Abstract
Coordination of two sets of movements, protraction–retraction versus opening–closing, of the feeding apparatus (the radula) in ingestive and egestive motor programs of Aplysiaresembles vertebrate intralimb coordination in that the relative timing of the two sets of movements differs in the two motor programs. In both ingestion and egestion, radula protraction and retraction alternate, whereas radula closure shifts its phase relative to protraction–retraction. In egestion, the radula closes in protraction; in ingestion, the radula closes in retraction. In both ingestive and egestive motor programs elicited by the command-like neuron, cerebral-buccal interneuron-2 (CBI-2), the protraction and retraction movements are mediated by the same sets of controller interneurons. In contrast, radula closure is mediated by two controller interneurons, B20 and B40, that are preferentially active in egestion and ingestion, respectively. In egestion, B20, active in protraction, drives closure motorneuron B8 in protraction, whereas in ingestion, B40, also active in protraction, uses a functionally novel mechanism, fast inhibition and slow excitation, to drive B8 in retraction. Our findings are summarized in a neural model that permits a conceptual comparison of our model with two previous hypothetical models of intralimb coordination in spinal circuits that were proposed by Grillner (1981,1985) and Berkowitz and Stein (1994). Although our model supports the existence of separate controllers for different movements as in theGrillner (1981, 1985) model; in terms of basic mechanisms, our model is similar to the Berkowitz and Stein (1994) model because the closure movement is mediated by separate controllers in different programs, and thus both models can be classified as recruitment models.
- Aplysia
- feeding
- ingestion
- egestion
- central pattern generator
- mollusc
- motor program
- motor pattern selection
- interneuron
- intralimb coordination
Complex motor acts often involve coordinated movements of multiple sets of agonist and antagonist muscles (Grillner, 1981, 1985; Stein and Smith, 1997; Orlovsky et al., 1999). Animals can produce multiple, related behaviors, e.g., different forms of locomotion, rostral, pocket, and caudal scratch, ingestion, and egestion (Kupfermann, 1974; Grillner, 1981; Mortin et al., 1985;Buford et al., 1990; Grasso et al., 1998), by generating distinct coordinated motor patterns, referred to as motor programs (Ayers and Davis, 1977) or forms of a motor task (Mortin et al., 1985). One common strategy of generating distinct motor programs consists of changing the relative timing of different sets of movements (Mortin et al., 1985;Smith et al., 1985, 1998a; Chrachri and Clarac, 1990; Morton and Chiel, 1993a; Combes et al., 1999).
In principle, different forms of coordination of multiple sets of movements could be implemented in a variety of ways. Two hypothetical models for intralimb coordination in spinal circuits (Stein and Smith, 1997) were proposed. Grillner (1981, 1985) proposed that agonist muscles (e.g., around one joint) are activated by a controller, termed a unit burst generator, or a module (Jordan, 1991; Stein et al., 1995). Coordination of sets of joint movements in different behaviors can be achieved by switching the sign of the phase coupling between the controllers of different joints of the limb through descending inputs. Grillner's (1981, 1985) hypothesis has been used to explain multiple forms of cat walking (Smith et al., 1998a,b). The second model (Berkowitz and Stein, 1994) was proposed to explain the observation that during a rostral scratch in turtle, knee extension occurs during hip flexion, whereas during pocket scratch, knee extension occurs during hip extension (Mortin et al., 1985; Robertson et al., 1985). The model states that in rostral scratch, the knee extensor is activated by the controllers for hip flexor, whereas in pocket scratch, the knee extensor is activated by controllers for hip extensor.
