after rostral muscimol injection in animals tested with both food and wood shavings present versus with food only (wood shavings absent) revealed that food intake was greater when wood shavings were absent (paired t test;p < 0.02). This suggests that extraneous stimuli such as wood shavings may inhibit muscimol-elicited food intake. Suppression by extraneous stimuli may account for why we observed slightly lower amounts of food intake after muscimol microinjection (even in rostral shell) than found previously by Kelley and colleagues (Stratford and Kelley, 1997; Basso and Kelley, 1999).

Defensive treading behavior elicited by GABA agonist: caudal shell localization

The rostrocaudal organization of defensive treading behavior elicited by GABA receptor activation within the accumbens was the reverse of eating behavior. Muscimol injections evoked strong defensive treading behavior from caudal and middle sites in the medial accumbens shell but very little treading from rostral sites. A two-way ANOVA comparing rostrocaudal site (three levels) and drug conditions (muscimol versus vehicle) found a significant main effect of site (F(2,101) = 17.66; p< 0.001), a significant main effect of drug (F(1,101) = 48.49; p< 0.001), and a significant interaction between site and drug (F(2,101) = 18.58; p< 0.001) (Fig. 2).

A subsequent one-way ANOVA of sites showed that muscimol at sites within the caudal one-third of the shell (1.0–0.4 mm anterior to bregma) produced the most vigorous defensive treading, which was 10 times more intense than at more rostral sites (F(2,49) = 18.05; p < 0.001) (Fig. 2). Muscimol microinjection at caudal sites robustly increased paw-treading behavior over vehicle-elicited levels, eliciting 2–6 min of cumulative defensive treading after muscimol compared with only a few seconds at most after vehicle (one-way ANOVA; drug,F(1,51) = 79.00; p < 0.001) (Fig. 3). Within this caudal shell zone, muscimol increased defensive treading behavior over vehicle levels at sites corresponding to 1.00 mm anterior to bregma (ANOVA; drug,F(1,21) = 46.97;p < 0.001) and 0.70 mm (ANOVA; drug,F(1,23) = 94.78; p < 0.001).

Defensive treading movements consisted of rhythmic cycles of vigorous and repetitive forelimb paw thrusts forward-and-backward (1.6–3.2 cm extension length), which served to shove or spray wood shavings 1–3 inches in front of the rat (usually 0–60° in front, but on occasion deviating in angle as far as 90°) in bouts of between 0.5 and 6 sec, usually with pauses of several seconds between successive bouts. Each bout contained 2–21 individual forelimb extension–retraction cycles (3.7–6.0 Hz). Most defensive treading bouts consisted of several unilateral paw pushes (69% of bouts), followed by several pushes by the other paw, although some bouts consisted of bilateral paw pushes emitted in an alternating right–left–right–left pattern (31% of bouts).

Defensive treading bouts typically resulted in the construction of elevated mounds of wood shavings placed in front of the rat (5–10 cm in height–width and up to 20 cm in length). Between the rat and the mound, a low trough or depression in the surface was also created by the displacement of shavings. Rats did not emit treading movements randomly, but instead coordinated their defensive treading bouts toward their mound locations so that the mounds tended to increase in size over successive treading bouts. In addition, the mounds themselves were not placed randomly but instead were constructed in strategic positions within the cage, usually placed to block the rat's exposure to the transparent front of the chamber and less commonly placed in back corners (as though the corners were also perceived as minor sources of threat). These features gave observers the impression of a coordinated defensive reaction toward the location of the mound rather than a simple series of stereotyped movements.

Only marginal defensive treading was elicited by muscimol at sites within the middle AP level (1.4–1.2 mm anterior to bregma) (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; drug, F(1,11)= 4.59; p = 0.08) Sporadic defensive treading was still elicited by muscimol at sites in the rostral one-third of the shell (2.2–1.6 mm anterior to bregma) (ANOVA; drug,F(1,37) = 13.20; p < 0.002), specifically at two AP levels: 1.70 mm anterior to bregma (ANOVA; drug, F(1,13) = 7.84;p < 0.05) and 1.60 mm (ANOVA; drug,F(1,15) = 6.29; p < 0.05). However, in the rostral shell, muscimol elicited only 10% the amount of defensive treading elicited at caudal sites, and many rats showed no defensive treading at all after rostral muscimol microinjections.

Rats that received muscimol infusions in caudal shell (but only caudal shell) often emitted distress vocalizations upon being handled at the end of the test session and attempted to bite the experimenter and to escape. The heightened defensiveness of rats that received caudal muscimol microinjections was sometimes so strong that the animals could not be retrieved for several hours after the test session.

Anatomical map: comparison of eating versus defensive function localization within accumbens shell

To construct an anatomical map of functional localization, functional criteria were set for the elicitation of food intake and paw treading, and microinjection sites that met them were plotted on a digitized stereotaxic atlas. An “eating site” was considered to be any site at which muscimol microinjection increased food intake at least 150% over vehicle baseline (or elicited at least 1 gm food intake in cases in which food intake was zero after vehicle microinjection). A “defensive treading site” was considered to be any site at which muscimol elicited at least 100 sec of total cumulative paw-treading behavior (which was orders of magnitude greater than vehicle levels because treading was generally zero after vehicle microinjection). A site could be classified as both an eating site and a defensive treading site if it met both criteria. Sites that met neither criteria were considered to be functionally negative.

