Skip to main content

Umbrella menu

  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • SfN.org
  • eNeuro
  • The Journal of Neuroscience
  • Neuronline
  • BrainFacts.org

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Preparing a Manuscript
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Fees
    • Journal Club
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
ARTICLE, Behavioral/Systems

Vestibular Information Is Required for Dead Reckoning in the Rat

Douglas G. Wallace, Dustin J. Hines, Sergio M. Pellis and Ian Q. Whishaw
Journal of Neuroscience 15 November 2002, 22 (22) 10009-10017; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-10009.2002
Douglas G. Wallace
1Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada T1K 3M4
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dustin J. Hines
1Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada T1K 3M4
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sergio M. Pellis
1Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada T1K 3M4
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ian Q. Whishaw
1Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada T1K 3M4
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Fig. 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 1.

    Test room and test procedures. Top, The test room. Many visual cues are available to the rat under light conditions; in the dark, visual cues are eliminated. A, Schematic of the table and a possible sequence of food pellet placements for 1 d. Black square indicates cued home base. Rats were required to find each food pellet before the table was rebaited. B, C, Schematics of the place probe–light and place probe–dark. Black circleindicates hidden home base. D, Schematic of the place probe–new location. Gray circle indicates former location of the home base; black circle indicates new location of the home base. For all task components, the heading direction circle that is drawn tangent to the inner portion of the holes was used to code when a return trip was terminated, and the point at which the rat crossed the circle was used to calculate the rat's heading direction.

  • Fig. 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 2.

    Homeward paths of control and vestibular rats. Each diagram plots the normalized homeward paths for a group of animals under one of the testing conditions: A, homeward paths with the home base cued (black square);B, homeward paths with the home base hidden (black circle); C, homeward paths in the dark. Note: Accuracy with respect to heading direction at the point where rats crossed the heading direction circle is shown in Figure3.

  • Fig. 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 3.

    Circular statistics for control and vestibular animals illustrating the poor performance of vestibular animals in the dark test. The three testing conditions are as follows:A, training with the home base cue; B, place probe with the home base hidden; C, place probe in the dark. White dots represent control animals;black dots represent vestibular animals. Inner circle represents raw data; outer circlerepresents averaged data. White arrows point to control average heading direction and parameter of concentration. Black arrows point to vestibular average heading direction and parameter of concentration. The bottom of the circular plot corresponds to normalized location of the home base, with heading direction angles increasing from 0 to 360o in a counterclockwise direction. The length of the arrow corresponds to the group's parameter of concentration, whereas the direction of thearrow represents the group's average heading direction. An arrow that extends to the perimeter of the inner circle represents a parameter of concentration equal to 1, withshorter arrows corresponding to lower values. Note: Only the vestibular animals are inaccurate under the dark condition.

  • Fig. 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 4.

    Representative control and vestibular animals' kinematic profiles. Outward paths are shown by solid lines; homeward paths are shown by the dotted line in both the representation of the rat's trip (circle) and its speed (graph). The three testing conditions are as follows: A, training with the home base cued; B, place probe with the home base hidden; C, place probe in the dark. Note: The vestibular animal's return trips are only longer and more nondirect in the dark condition.

  • Fig. 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 5.

    Exploratory trip distance and time (mean and SE). The left-hand panels plot control and vestibular average time (top) and distance (bottom) for the outward trip segment. The right-hand panels plot control and vestibular average time (top) and distance (bottom) for the homeward trip segment (*p <0.05; LSD test). Note: Elevated time and distance in the vestibular rats in the dark.

  • Fig. 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 6.

    Number of returns (mean and SE) to the old home base location for control and vestibular groups on 2 d of testing with the home base in a new location (*p <0.05; LSD test).

  • Fig. 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 7.

    Group hole visits when released from a new home base location. A, Location of the first hole selected.B, Location of the second hole selected. Each plot is a diagram of the table with the correct location of the home base, previous location of the home base, and other home base locations indicated by black, gray, andwhite circles, respectively. Hole visits are represented by the black dots located around the outside of the circular diagram. The black arrows indicate average heading direction and parameter of concentration. The length and direction of the black arrow at thecenter of each graph corresponds to a group's parameter of concentration and average heading direction, respectively. Ther values and associated probabilities reflect the results of each plot from the Rayleigh test of randomness.

  • Fig. 8.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Fig. 8.

    Regression analyses using vestibular reflex impairments as a predictor of spatial performance. White dots represent control animals; black dotsrepresent vestibular animals. The four testing conditions are as follows: A, training with the home base cued;B, place probe with the home base hidden;C, place probe in the dark; D, place probe with the hidden home base in a new location. Spatial performance in A–C was indexed by the parameter of concentration (i.e., variability of heading direction). Spatial performance in D was indexed by mean number of perseverations to the old home base location.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Vestibular assessment scale

    Reflex typeBehavioral testYesNo
    Static reflexesHead righting
     Asymmetrical
      Right01
      Left01
     Symmetrical
      Dorsal flexion01
    Statokinetic reflexesLinear acceleration
     Head01
     Limbs01
    Angular acceleration
     Right01
     Left01
    Air righting
     Complete0
     Partial1
     None2
    TotalMinimum0
    Maximum9
    • 0, No vestibular damage; 9, complete vestibular damage.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 22 (22)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 22, Issue 22
15 Nov 2002
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Vestibular Information Is Required for Dead Reckoning in the Rat
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Vestibular Information Is Required for Dead Reckoning in the Rat
Douglas G. Wallace, Dustin J. Hines, Sergio M. Pellis, Ian Q. Whishaw
Journal of Neuroscience 15 November 2002, 22 (22) 10009-10017; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-10009.2002

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Vestibular Information Is Required for Dead Reckoning in the Rat
Douglas G. Wallace, Dustin J. Hines, Sergio M. Pellis, Ian Q. Whishaw
Journal of Neuroscience 15 November 2002, 22 (22) 10009-10017; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-10009.2002
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • dead reckoning
  • spatial navigation and dead reckoning
  • spatial navigation and vestibular system
  • vestibular system and dead reckoning
  • vestibular reflexes
  • food hoarding

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

ARTICLE

  • Neural Correlates of Competing Fear Behaviors Evoked by an Innately Aversive Stimulus
  • Calcium Influx via L- and N-Type Calcium Channels Activates a Transient Large-Conductance Ca2+-Activated K+Current in Mouse Neocortical Pyramidal Neurons
  • Distinct Developmental Modes and Lesion-Induced Reactions of Dendrites of Two Classes of Drosophila Sensory Neurons
Show more ARTICLE

Behavioral/Systems

  • Spinal Inhibitory Neurons that Modulate Cutaneous Sensory Pathways during Locomotion in a Simple Vertebrate
  • Efferent Protection from Acoustic Injury Is Mediated via α9 Nicotinic Acetylcholine Receptors on Outer Hair Cells
  • Attenuation of Nicotine-Induced Antinociception, Rewarding Effects, and Dependence in μ-Opioid Receptor Knock-Out Mice
Show more Behavioral/Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
  • Feedback
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2021 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.