Because of the large number of interneurons involved, these models are difficult to verify in spinal circuits. The Aplysia CNS is amenable to testing of specific models of coordination (Jing and Weiss, 2001). In Aplysia, two forms of feeding, ingestion and egestion, involve different coordination patterns of two sets of movements of the feeding apparatus (radula), protraction and retraction versus opening and closing. In both motor programs, whenAplysia transitions from a quiescent to an active state, protraction occurs first and is followed by retraction. However, in ingestion, the radula closes during retraction to pull food in; in egestion, the radula closes during protraction to push inedible objects out (Morton and Chiel, 1993a). Thus, we consider the protraction–retraction movements to be phase-fixed movements (PFMs), and we consider the radula closure movement to be a phase-shifting movement (PSM). In ingestion and egestion, the same sets of interneurons mediate the PFMs (Jing et al., 1999; Jing and Weiss, 2001). We now show that the PSM in ingestion is mediated by a separate interneuron from the interneuron that mediates the PSM in egestion (Jing and Weiss, 2001). Comparison with the Grillner (1981, 1985) andBerkowitz and Stein (1994) models suggests that Aplysiacircuits display both similarities and differences with the two spinal models.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Specimens of Aplysia californica were obtained from Marinus (Long Beach, CA) and from the National Resource forAplysia at the University of Miami. They were maintained in circulating artificial seawater (ASW), made from Instant Ocean (Aquarium Systems, Mentor, OH) at 14–15°C. Animals weighing 50–250 gm were anesthetized by injection (50% of the body weight) of isotonic MgCl2 (337 mm). Cerebral ganglia together with buccal ganglia were dissected out and desheathed. The ganglia were then pinned in a chamber that had a volume of ∼1.5 ml. The preparation was continuously perfused with ASW (in mm: 460 NaCl, 10 KCl, 55 MgCl2, 11 CaCl2, and 10 HEPES buffer), pH 7.6, at a rate of 0.3 ml/min, and maintained at 14–17°C. All chemicals were purchased from Sigma (St. Louis, MO).
Standard intracellular and extracellular recordings were obtained as described (Jing and Weiss, 2001). Digitized data were plotted with Axum (Mathsoft, Cambridge, MA). Functional synaptic connections were examined in normal saline, and the ability of postsynaptic potentials (PSPs) to follow presynaptic spikes one-for-one was taken as an indication of probable monosynapticity. Assays of monosynapticity were conducted in high-divalent saline (in mm: 312 NaCl, 10 KCl, 132 MgCl2, 33 CaCl2, and 10 HEPES), pH 7.6, a solution that elevates spike thresholds and thus curtails polysynaptic activation. Neurons were identified based on location, size, electrophysiological, and morphological characteristics (Jing and Weiss, 2001).
Throughout the results, n refers to the number of preparations, unless otherwise stated.
RESULTS
Differential activity of B40 and B20 in ingestion and egestion
Ingestive and egestive behaviors of Aplysia were initially observed in behaving animals (Kupfermann, 1974). The specific timing of the two sets of radula movements, protraction and retraction versus opening and closing, involved in these behaviors has been analyzed in intact animals with video recording (Morton and Chiel, 1993a) that showed that in ingestion, the radula closes during retraction; whereas in egestion, the radula closes during protraction. These observations have been verified in reduced preparations in which muscles or motor nerves that mediate the radula movements have been recorded (Cropper et al., 1990; Morton and Chiel, 1993b). Numerous other studies have shown that these timing differences observed in actual behaviors can also be extended to neural activity observed in fictive ingestive and egestive motor programs in the isolated CNS (Susswein and Byrne, 1988; Church and Lloyd, 1994; Hurwitz et al., 1997; Nargeot et al., 1997; Kabotyanski et al., 1998; Sanchez and Kirk, 2000; Jing and Weiss, 2001; Morgan et al., 2002). In the present study, we elicited ingestive and egestive motor programs through stimulation of the cerebral-buccal interneuron-2 (CBI-2; Rosen et al., 1991). CBI-2 is a command-like interneuron that is activated by appropriate sensory stimuli. Furthermore, when CBI-2 is activated through current injection, it can elicit both ingestive and egestive motor programs (Rosen et al., 1991; Church and Lloyd, 1994; Jing and Weiss, 2001;Morgan et al., 2002). In a given preparation, however, if the stimulation paradigm remains the same, the motor programs elicited by stimulation of CBI-2 stabilize after several episodes of CBI-2 stimulation, and the type of motor programs elicited by stimulation of CBI-2 alone remains the same thereafter throughout the experiment.
The radula protraction, retraction, and closing movements are mediated by feeding interneurons located in the buccal ganglion. Previous data indicate that the buccal interneurons, which control protraction (B34, B63) (Hurwitz et al., 1997) and retraction (B64) (Hurwitz and Susswein, 1996) are active in both ingestive and egestive motor programs (Jing et al., 1999; Jing and Weiss, 2001). Furthermore, the interneuron B20, active in protraction, is the major driver of radula closure motorneuron B8 in egestive motor programs (Jing and Weiss, 2001). An interneuron that controls B8 in ingestive motor programs, however, has not been identified. Because B8 is active in the retraction phase, we hypothesized that the interneuron that controls radula closure in ingestion may exhibit the following properties: (1) the interneuron should be preferentially active in ingestive rather than egestive motor programs, (2) this neuron should be active in the retraction phase, and (3) this neuron should excite directly radula closure motor neurons through fast excitation. Our extensive search for an interneuron that meets all of the above criteria was unsuccessful. We did, however, find a neuron, which we named B40, that plays a major role in ingestive motor programs, but is active in protraction, rather than retraction, of the motor programs. B40 is located on the caudal surface of the buccal ganglion, posteriorly to B34, and is a bilaterally symmetrical cell. Similar to B63 and B34, B40 is a buccal-cerebral interneuron that sends its sole axon out of the buccal ganglion to the cerebral ganglion through the contralateral cerebral-buccal connective (CBC).