Eating sites were clearly concentrated in the rostral one-half of the accumbens shell (Figs. 4, 5), from the rostral tip of the shell to a point at which the anterior commissure merges and the nucleus of the vertical limb diagonal band begins. Defensive treading sites were clustered in the caudal one-third of the accumbens shell, beginning at the level of the optic nerve, just behind the tenia tecta. The most rostral defensive treading sites overlapped slightly with the most caudal eating sites, and a number of sites in this zone of overlap supported both types of muscimol-elicited behavior. “Negative sites,” in which GABA activation produced neither behavior, were chiefly located bilaterally outside of the accumbens shell, usually placed laterally in or near the core of the accumbens, ventral to the shell in the islands of Calleja, or medial to the shell in the vertical limb diagonal band near the medial septal nucleus. Animals whose microinjections landed bilaterally outside of the accumbens shell showed neither eating nor defensive behavior (e.g., in accumbens core, at the core–shell border but not penetrating into the shell, or in other structures outside the shell). Thus, it appeared that these behaviors were attributable to activation of receptors within the shell.

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Coronal map of microinjection sites for appetitive behavior versus defensive behavior elicited within the accumbens shell. Eating sites are denoted by crosses and are restricted to the rostral accumbens shell. Defensive treading sites are denoted by open squares and are concentrated in the caudal one-third of the shell. Mixed eating and defensive treading sites, in which both behaviors were elicited by muscimol, are denoted by cross-filled squares and appear in intermediate levels. Negative sites, in which neither behavior was elicited, are denoted by filled circles, typically placed near the outside of the shell. Atlas based on Paxinos and Watson, 1986.

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

Sagittal map of microinjection sites for appetitive versus defensive behavior. Eating sites are denoted bycrosses and are restricted to the rostral accumbens shell. Defensive treading sites are denoted by open squares and are clustered in the caudal shell. Mixed eating and defensive treading sites that caused both behaviors are denoted bycross-filled squares and appear at midshell levels. Negative sites are denoted by filled circles and are located outside the accumbens shell. Only microinjection sites are shown that were located in the sagittal plane ∼0.9 mm lateral from midline (∼85% of total sites; atlas based on Paxinos and Watson, 1986).

In summary, accumbens muscimol administration into different sites elicited two different types of behavior with opposite motivational valence organized along a rostrocaudal gradient within the medial accumbens shell. Eating was elicited by GABAAreceptor activation at rostral sites within medial shell, whereas defensive treading was elicited at caudal sites. The functional regions appeared to overlap slightly, and some intermediate shell sites supported both behaviors.

Experiment 2: dose–response effects for elicited eating versus defensive treading behavior

When the effects of 0, 25, 75, and 225 ng of muscimol doses were compared at rostral eating sites and caudal defensive treading sites, different dose–response behavior was seen (Fig.6). Paw treading increased with dose in an approximately linear manner until reaching an asymptote, whereas the greatest food intake was produced by the lowest muscimol dose.

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

Dose–response functions for mean ± SEM food intake and defensive paw-treading behavior after muscimol microinjection into the rostral and caudal shell. A, Food intake was significantly increased by the two lowest doses of muscimol compared with vehicle infused into the rostral shell, especially by the 25 ng dose. B, Defensive treading was slightly increased by rostral shell muscimol at the higher 75 and 225 ng doses compared with vehicle. C, Food intake was significantly decreased below vehicle levels by muscimol microinjection within the caudal shell. D, Defensive treading was significantly increased by muscimol microinjection compared with vehicle microinjection into the caudal shell, especially at the 75 and 225 ng doses. *p <0.05; **p < 0.001 muscimol doses compared with vehicle.

Dose–response effects for rostral shell eating behavior

Muscimol microinjection at rostral eating sites significantly increased food intake over vehicle baseline levels (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; dose, F(3,43) = 7.92;p < 0.001) at the two lowest doses (specific dose compared with vehicle; 25 ng, p < 0.001; 75 ng,p < 0.04; 225 ng, p = 0.11). However, the 25 ng dose of muscimol appeared to be the most effective for eliciting eating behavior, more than doubling food intake and causing a larger increase than higher doses (25 ng dose compared with 225 ng dose, p < 0.05; paired t test). In contrast, muscimol infusions into the caudal shell suppressed eating to 80–10% of vehicle levels in a linear dose–response manner (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; dose, F(3,46)= 11.70; p < 0.001) (specific dose compared with vehicle; 25 ng, p = 0.31; 75 ng, p < 0.001; 225 ng, p < 0.001).

Dose–response effects for caudal shell defensive treading behavior

Muscimol microinjections into the caudal shell elicited robust paw treading (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; dose,F(3,46) = 9.66; p < 0.001) (specific dose compared with vehicle; 25 ng, p = 0.55; 75 ng, p < 0.001; 225 ng, p < 0.001). The highest two caudal doses produced the greatest amount of treading, which averaged over 3 min of cumulative treading, although a ceiling effect appeared at ∼75 ng. The highest 225 ng caudal dose caused two rats to become immobile for several hours, beginning ∼40 min after microinjection, during which time they lay spread-eagle and were unresponsive when touched. Rostral microinjections of muscimol also elicited small but significant amounts of defensive treading at the highest two doses (one-way repeated measures ANOVA; dose,F(3,42) = 6.69;p < 0.001) (specific dose compared with vehicle; 25 ng, p = 0.63; 75 ng, p < 0.02; 225 ng,p < 0.001).

Experiment 3: comparison of muscimol-elicited treading with real defensive treading elicited by electric shock prod

Orientation toward external stimuli

Normal defensive burying behavior elicited from undrugged rats by a shock prod was compared with defensive burying elicited by muscimol microinjection (75 ng) in the caudal accumbens. The two types of burying behavior were similar in both movement pattern and mound construction. All rats in the undrugged shock-prod condition explored the chamber and touched the electrified prod one or two times (mean, 1.4 ± 0.2 touches) with their paw or snout, withdrawing immediately and vigorously upon receiving a shock. Defensive burying behavior was subsequently emitted by those rats (109.5 ± 24.3 sec/30 min) toward the shock prod positioned at the front of the cage (compared with sides or back of the chamber; one-way repeated measures ANOVA comparing direction of treading orientation;F(3,23) = 222.72; p < 0.001).

In comparison, rats that received muscimol microinjections in the caudal accumbens shell (but in the absence of the shock prod) similarly tended to orient their defensive burying behavior (268.3 ± 62.9 sec/60 min) toward the transparent front of the chamber (which faced the camera, experimenter, and light source) than toward the more sheltered sides or back of the cage (which faced opaque surfaces; one-way repeated measures ANOVA; direction,F(2,23) = 19.12; p < 0.001) (Fig. 7).

Fig. 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 7.

Comparison of defensive treading direction and bedding mound placement within the test chamber of rats exposed to either caudal shell muscimol (without shock prod present) or a shock prod (without microinjection). Striped areas represent distribution of shaving mounds, dimensions are indicated by moundarrows (in centimeters), and numbersin squares indicate height (in centimeters) of mounds. The orientation or direction of actual treading behavior is indicated by arrows next to the rat. Each arrow length indicates the relative proportion of treading duration directed toward each of the four cage sides. A, Treading induced by muscimol microinjection in caudal shell was directed predominantly toward the front of the cage, which faced the light source, camera, and experimenter, and resulted in mounds of wood shavings spread along the cage front and occasionally in smaller mounds located in back corners. B, Shock prod-elicited treading from undrugged rats was also directed primarily toward the cage front, which contained the electric prod, and resulted in a large bedding mound that covered the shock prod. **p < 0.001, significant difference in direction of treading for each type of treading.

Mound construction

Defensive burying behavior elicited by shock prod and caudal muscimol microinjection both resulted in the construction of similar bedding mounds that were of similar height and size (shock prod mound height, 5.4 ± 0.7 cm; length, 21.4 ± 1.5 cm; width, 14.0 ± 0.4 cm; muscimol mound height, 4.4 ± 0.5; length, 10.3 ± 2.1 cm; width, 18.6 ± 1.1 cm). Seven of eight rats encountering a shock prod buried the prod entirely under a mound of wood shavings, whereas seven of eight muscimol-treated rats constructed long mounds (over 15-cm-long) that extended across the entire front wall of their chambers. One difference in mound construction was that rats encountering a shock prod constructed only one mound (burying the shock prod), whereas three of eight rats after muscimol microinjections constructed more than one mound: a major mound at the front of the chamber as described above, and one or two smaller mounds (2.2 ± 0.2 cm in height) in the back corners.

Movement parameters

There were no significant differences in movement parameters between defensive burying elicited by a shock prod compared with defensive burying elicited by caudal shell muscimol regarding either cycle duration, number of cycles per bout, percentage of unilateral versus bilateral paw cycles, or direction of forelimb stroke (Table2). The only significant differences in motor parameters between the two forms of treading behavior was that bout duration for muscimol-induced paw treading was very slightly longer than for shock-induced paw treading (p < 0.05), and conversely, the length of forelimb extension was longer during shock-induced burying than during muscimol-induced burying (p < 0.01). These differences were very small (<25%), and in general the movements appeared highly similar in defensive treading behavior elicited by shell muscimol microinjection and in normal defensive burying behavior elicited by an electric shock prod.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Forelimb motor parameters in paw treading elicited by a shock prod versus muscimol microinjections into caudal accumbens shell