In the feeding motor programs elicited by CBI-2 (Figs.1, 2), protraction is monitored by activity in the I2 nerve, which contains the axons of protraction motor neurons B31/32 and B61/62 (Hurwitz et al., 1996). Retraction, mediated by the retraction-phase interneuron B64, is monitored by periods of hyperpolarization in either B40 or B20 (Jing and Weiss, 2001) after the end of protraction. The types of motor programs can be determined based on the activity pattern of the radula closure motor neuron B8. In ingestive motor programs, B8 is weakly active in protraction and strongly active in retraction, whereas in egestive motor programs, B8 is strongly active in protraction and is not active or is weakly active in retraction (Fig. 1, compareA,B). Based on the quantitative study of B8 activity patterns in a large number of motor programs (compare Fig. 1C), CBI-2 was recently found to produce a third type of motor programs, in which B8 fires at similar rates in protraction and retraction, and which was named the intermediate (ambiguous) program (Morgan et al., 2002).
Like interneurons B63, B34, and B20, B40 is active in the protraction phase of motor programs, but unlike these interneurons, it is preferentially active in ingestive motor programs (Fig.1A, C1). We have shown that B20 is more active in egestion than in ingestion (Jing and Weiss, 2001), thus, B40 activity level is opposite to that of B20 in the two types of motor programs (Fig. 1B, C2). To further examine the causal relationship between activity levels of B40 and B20 and the types of motor programs, we quantified the average firing rate of B8 during protraction and retraction as well as the average firing rate of B40 and B20 in different types of motor programs. These motor programs were elicited by stimulation of CBI-2 with DC or current pulses and are from 47 episodes recorded from 32 preparations. The data are plotted in three-dimensional (3-D) graphs (Fig. 1C). In the graphs, thex–y axis is B8 firing frequency in protraction and retraction, and the z-axis is B40 or B20 firing frequency. Based on a 2-D plot of B8 activity in protraction and retraction in different motor programs, Morgan et al. (2002) have used cluster analysis to classify these motor programs into three groups: ingestive (low B8 firing in protraction and high B8 firing in retraction), egestive (high B8 firing in protraction and low B8 firing in retraction), intermediate (similar B8 firing in protraction and retraction). Similarly, the data points in the 3-D plots can also be grouped into three clusters along the x–y axis plane. Specifically, along the x–y axis plane, the data points that are located at top-left corner are ingestive, whereas the data points that are located at the bottom-right corner are egestive. Three-dimensional regression planes, generated using ordinary least-squares (OLS) to calculate Z values for a given X and Y, are drawn to show the relationship of B40/B20 activity with different types of motor programs. Most data points fell in the regression planes (shown as only the dots) or close by (shown as the dots with a vertical line that joins the dot with the regression plane, the shorter the line, the better the fit), suggesting the regression provides a good fit. For B40, when the data points are in the top-left corner (ingestive), B40 activity is the highest (∼11 Hz), and as the programs shift from top-left corner toward the bottom-right corner (egestion), B40 activity drops (the regression plane declines). In contrast, B20 activity is the highest (∼11 Hz) when the data points are clustered in bottom-right corner (egestive) and as the programs shift from the bottom-right corner toward the top-left corner (ingestion), B20 activity drops precipitously (the regression plane declines). Finally, we calculated the average frequency of B40/B20 in the three groups. Average firing frequency (± SEM) of B40 in ingestive motor programs is 9.85 ± 0.33 (n = 12), in intermediate programs is 7.93 ± 0.57 (n = 3), and in egestive programs is 5.32 ± 0.73 (n = 9). Average firing frequency of B20 in egestive motor programs is 9.41 ± 0.61 (n = 11), in intermediate programs is 5.21 ± 0.53 (n = 2), and in ingestive programs is 2.89 ± 0.75 (n = 10). Thus, these data provide strong supporting evidence that B40 and B20 may be important controllers of radula closure motor neuron B8 for ingestive and egestive motor programs, respectively.