DISCUSSION

Our results showed that GABAA receptor activation in nucleus accumbens shell can produce either appetitive eating behavior or defensive treading behavior (in which paw treading movements spray wood shavings into protective mounds), depending on whether the muscimol microinjection site is in rostral versus caudal shell. Microinjection in rostral shell elicited positive eating behavior, especially at low doses (e.g., 25 ng). That confirms previous reports of food intake after accumbens muscimol (Stratford and Kelley, 1997; Basso and Kelley, 1999). In contrast, muscimol microinjection in caudal shell produced negative defensive burying behavior, especially after high doses (e.g., 225 ng). Defensive treading elicited by accumbens microinjection has not to our knowledge been reported previously. GABAergic inhibition of medium spiny neurons and their projections may be the cellular mechanism for both positive and negative types of muscimol-elicited behavior (Carlezon and Wise, 1996;Stratford and Kelley, 1997, 1999; Zahm, 2000).

Defensive treading behavior: motor stereotypy or fearful response to threat?

Normal rodents display defensive treading behavior toward threatening stimuli (electric shock, scorpions, predators, noxious foods, etc). However, is paw-treading behavior caused by caudal shell muscimol a motivated defensive response to a perceived threat? Or is it instead a simple motor pattern triggered in the absence of motivational valence?

Three lines of evidence indicate that burying behavior after caudal shell muscimol was motivated rather than motor. First, movement parameters used in muscimol-elicited treading were similar to those of normal motivated defensive treading elicited by a shock prod. Second, muscimol-elicited treading was coordinated as though to defend against major features of the environment. Mounds constructed after caudal shell muscimol were placed strategically in the chamber. For example, the major mound was always placed as a barrier between the rat and the transparent front of the chamber, beyond which were other objects in the room, such as the experimenter, camera, and light source. Third, muscimol-elicited treading behavior was often followed by other defensive behaviors when touched, such as distress vocalization, biting, and escape attempts. In other words, caudal shell muscimol appeared to cause rats to respond as though to a threat and as though defensive burying was one component of their defensive response.

A conclusion that GABAA receptor activation in caudal shell causes a motivated defensive or fearful reaction does not imply that it is identical to conventional fear-inducing procedures, such as classical conditioning of freeze or startle responses to a signal for shock (LeDoux, 1998; Fanselow and LeDoux, 1999; Maren, 1999), or even identical to natural defensive burying reactions elicited by a shock prod or other threatening stimulus (Treit et al., 1981). Fear may not be a single reaction caused identically by all of these. Several investigators have suggested that there may exist different types of fearful reaction, which serve different purposes (Bolles and Fanselow, 1980; Marks and Nesse, 1994; Kagan and Schulkin, 1995), and are mediated by different neural systems (Treit et al., 1993; Gray and McNaughton, 1996; Killcross et al., 1997; Davis and Lee, 1998; Rosen and Schulkin, 1998; Lehmann et al., 2000). For example,Gray and McNaughton (1996) suggested that different neural systems may mediate active panic defense reactions versus passive inhibitory withdrawal reactions. If so, defensive burying seems likely to correspond most closely to active defense. Alternatively, the defensive state triggered by caudal shell muscimol might be different from all types of naturally triggered fearful reactions, being instead one isolated fragment of normal fear processes. For example, Berridge and Robinson (1998) suggested that, just as positive incentive salience may be contributed by mesolimbic systems to appetitive motivation and reward, so also a related but negative mesolimbic form of motivational salience might contribute attention-grabbing properties to frightening stimuli. A GABAA agonist in caudal accumbens shell could conceivably cause aversive motivational salience to be attributed to the neural representation of stimuli, such as the experimenter and other objects in the room, causing the rat to perceive them as frighteningly salient. In contrast, the positive form of the same process triggered in rostral shell could cause attribution of positive incentive salience to food stimuli, leading to appetitive eating behavior. Finally, it is possible that the eating behavior evoked by rostral shell muscimol is related to stress-induced eating caused by stimuli, such as tail pinch (Antelman et al., 1975), and that defensive treading represents a related response to stress coded by rostral versus caudal shell GABAergic substrates. All such conjectures need to be evaluated by future research, but it seems clear at least that the treading behavior elicited by muscimol in caudal accumbens shell was an active defensive response (rather than a motor reflex or a passive withdrawal response) emitted to repel, diminish, and sometimes bury a fearful stimulus.

Localization of function within accumbens shell

Our results revealed rostrocaudal segregation of behavioral valence coded by GABAA substrates in the accumbens shell. Eating is typically viewed as a positive or appetitive behavior and was elicited only by GABAA agonist microinjections in the rostral shell (1.2–2.7 mm anterior to bregma). This region corresponds to the same coordinates at which Kelly and colleagues found muscimol-elicited eating (Stratford and Kelley, 1997;Basso and Kelley, 1999). In contrast, we also found that microinjections in caudal shell, behind the feeding site, not only failed to increase food intake but actually decreased eating behavior.

Highest amounts of negative defensive burying behavior are elicited predominately by GABAA receptor stimulation of caudal shell regions (AP, +1.2 to +0.48 mm). Slight defensive treading was also elicited by high doses at rostral sites but at much lower intensity than at caudal sites. It is as yet unclear whether the slight treading behavior after rostral microinjection results directly from action on rostral receptors there or instead from drug diffusion to more caudal receptors. Thus, GABA substrates in rostral regions of the medial shell trigger an apparently positive motivated behavior, whereas GABA substrates in caudal shell trigger an apparently negative motivated behavior. Microinjection sites in the accumbens core or other structures outside the shell failed to elicit either behavior.

Functional interaction between neuroanatomical and neurochemical coding

Our rostral shell region for GABAergic eating behavior overlaps considerably with an earlier map by our laboratory of an opioid eating site in the shell, in which morphine microinjection caused increased food intake (Peciña and Berridge, 2000). However, the appetitive opioid eating site of Peciña and Berridge (+1.0 to +2.2 mm anterior to bregma) also extended posteriorly into our caudal shell region in which GABAergic muscimol elicited negative defensive burying. An overlap between positive and negative motivational systems in caudal shell is also consistent with our previous finding using a pure conditioned incentive paradigm that amphetamine microinjection in this same caudal shell site increases appetitive cue-triggered bar pressing for a sucrose reward (Wyvell and Berridge, 2000) and with findings of greatest reward effects in place preference and brain self-stimulation paradigms after histamine receptor blockade in the caudal shell (Zimmermann et al., 1999). These comparisons indicate that positive–negative function is determined interactively by neurochemical receptor activation, as well as by neuroanatomical localization of function within the nucleus accumbens shell.

Neurobiological bases of rostrocaudal shell segregation of appetitive–defensive function

Rostral versus caudal portions of the accumbens shell differ in cell morphology, connectivity, and neurochemical organization (Herkenham et al., 1984; Phillipson and Griffiths, 1985; Oades and Halliday, 1987; Zahm and Brog, 1992; Brog et al., 1993; Groenewegen et al., 1993; Zahm and Heimer, 1993; Voorn et al., 1994; Gorelova and Yang, 1997; Usuda et al., 1998). Although the entire shell receives afferent projections from the prefrontal cortex, subiculum, amygdala, ventral pallidum, lateral hypothalamus, ventral tegmental area, etc., the rostral shell receives denser innervation from the subiculum and basolateral amygdala, whereas the caudal shell receives sparser projections from those structures (Phillipson and Griffiths, 1985; Brog et al., 1993). Furthermore, segregated projections from different regions within afferent structures that project differentially to rostral and caudal shell may be another source of functional variance (Oades and Halliday, 1987; Groenewegen et al., 1993; Gorelova and Yang, 1997). Regarding efferent projections, both rostral and caudal shell regions project to the ventral pallidum, lateral hypothalamus, ventral tegmental area, substantia nigra compacta, pedunculopontine nuclei, and periaqueductal gray area. However, the rostral shell may send denser efferents to the lateral preoptic area, globus pallidus, and substantia nigra pars reticulata, whereas the caudal shell sends denser projections to anterior regions of the extended amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and the locus ceruleus (Heimer et al., 1991; Zahm and Heimer, 1993; Usuda et al., 1998).

Neurochemically, the rostral shell has higher levels of D1 and D2 mRNA and opioid enkephalin mRNA (Bardo and Hammer, 1991; Voorn and Docter, 1992). In contrast, the caudal shell has higher levels of cholecystokinin, acetylcholinesterase, vasopressin–oxytocin receptors, and greater norepinephrine and serotonin innervation (Zaborszky et al., 1985; Meredith et al., 1989; Zhou et al., 1991;Tribollet et al., 1992; Berridge et al., 1997; Veinante and Freund-Mercier, 1997; Delfs et al., 1998). These various neurochemical and neuroanatomical differences between rostral versus caudal shell may contribute to the functional differences we have reported here.

Conclusion

Motivational functions are segregated within the nucleus accumbens shell. GABAA receptor activation in the rostral accumbens shell elicits food intake (especially at relatively low doses), whereas GABAA receptor activation in the caudal shell elicits defensive treading behavior (especially at high doses). The rostrocaudal segregation of positive eating versus negative defensive behavior by GABAergic systems indicates that the nucleus accumbens shell may heterogeneously code behavioral function and motivational valence.

Footnotes

  • This research was supported by National Science Foundation Grant IBN 9604408 to K.C.B. and National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Training Grant T32 DC00011 to S.M.R. We thank Prof. Craig Berridge for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript and Profs. Donald Owings and Eugene Morton for providing the illustration of anti-rattlesnake defensive treading.