Functional role of B40 in ingestive motor programs
In the earlier report (Jing and Weiss, 2001), we showed that B20s are necessary for strong B8 firing during protraction in egestive motor programs by hyperpolarizing bilateral B20s. In the second series of the current experiments, we performed similar experiments on B40 to determine if B40 is indeed an important controller of B8 phasing in ingestion. Ingestive motor programs elicited by CBI-2 were monitored by activity in the radula closer B8 (which fired weakly in protraction and fired strongly in retraction). B40s fired at high frequency during protraction (Fig. 2A1). However, when B40s were bilaterally hyperpolarized, B8 fired more during protraction and fired less during retraction (Fig. 2A2), and the program became intermediate rather than ingestive (compare Fig.3). B8 activity returned to the ingestive pattern when B40s were not hyperpolarized in the third motor program elicited by CBI-2 (Fig. 2A3).
To determine whether the effect of B40 on the B8 firing pattern in ingestive motor programs is specific, we also performed similar experiments on another protraction-phase interneuron, B34. We have shown earlier (Jing and Weiss, 2001) that B34s are similarly active in both ingestion and egestion elicited by CBI-2, and in another study (Hurwitz et al., 1997), B34 has been shown to provide strong synaptic excitation to motor neurons that produce radula protraction movements. The evidence suggests that B34 is an important mediator for protraction movements. Bilateral hyperpolarization of B34s in ingestive motor programs had a minimum impact on B8 firing pattern in ingestion (Fig.2B). This indicates that B34 is not an important controller element of radula closure for ingestion, consistent with the earlier study (Jing and Weiss, 2001).
The effects of B40 (n = 5) and B34 (n = 3) hyperpolarization on B8 firing pattern in CBI-2 elicited ingestive motor programs are summarized in a plot shown in Figure 3(x-axis, B8 firing frequency in protraction vsy-axis, B8 firing frequency in retraction). In the plot, each experiment is represented by two data points connected by a line (dashed lines for B40, straight lines for B34). For each pair of data points, the top one represents the data obtained before hyperpolarization of B40 or B34, and the bottom one represents data obtained during hyperpolarization of B40 or B34. Dotted lines encircling the data points show three distinct clusters derived from cluster analysis (Morgan et al., 2002) that represent ingestive, intermediate, and egestive motor programs (compare Fig. 1C). Thus, all the motor programs were ingestive before hyperpolarization of B40 or B34. Hyperpolarization of B40s shifted B8 activity patterns from top-left corner to bottom-right direction, i.e., from ingestive programs to intermediate programs. In other words, B8 now fired at similar rates in both protraction and retraction, which is characteristic of intermediate programs. Note that all B40 hyperpolarization experiments were conducted in preparations in which CBI-2 stimulation consistently elicited ingestive motor programs, because the purpose of these experiments was to examine the functional roles of B40 in ingestion. Overall, in this study (47 episodes in 32 preparations), we found that under our conditions of CBI-2 stimulation, without B40 hyperpolarization, 46.8% of motor patterns elicited by CBI-2 stimulation were ingestive, 42.6% of motor patterns were egestive, and 10.6% of motor patterns were intermediate. Although the B8 firing was altered somewhat when B34s were hyperpolarized, the motor programs remained ingestive. Thus, B40, but not B34, is critical for expression of ingestive motor programs elicited by CBI-2.
Control of the radula closure motoneuron B8 by the interneuron B40
To examine how B40 controls the activity of the radula closure B8, we studied the synaptic connection from B40 to B8. The PSPs that B40 elicited in B8 had dual components, fast IPSPs following presynaptic spikes one-for-one and slow EPSPs occurring at a delay from the onset of B40 activity (n = 27) (Fig.4). As shown in Figures 2 and 3, hyperpolarization of B40 causes more B8 firing in protraction and less B8 firing in retraction, indicating that the normal function of B40 is to reduce B8 firing in protraction when B40 is active and promote B8 firing in retraction when B40 ceases firing. The dual function can be efficiently accomplished by the dual-component PSPs. Because the fast IPSPs followed B40 presynaptic spikes one for one (Fig.4A2, B2), and both the fast IPSPs and slow EPSPs persisted in high-divalent saline (n = 18) (Fig.4B,C), both components of the PSPs from B40 to B8 are monosynaptic. The slow EPSPs were not obvious when B40 was stimulated for a brief time (≤250 msec) (Fig. 4C1,C2) and began to appear when B40 was stimulated for >500 msec (Fig.4C3–6). This suggested that slow EPSPs may not follow B40 spikes one-for-one and may require multiple B40 spikes. We also examined if postinhibitory rebound of B8 may contribute to the slow depolarization that is most prominent when B40 ceases firing because during B40 stimulation, B8 is hyperpolarized by the fast IPSPs. Toward this goal, we delivered hyperpolarizing current pulses of the same duration and similar amplitude with the hyperpolarizations caused by B40 stimulation, and we found that the postinhibitory rebound was too small to account for the slow depolarization (Fig.4C4-C6). This experiment indicates that the slow depolarization represents slow synaptic potential that is elicited by B40.