    Correspondence should be addressed to Sheila M. Reynolds, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109. E-mail:sheilar{at}umich.edu.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Antelman SM,
    2. Szechtman H,
    3. Chin P,
    4. Fisher AE
    (1975) Tail pinch-induced eating, gnawing and licking behavior in rats: dependence on the nigrostriatal dopamine system. Brain Res 99:219–237.
    OpenUrl
  2. ↵
    1. Bardo MT,
    2. Hammer RP Jr.
    (1991) Autoradiographic localization of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in rat nucleus accumbens: resistance to differential rearing conditions. Neuroscience 45:281–290.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Basso AM,
    2. Kelley AE
    (1999) Feeding induced by GABA(A) receptor stimulation within the nucleus accumbens shell: regional mapping and characterization of macronutrient and taste preference. Behav Neuroscience 113:324–336.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Beck CH,
    2. Fibiger HC
    (1995) Conditioned fear-induced changes in behavior and in the expression of the immediate early gene c-fos: with and without diazepam pretreatment. J Neurosci 15:709–720.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Berridge KC,
    2. Robinson TE
    (1998) What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Rev 28:309–369.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Berridge CW,
    2. Stratford TL,
    3. Foote SL,
    4. Kelley AE
    (1997) Distribution of dopamine beta-hydroxylase-like immunoreactive fibers within the shell subregion of the nucleus accumbens. Synapse 27:230–241.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Berridge CW,
    2. Mitton E,
    3. Clark W,
    4. Roth RH
    (1999) Engagement in a non-escape (displacement) behavior elicits a selective and lateralized suppression of frontal cortical dopaminergic utilization in stress. Synapse 32:187–197.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Bertolucci-D'Angio M,
    2. Serrano A,
    3. Scatton B
    (1990) Differential effects of forced locomotion, tail-pinch, immobilization, and methyl-beta-carboline carboxylate on extracellular 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid levels in the rat striatum, nucleus accumbens, and prefrontal cortex: an in vivo voltammetric study. J Neurochem 55:1208–1215.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Bolles RC,
    2. Fanselow MS
    (1980) A perceptual-defensive-recuperative model of fear and pain. Behav Brain Sci 3:291–323.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    1. Brog JS,
    2. Salyapongse A,
    3. Deutch AY,
    4. Zahm DS
    (1993) The patterns of afferent innervation of the core and shell in the “accumbens” part of the rat ventral striatum: immunohistochemical detection of retrogradely transported fluoro-gold. J Comp Neurol 338:255–278.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Carlezon WA Jr.,
    2. Wise RA
    (1996) Rewarding actions of phencyclidine and related drugs in nucleus accumbens shell and frontal cortex. J Neurosci 16:3112–3122.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. D'Angio M,
    2. Serrano A,
    3. Rivy JP,
    4. Scatton B
    (1987) Tail-pinch stress increases extracellular DOPAC levels (as measured by in vivo voltammetry) in the rat nucleus accumbens but not frontal cortex: antagonism by diazepam and zolpidem. Brain Res 409:169–174.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Davis M,
    2. Lee Y
    (1998) Fear and anxiety: possible roles of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Cognit Emotion 12:277–305.
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    1. Delfs JM,
    2. Zhu Y,
    3. Druhan JP,
    4. Aston-Jones GS
    (1998) Origin of noradrenergic afferents to the shell subregion of the nucleus accumbens: anterograde and retrograde tract-tracing studies in the rat. Brain Res 806:127–140.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Fanselow MS,
    2. LeDoux JE
    (1999) Why we think plasticity underlying Pavlovian fear conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala. Neuron 23:229–232.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Gorelova N,
    2. Yang CR
    (1997) The course of neural projection from the prefrontal cortex to the nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience 76:689–706.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Gray JA
    (1995) Dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens: the perspective from aberrations of consciousness in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia 33:1143–1153.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Gray JA,
    2. McNaughton N
    (1996) The neuropsychology of anxiety: reprise. Nebr Symp Motiv 43:61–134.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Groenewegen HJ,
    2. Berendse HW,
    3. Haber SN
    (1993) Organization of the output of the ventral striatopallidal system in the rat: ventral pallidal efferents. Neuroscience 57:113–142.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Heimer L,
    2. Zahm DS,
    3. Churchill L,
    4. Kalivas PW,
    5. Wohltmann C
    (1991) Specificity in the projection patterns of accumbal core and shell in the rat. Neuroscience 41:89–125.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Herkenham M,
    2. Edley SM,
    3. Stuart J
    (1984) Cell clusters in the nucleus accumbens of the rat, and the mosaic relationship of opiate receptors, acetylcholinesterase and subcortical afferent terminations. Neuroscience 11:561–593.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Inoue T,
    2. Tsuchiya K,
    3. Koyama T
    (1994) Regional changes in dopamine and serotonin activation with various intensity of physical and psychological stress in the rat brain. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 49:911–920.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Kagan J,
    2. Schulkin J
    (1995) On the concepts of fear. Harv Rev Psychiatry 3:231–234.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Kalivas PW,
    2. Duffy P
    (1995) Selective activation of dopamine transmission in the shell of the nucleus accumbens by stress. Brain Res 675:325–328.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Killcross S,
    2. Robbins TW,
    3. Everitt BJ
    (1997) Different types of fear-conditioned behaviour mediated by separate nuclei within amygdala. Nature 388:377–380.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Kopchia KL,
    2. Altman HJ,
    3. Commissaris RL
    (1992) Effects of lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala on anxiety-like behaviors in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 43:453–461.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. LeDoux J
    (1998) Fear and the brain: where have we been, and where are we going? Biol Psychiatry 44:1229–1238.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Lehmann H,
    2. Treit D,
    3. Parent MB
    (2000) Amygdala lesions do not impair shock-probe avoidance retention performance. Behav Neurosci 114:107–116.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Liberzon I,
    2. Taylor SF,
    3. Amdur R,
    4. Jung TD,
    5. Chamberlain KR,
    6. Minoshima S,
    7. Koeppe RA,
    8. Fig LM
    (1999) Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biol Psychiatry 45:817–826.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Londei T,
    2. Valentini AM,
    3. Leone VG
    (1998) Investigative burying by laboratory mice may involve non-functional, compulsive, behaviour. Behav Brain Res 94:249–254.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Maldonado-Irizarry CS,
    2. Swanson CJ,
    3. Kelley AE
    (1995) Glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell control feeding behavior via the lateral hypothalamus. J Neurosci 15:6779–6788.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Maren S
    (1999) Long-term potentiation in the amygdala: a mechanism for emotional learning and memory. Trends Neurosci 22:561–567.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Marks IM,
    2. Nesse RM
    (1994) Fear and fitness: an evolutionary analysis of anxiety disorders. Special issue: Mental disorders in an evolutionary context. Ethol Sociobiol 15:247–261.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    1. McCullough LD,
    2. Salamone JD
    (1992) Anxiogenic drugs beta-CCE and FG 7142 increase extracellular dopamine levels in nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology 109:379–382.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Meredith GE,
    2. Blank B,
    3. Groenewegen HJ
    (1989) The distribution and compartmental organization of the cholinergic neurons in nucleus accumbens of the rat. Neuroscience 31:327–345.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Oades RD,
    2. Halliday GM
    (1987) Ventral tegmental (A10) system: neurobiology. I. Anatomy and connectivity. Brain Res 434:117–165.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Owings DH,
    2. Coss RG
    (1977) Snake mobbing by California ground squirrels: adaptive variation and ontogeny. Behaviour 62:50–69.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  38. ↵
    1. Parker LA
    (1988) Defensive burying of flavors paired with lithium but not amphetamine. Psychopharmacology 96:250–252.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Paxinos G,
    2. Watson C
    (1986) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates, Ed 2. (Academic, Sydney).
  40. ↵
    1. Peciña S,
    2. Berridge KC
    (2000) Opioid eating site in accumbens shell mediates food intake and hedonic “liking”: map based on microinjection Fos plumes. Brain Res 863:71–86.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Phillipson OT,
    2. Griffiths AC
    (1985) The topographic order of inputs to nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience 16:275–296.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Pinel JPJ,
    2. Jones CH,
    3. Whishaw IQ
    (1992) Behavior from the ground up: rat behavior from the ventral perspective. Psychobiology 20:185–188.
    OpenUrl
  43. ↵
    1. Rodgers RJ,
    2. Cao BJ,
    3. Dalvi A,
    4. Holmes A
    (1997) Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective. Braz J Med Biol Res 30:289–304.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Rosen JB,
    2. Schulkin J
    (1998) From normal fear to pathological anxiety. Psychol Rev 105:325–350.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Salamone JD,
    2. Cousins MS,
    3. Snyder BJ
    (1997) Behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine: empirical and conceptual problems with the anhedonia hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 21:341–359.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Saul'skaya NB,
    2. Marsden CA
    (1998) gamma-Aminobutyric acid levels in the intercellular space in the nucleus accumbens of the rat brain during a nociceptive conditioned response. Neurosci Behav Physiol 28:60–64.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Stratford TR,
    2. Kelley AE
    (1997) GABA in the nucleus accumbens shell participates in the central regulation of feeding behavior. J Neurosci 17:4434–4440.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Stratford TR,
    2. Kelley AE
    (1999) Evidence of a functional relationship between the nucleus accumbens shell and lateral hypothalamus subserving the control of feeding behavior. J Neurosci 19:11040–11048.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    1. Thierry AM,
    2. Tassin JP,
    3. Blanc G,
    4. Glowinski J
    (1976) Selective activation of mesocortical DA system by stress. Nature 263:242–244.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Treit D
    (1985) The inhibitory effect of diazepam on defensive burying: anxiolytic vs. analgesic effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 22:47–52.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Treit D,
    2. Pinel JP,
    3. Fibiger HC
    (1981) Conditioned defensive burying: a new paradigm for the study of anxiolytic agents. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 15:619–626.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Treit D,
    2. Pesold C,
    3. Rotzinger S
    (1993) Dissociating the anti-fear effects of septal and amygdaloid lesions using two pharmacologically validated models of rat anxiety. Behav Neurosci 107:770–785.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Treit D,
    2. Aujla H,
    3. Menard J
    (1998) Does the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis mediate fear behaviors? Behav Neurosci 112:379–386.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Tribollet E,
    2. Barberis C,
    3. Dubois-Dauphin M,
    4. Dreifuss JJ
    (1992) Localization and characterization of binding sites for vasopressin and oxytocin in the brain of the guinea pig. Brain Res 589:15–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Usuda I,
    2. Tanaka K,
    3. Chiba T
    (1998) Efferent projections of the nucleus accumbens in the rat with special reference to subdivision of the nucleus: biotinylated dextran amine study. Brain Res 797:73–93.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Veinante P,
    2. Freund-Mercier MJ
    (1997) Distribution of oxytocin- and vasopressin-binding sites in the rat extended amygdala: a histoautoradiographic study. J Comp Neurol 383:305–325.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Voorn P,
    2. Docter GJ
    (1992) A rostrocaudal gradient in the synthesis of enkephalin in nucleus accumbens. NeuroReport 3:161–164.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Voorn P,
    2. Docter GJ,
    3. Jongen-Relo AL,
    4. Jonker AJ
    (1994) Rostrocaudal subregional differences in the response of enkephalin, dynorphin and substance P synthesis in rat nucleus accumbens to dopamine depletion. Eur J Neurosci 6:486–496.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Wilkie DM,
    2. MacLennan AJ,
    3. Pinel JP
    (1979) Rat defensive behavior: burying noxious food. J Exp Anal Behav 31:299–306.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Wyvell CL,
    2. Berridge KC
    (2000) Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the pure incentive salience of a Pavlovian cue for food reward: enhancement of “wanting” without either “liking” or reinforcement. J Neurosci 20:8122–8130.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Young AM,
    2. Ahier RG,
    3. Upton RL,
    4. Joseph MH,
    5. Gray JA
    (1998) Increased extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of the rat during associative learning of neutral stimuli. Neuroscience 83:1175–1183.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Zaborszky L,
    2. Alheid GF,
    3. Beinfeld MC,
    4. Eiden LE,
    5. Heimer L,
    6. Palkovits M
    (1985) Cholecystokinin innervation of the ventral striatum: a morphological and radioimmunological study. Neuroscience 14:427–453.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Zahm DS
    (2000) An integrative neuroanatomical perspective on some subcortical substrates of adaptive responding with emphasis on the nucleus accumbens. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24:85–105.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Zahm DS,
    2. Brog JS
    (1992) On the significance of subterritories in the “accumbens” part of the rat ventral striatum. Neuroscience 50:751–767.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Zahm DS,
    2. Heimer L
    (1993) Specificity in the efferent projections of the nucleus accumbens in the rat: comparison of the rostral pole projection patterns with those of the core and shell. J Comp Neurol 327:220–232.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Zhou FC,
    2. Bledsoe S,
    3. Lumeng L,
    4. Li TK
    (1991) Immunostained serotonergic fibers are decreased in selected brain regions of alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol 8:425–431.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Zimmermann P,
    2. Privou C,
    3. Huston JP
    (1999) Differential sensitivity of the caudal and rostral nucleus accumbens to the rewarding effects of a H1-histaminergic receptor blocker as measured with place-preference and self-stimulation behavior. Neuroscience 94:93–103.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 21 (9)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 21, Issue 9
1 May 2001
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Fear and Feeding in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell: Rostrocaudal Segregation of GABA-Elicited Defensive Behavior Versus Eating Behavior
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Fear and Feeding in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell: Rostrocaudal Segregation of GABA-Elicited Defensive Behavior Versus Eating Behavior
Sheila M. Reynolds, Kent C. Berridge
Journal of Neuroscience 1 May 2001, 21 (9) 3261-3270; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-09-03261.2001