To further characterize the properties of the PSPs that B40 elicits in B8, we examined whether the PSPs are associated with conductance change in B8 (n = 3) and whether they are dependent on the membrane potentials of B8 (n = 4). We measured the B8 conductance by applying a small hyperpolarizing current pulse (< 0.5 nA) of 500 msec duration at 0.5 Hz (Fig.5A). During B40 stimulation when the fast IPSPs were most prominent, the voltage deflections in B8 caused by the current pulses became smaller indicating that the fast IPSPs are associated with conductance increase. On the other hand, immediately after B40 stimulation when the slow EPSPs were most prominent, the voltage deflections in B8 caused by current pulses became larger indicating that the slow EPSPs are associated with conductance decrease. These conductance changes in B8 elicited by B40 are present in both normal saline (Fig. 5A1) and high-divalent saline (Fig. 5A2). To test whether B8 membrane potentials affect the PSPs elicited by B40, we stimulated B40 with brief current pulses for 1 sec at 20 Hz (Fig. 5B) and changed B8 membrane potential by current injection through a second electrode. The fast IPSPs reversed to become depolarizing potentials when B8 membrane potentials were held below −80 mV. However, the slow EPSPs became smaller when B8 was more hyperpolarized and did not reverse even when B8 membrane potentials were held at −90 mV.
Previously, it was reported that B34 also elicited fast IPSPs and slow EPSPs in B8 (Hurwitz et al., 1997). The slow EPSPs were also found to be associated with conductance decrease similar to those elicited by B40. Slow synaptic potentials that are associated with conductance decrease and become smaller when the postsynaptic cell is hyperpolarized have also been demonstrated in other parts ofAplysia feeding network (Chiel et al., 1986; Weiss et al., 1986).
Because hyperpolarization of B40s had a significant impact on B8 activity pattern in ingestive motor programs, i.e., B8 fired more in protraction and fired less in retraction (Figs. 2, 3), we examined the effect of B40 activity on B8 excitability (Fig.6A) to determine whether the fast IPSPs and slow EPSPs were responsible for the B40 functional role. In addition, hyperpolarization of B34s had a small effect on B8 activity pattern in ingestive programs, although B34 also elicited fast IPSPs and slow EPSPs in B8. Therefore, we also examined the effects of B34 on B8 excitability (Fig. 6B) and compared these effects with those of B40 (Fig.7). In these experiments (Figs. 6, 7), B8 excitability was tested by 3 sec constant current pulses every 30 sec and number of B8 spikes during the current pulses was counted. For the experiments to be comparable with each other, in the beginning of each experiment, the size of current pulses was adjusted so that B8 fired ∼12 spikes and remained the same throughout the rest of the experiment.
Two stimulation paradigms were used to examine the effect of B40/B34 activity on B8 excitability. In the first stimulation paradigm, B40 (Figs. 6A1, 7A1) or B34 (Figs.6B1, 7B1) was stimulated 300 msec before and throughout the current pulses injected into B8. This paradigm examines functional effects of the fast IPSPs and simulates the situation in a feeding motor program, where B40/B34 fire together with B8 during protraction. Under such conditions, B40 activity reduced B8 excitability (Fig. 6A1), whereas B34 activity slightly increased B8 excitability (Fig. 6B1). Group data are shown in Figure 7, A1 (B40) and B1(B34). For B40 effect (n = 11), one-way repeated measures ANOVA on the three groups (Before, B40, and After) revealed that B40 stimulation had a significant effect (F(2,20) = 55.1; p < 0.0001). Bonferroni multiple comparisons (post-test) of the three groups revealed that both “Before” and “After” groups are significantly different from “B40” group (p < 0.001), whereas Before and After groups are not significantly different from each other (p > 0.05). For B34 effect (n = 6), the three groups (Before, B34, and After) did not differ from each other. The data suggest that, consistent with the data shown in Figure3, during protraction B40 activity would reduce B8 activity while B34 activity would not. Indeed, when B40 and B34 were recorded from the same preparation, the IPSPs in B8 elicited by B34 were smaller than those elicited by B40 [compare the amplitude of hyperpolarization caused by the fast IPSPs in B8 elicited by B40 or B34 stimulations in the middle panels of Fig. 6A2 (B40) and B2(B34), which were recorded from the same preparation, and B8 was held at the same membrane potential].