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Fear and Feeding in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell: Rostrocaudal Segregation of GABA-Elicited Defensive Behavior Versus Eating Behavior
Sheila M. Reynolds, Kent C. Berridge
Journal of Neuroscience 1 May 2001, 21 (9) 3261-3270; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-09-03261.2001
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • Experiment 3: comparison of muscimol-elicited treading with “real defensive treading” elicited by electric shock prod
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • accumbens shell
  • GABA
  • food intake
  • reward
  • appetite
  • motivation
  • glutamate
  • dopamine
  • fear
  • defense
  • aggression
  • mesolimbic
  • microinjection

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

ARTICLE

  • Neural Correlates of Competing Fear Behaviors Evoked by an Innately Aversive Stimulus
  • Sp1 and Sp3 Are Oxidative Stress-Inducible, Antideath Transcription Factors in Cortical Neurons
  • Distinct Developmental Modes and Lesion-Induced Reactions of Dendrites of Two Classes of Drosophila Sensory Neurons
Show more ARTICLE

Behavioral/Systems

  • Vocal Control Neuron Incorporation Decreases with Age in the Adult Zebra Finch
  • Modulation by Central and Basolateral Amygdalar Nuclei of Dopaminergic Correlates of Feeding to Satiety in the Rat Nucleus Accumbens and Medial Prefrontal Cortex
  • Confocal Analysis of Reciprocal Feedback at Rod Bipolar Terminals in the Rabbit Retina
Show more Behavioral/Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2021 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.