In the second stimulation paradigm, B40 (Figs.6A2,A3, 7A2) or B34 (Figs.6B2, 7B2) was stimulated for 5 sec immediately before current pulses injected into B8. This paradigm examines functional effects of the slow EPSPs and simulates the situation in an ingestive motor program, where B40/B34 fire during protraction, whereas B8 fires during retraction. Under such conditions, both B40 (Fig. 6A2) and B34 (Fig.6B2) activity increased B8 excitability. Group data are shown in Figure 7A2 (B40), and 7B2 (B34). For B40 effect (n = 9), one-way repeated measures ANOVA on the three groups (Before, B40, and After) revealed that B40 stimulation had a significant effect (F(2,16) = 348.4; p < 0.0001). Bonferroni multiple comparisons (post test) of the three groups revealed that both Before and After groups are significantly different from B40 group (p < 0.001), whereas Before and After groups are not significantly different from each other (p > 0.05). For B34 effect (n = 6), one-way repeated measures ANOVA on the three groups (Before, B34, and After) also revealed that B34 stimulation had a significant effect (F(2,12) = 454.8; p < 0.0001). Bonferroni multiple comparisons (post test) of the three groups revealed that both Before and After groups are significantly different from B34 group (p < 0.001), whereas Before and After groups are not significantly different from each other (p > 0.05).
The increase in B8 excitability appeared larger after stimulation of B40 than stimulation of B34. To determine whether these differences are statistically significant, we performed two-way ANOVA. The two main factors in this analysis were the cell type (B40 vs B34) and stimulation conditions (Before, During, and After B40/B34 stimulation). We found that there was a significant effect of cell types (F(1,42) = 5.76; p < 0.05), stimulation conditions (F(1,42)= 139.6; p < 0.0001), and interaction of these two main factors (F(1,42) = 11.04;p < 0.001). Individual comparisons (post test) showed that this significant interaction was attributable to a larger increase in B8 excitability after B40 stimulation than after B34 stimulation (p < 0.001). There was no significant difference between the B8 excitability when neither B40 nor B34 was stimulated in two control conditions (Before, p > 0.05; After, p > 0.05).
To further examine potential contribution of possible postinhibitory rebound in B8 to the enhancement of B8 firing by B40 (compare Fig.4C4–C6) because B8 was inhibited throughout B40 firing (Figs. 4C6, 6A2), we applied a small constant hyperpolarizing current in B8 so that B40 no longer elicited fast IPSPs in B8. Under these conditions, B8 firing still increased (Fig. 6A3), suggesting that the slow EPSPs were indeed functional.
Taken together, these experiments indicate that both the fast IPSPs and the slow EPSPs from B40 to B8 are in fact functional, and B40 effects are larger than B34 effects.
Synaptic connectivity of B40 within the feeding central pattern generator
In feeding motor programs, B40 is active in protraction and inhibited in retraction. In this study, we examined the synaptic mechanism that may account for this pattern of B40 activity. One potential source of B40 excitation could be CBI-2. Indeed, CBI-2 produced facilitating EPSPs in B40 that followed presynaptic spikes one-for-one (n = 12) (Fig.8A). These EPSPs persisted in high-divalent saline (Fig. 8A2), suggesting that they are monosynaptic. No reciprocal connection from B40 to CBI-2 was observed. Second source of excitation may come from B34. B34 is electrically coupled to B40 (n = 5) (Fig.8B). In addition, B40 is also electrically coupled to its contralateral homolog (n = 10) (Fig.8B).
The inhibition may be mediated, at least partly, by the retraction-phase interneuron B64. B64 elicited IPSPs in B40 (n = 4). When B64 was stimulated with DC or current pulses, the first few B64 spikes typically did not induce visible IPSPs, but subsequent spikes elicited one-for-one IPSPs that facilitated significantly (Fig. 8C). The IPSPs also persisted in high-divalent saline (Fig. 8C2), suggesting they are monosynaptic. Thus, B64 may contribute to inhibition of B40 during the retraction phase of motor programs.
DISCUSSION
The two types of Aplysia feeding motor programs, ingestion and egestion, are generated by alterations of the relative timing of the PFMs (protraction–retraction) and the PSM (closure). Radula closure occurs in protraction in egestion, and in retraction in ingestion. The same set of interneurons (B63/B34 for protraction and B64 for retraction) fire similarly (Jing et al., 1999; Jing and Weiss, 2001) and mediate the PFMs in ingestion and egestion. This notion is reinforced by our finding that B34, which excites strongly protraction motorneurons (Hurwitz et al., 1997), has a small impact on the activity of B8, a closure motorneuron (Fig. 3). We have shown that B8 phasing in egestion is mediated by B20, which is preferentially active in egestion, is active in protraction, and uses fast excitation to drive B8 (Jing and Weiss, 2001). Here, we identified an interneuron B40 that is preferentially active in ingestion and is crucial for the expression of ingestive programs (Fig.9C).
Unexpectedly, B40 is active in protraction, but not active in retraction, the phase in which B8 is most active in ingestion. Consequently, B40 cannot use fast excitation to drive B8 in retraction as B20 does in protraction. Instead, B40 uses dual component PSPs, fast inhibition and slow excitation to promote B8 activity in retraction. In functional terms, the delayed excitation of B8 by B40 activity during protraction is equivalent to the fast excitation that B40 would have to exert if it were active in retraction. One implication of these findings is that activity phasing of interneurons may actually be different from the activity phasing of the motor neurons they control. Thus, caution should be exercised when interpreting data based solely on the phase of activity of interneurons.
While the functional role of the slow excitation from B40 to B8 is to drive closure during retraction in ingestive programs, the functional role of the fast inhibition from B40 to B8 is less obvious. This may be understood from two perspectives. First, the onset of B8 activation should occur at the beginning of retraction, not earlier, but the slow excitation that B40 uses to drive B8 cannot achieve precise timing on its own. The fast inhibition can thus be used to ensure that B8 is activated to fire at high rates only during retraction when B40 halts its firing and when the slow excitation is still effective but the fast inhibition ceases to exist. Second, one essential feature of the circuit is that changes in the activity phase of radula closure are implemented by recruitment of separate interneurons B20 and B40 (Fig.9). Both interneurons are active during protraction and receive excitation from partially overlapping sets of protraction-phase interneurons, e.g., CBI-2 and B34, that lead to a certain degree of coactivation of B20 and B40 in some motor programs (Fig. 1). Because activity of B20 in ingestive programs can generate a dysfunctional increase in B8 firing during protraction, B40-elicited fast inhibition can thus counter the effects of B20 activity. This notion is supported by observations that hyperpolarization of B40 resulted not only in the decrease of B8 firing in retraction but also in its increase during protraction. Conversely, in egestive programs, alternate mechanisms must be present to prevent B8 activity in retraction that may be promoted by coactivation of B40 with B20. This function is accomplished by a separate element B4/5, which B20 excites (Jing and Weiss, 2001). In ingestion, the dual-component PSPs from B40 accomplishes two functions. Such implementation is thus effective and efficient.
One important feature of the Aplysia circuit (Fig.9C) is that activity of interneurons for the PSM is coordinated with those for the PFMs through direct synaptic connections between these interneurons. The interneurons for the PFMs, B63, B34 (protraction) and B64 (retraction) reciprocally inhibit each other (Hurwitz et al., 1997), thus forming a half-center oscillator. Interneurons B20 and B40 for the PSM receive excitation from protraction-phase interneurons (e.g., CBI-2, B34) and are inhibited by retraction-phase interneuron (B64). Thus, they are active only in protraction and they, together with interneurons for the PFMs, form a single, multifunctional, central pattern generator (CPG) (Delcomyn, 1980; Getting, 1989; Marder and Calabrese, 1996). This coordination scheme contrasts with intersegmental coordination between multiple segmental oscillators in lamprey (Cohen et al., 1992; Williams, 1992;Grillner et al., 1995), leech (Friesen and Pearce, 1993), and crayfish (Tschuluun et al., 2001). In these latter cases, the timing of sets of movements in each segment is determined by activity of each oscillator, and the timing of overall behaviors has to be coordinated through more elaborate coupling mechanisms between these oscillators.
Comparison with other models of movement coordination
Two models of spinal circuits were proposed to explain intralimb coordination in forward–backward locomotion (Grillner, 1981, 1985) and in turtle rostral and pocket scratch (Berkowitz and Stein, 1994). For comparison, we translated these two spinal models into theAplysia feeding circuit (Fig. 9).
One major stipulation of the Grillner (1981, 1985) model is that a unit burst generator, or a controller (Orlovsky et al., 1999), exists for each agonist muscle movement. In terms of movement control, we define a controller as an interneuron or interneurons that provides direct excitation to a relevant motorneuron or motorneurons and whose activity is important, in a motor program-specific manner, to determine the activity of the motorneuron or motorneurons. Aplysia version of the Grillner (1981, 1985) model postulates separate controllers for protraction (PC), retraction (RC), and closure (CC). Aplysiaversion of the Berkowitz and Stein (1994) model postulates no separate controllers for closure. Instead, closure is controlled directly by protraction controllers (PCE) that are preferentially active in egestion or by retraction controllers (RCI) that are preferentially active in ingestion. Aplysia studies support the existence of separate controllers for different movements and also different CCs for egestion (CCE) and ingestion (CCI) (see the beginning of Discussion). However, the distinction between the controllers is not as strict as in the Grillner (1981, 1985) model, because B34, as a PC, does have some effect on B8 activity, albeit small. Thus, our model incorporates features of both spinal models, because the interneurons for PFMs remain the same as is the case in theGrillner (1981, 1985) model, but preferential activation of different interneurons implements the PSM in ingestion and egestion as is the case in the Berkowitz and Stein (1994) model.
Organizational differences of movement controllers in the three models have several implications for the mechanisms that generate different motor programs. In the Grillner (1981, 1985) model, CC does not change in different programs, and descending fibers change the sign of coupling between PC/RC and CC so that CC changes the phase together with closure motorneurons. In contrast, both in the Berkowitz and Stein (1994) and our models, CC does not change its activity phase, instead, separate CCs are recruited into different programs. Thus, these two models are similar and can be classified as recruitment model. Specifically, in both models, closure motorneurons are controlled by two separate sets of interneurons in the two programs. These two CCs are active in both programs, but display different activity levels. This scheme is consistent with population coding that has been described in behavioral choice and directional behaviors (Kristan and Shaw, 1997; Sparks et al., 1997), such as leech bending (Lockery and Kristan, 1990) and cockroach escape turns (Levi and Camhi, 2000). To generate different motor programs, sensory and/or descending fibers or other mechanisms must exist to preferentially activate PCE for the Berkowitz and Stein (1994) model, or CCE for our model in egestion. The same scenario applies for RCI or CCI for ingestion. However, the Berkowitz and Stein (1994) and our models differ in how closure is controlled in ingestion. Closure is implemented by dual excitation of two motor pools in the former model, and by a novel mechanism, fast-inhibition and slow-excitation, in the latter model.
In addition to Aplysia feeding (Morton and Chiel, 1993a) and turtle-scratching (Mortin et al., 1985), there are several examples in which related motor programs are generated by different combinations of multiple sets of relevant movements, e.g., different forms of locomotion in cat (Perret and Cabelguen, 1980; Smith et al., 1985,1998a; Buford and Smith, 1990; Pearson and Rossignol, 1991; Rossignol, 1996; Carlson-Kuhta et al., 1998), crayfish (Ayers and Davis, 1977;Chrachri and Clarac, 1990), and locust (Duch and Pfluger, 1995), feeding in mollusks (Croll and Davis, 1981) and lobster (Combes et al., 1999). Although it is interesting to compare the mechanisms inAplysia with other systems, there are few detailed circuitry or mechanistic-level studies available to make meaningful comparisons. One exception may be the gastric mill system of lobster stomatogastric ganglion, in which the motor sequences of two sets of teeth movements in type I and type II motor programs are similar to those of radula movements in egestive and ingestive motor programs inAplysia (Combes et al., 1999). However, neural organization of the stomatogastric ganglion is simpler than that ofAplysia in that the motor neurons are by themselves CPG elements (see Discussion in Jing and Weiss, 2001). Thus, the change of the phasing of the PSMs relative to the PFMs are implemented by the direct change of phasing of relevant motor neurons, rather than being implemented by a separate layer of CPG interneurons as is the case inAplysia (Fig. 9) and vertebrate spinal circuits.
In summary, by elucidating specific roles of Aplysia feeding interneurons involved in generation of two coordinated motor programs, we established a neural model which has some similarities to but also difference from the two spinal models for vertebrate intralimb coordination (Grillner, 1981, 1985; Berkowitz and Stein, 1994), and thus the significance of our findings may extend beyond theAplysia feeding network.
Footnotes
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants RO1 MH50235, KO5 MH 01427, and F32 MH12890. The National Resource for Aplysia of the University of Miami provided some of the animals used in this study under Grant RR-10294 from the National Center for Research Resources, National Institutes of Health. We thank Dr. E. C. Cropper for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jian Jing, Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Box 1218, Mt. Sinai School of Medicine, 1 Gustave Levy Place, New York, NY 10029. E-mail: jjing{at}inka.mssm.edu.