Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
ARTICLE, Behavioral/Systems

Dynamic Cortical and Subcortical Networks in Learning and Delayed Recall of Timed Motor Sequences

Virginia B. Penhune and Julien Doyon
Journal of Neuroscience 15 February 2002, 22 (4) 1397-1406; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-01397.2002
Virginia B. Penhune
1Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, H4B 1R6 Canada,
3McConnell Brain Imaging Center, Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Julien Doyon
2Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, H3C 3J7 Canada, and
3McConnell Brain Imaging Center, Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

We used positron emission tomography to examine learning and retention of timed motor sequences. Subjects were scanned during learning (LRN) and baseline (ISO) on 3 d: day 1, after 5 d of practice (day 5) and after a 4 week delay (recall). Blood flow was compared across days of learning and between the LRN and ISO conditions. Overall, significant changes in activity were seen across days for the LRN condition, but not the ISO baseline. Day 1 results revealed extensive activation in the cerebellar cortex, particularly lobules III/IV and VI. Day 5 results showed increased activity in the basal ganglia (BG) and frontal lobe, with no significant cerebellar activity. At recall, significantly greater activity was seen in M1, premotor, and parietal cortex. Blood flow in the cerebellum decreased significantly between day 1 and recall. These results reveal a dynamic network of motor structures that are differentially active during different phases of learning and delayed recall. For the first time our findings show that recall of motor sequences in humans is mediated by a predominantly cortical network. Based on these results, we suggest that during early learning cerebellar mechanisms are involved in adjusting movement kinematics according to sensory input to produce accurate motor output. Thereafter, the cerebellar mechanisms required for early learning are no longer called into play. During late learning, the BG may be involved in automatization. At delayed recall, movement parameters appear to be encoded in a distributed representation mediated by M1, premotor, and parietal cortex.

  • motor-skill learning
  • motor cortex
  • basal ganglia
  • PMC
  • cerebellum
  • frontal lobe
  • memory
  • human
  • procedural learning

Humans learn a wide variety of complex motor skills and retain them over long periods of time. Although robust long-term retention is a hallmark of motor learning, very few studies have looked at the neural structures involved in maintaining long-term representations of motor skills. Therefore, the present experiment used positron emission tomography (PET) to compare brain regions active during recall of a timed motor sequence with those active on two earlier days of learning.

A large body of literature exists related to motor-skill learning. Studies in animals and humans have shown that motor cortical regions, the cerebellum, and the basal ganglia (BG) are critically involved in learning skilled movements (Graybiel, 1995; Thach, 1996;Doyon, 1997; Karni et al., 1998; Van Mier, 2000). Current models suggest that different networks of cortical and subcortical regions are preferentially involved at the early and late phases of skill acquisition (Karni et al., 1998; Hikosaka et al., 1999; Van Mier, 2000;Doyon and Ungerleider, 2002). Neuroimaging studies of motor sequence learning have shown decreasing cerebellar activation as a task is learned, accompanied by increasing activation in the BG, primary motor cortex (M1), and the supplementary motor area (SMA) (Grafton et al., 1994; Jenkins et al., 1994; Karni et al., 1995; Doyon et al., 1996,1999; Van Mier et al., 1997; Toni et al., 1998). Based on current evidence, Doyon and Ungerleider (2002) have hypothesized that early learning of motor sequences recruits a predominantly cerebello-cortical network, but that late learning and delayed recall may rely on a predominantly striato-cortical network. In contrast, available data on long-term retention of motor skills is sparse. Although not examining recall directly, studies of long-term practice have shown plasticity in M1 of both humans (Pascual-Leone et al., 1995; Karni et al., 1998) and monkeys (Nudo et al., 1996). Neuroimaging studies of overlearned skills, such as typing and writing, have also shown involvement of M1, along with the SMA and premotor cortex (PMC) (Seitz et al., 1994;Gordon et al., 1998). The majority of evidence shows reduced cerebellar activity in well learned tasks. However, a single study in monkeys suggests that the cerebellar nuclei may be important in delayed recall (Hikosaka et al., 1999).

In summary, both cortical and subcortical regions play important roles in motor-skill learning. However, the pattern of activity across these regions for both learning and delayed recall has not previously been examined. Therefore, in the present experiment, subjects were tested during early learning (day 1); late learning (day 5: after 5 d of practice); and delayed recall (after a 4 week delay with no additional practice). We predicted decreased cerebellar activity between days 1 and 5, with increased activation in the BG and motor cortical regions. At delayed recall, we predicted no residual cerebellar activation, and a complete shift of activity to the BG and motor cortical regions such as M1, PMC, and the SMA.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects. Subjects were nine healthy, right-handed volunteers selected to have not >3 years of musical training or experience (five female, four male; average age, 23.5). Subjects were paid for their participation and gave informed consent. The experimental protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Montreal Neurological Institute.

Stimuli and task conditions. The task used in this experiment required subjects to reproduce a complex timed motor sequence by tapping in synchrony with a visual stimulus using a single key of the computer mouse (Fig. 1). Stimuli were 10-element visual sequences made up of a series of white squares (3 cm2) presented sequentially in the center of the computer screen. In the learned condition (LRN), two sequences and two tempos were used. Each sequence was made up of five long (750/600 msec) and five short (250/300 msec) elements with a constant interstimulus interval (500/300 msec). The two tempos and the two sequences were crossed and counterbalanced across subjects. In the LRN condition, each subject performed a single sequence at one of the two tempos. Sequences were constructed to have five short and five long elements, to have no more than two repeated elements, and to have seven transitions from short to long. This resulted in sequences that were temporally regular, but did not conform to a standard musical rhythm. In the isochronous baseline condition (ISO), sequences were made up of either all short or all long elements. The all-long and all-short sequences alternated across the block of trials. Each block of trials contained 12 presentations of the learned or isochronous sequences. Therefore, the same number of short and long stimuli were present in each block of the LRN and ISO conditions, so that subjects received the same amount of visual stimulation and made the same number of motor responses. The ISO condition was selected as the baseline because it requires similar timing and sensorimotor integration components as the LRN condition, but does not require learning of a complex temporal sequence. Before performing the LRN or ISO sequences on each day, subjects were given a set of practice sequences (Fig. 1) that were used to score performance on the LRN and ISO conditions. Subjects' key-press and release durations were recorded by a computer and used to calculate the three indices of learning: accuracy, response variance, and response asynchrony (described in detail below). We performed 5.5 trials of the LRN or ISO condition (trial length, 11 sec) during the period of each 60 sec scan.

Fig. 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1.

Illustrates the stimuli sequences used for practice and for the isochronous and learning conditions (see stimuli and task conditions). Stimulus sequences were made up of white squares that appeared sequentially at the center of the computer screen. Squares appeared for either short or long durations, represented by the short or long line lengths in the figure. For each condition, one example of each sequence type is illustrated. For the learning condition, subjects were tested on only one of the two possible sequences. Blocks of practice sequences contained three repetitions of each sequence type. Blocks of isochronous sequences contained six repetitions of each sequence type. Blocks of learning sequences contained 12 repetitions of each sequence.

Procedure. Each subject was scanned on three separate days (Fig. 2): day 1 of learning, after 5 d of practice (day 5) and after a 4 week delay with no further practice (recall). Two scans related to this experiment were performed on each day. On day 1, subjects were placed in the scanner and trained on the task using a set of practice sequences. They were then explicitly taught the learned sequence to a criterion of three consecutive correct repetitions. After this initial training, subjects were not given feedback on their performance. Subjects were then scanned while performing one block of the LRN condition (LRN1). Three additional blocks of practice were performed without scanning, for a total of four blocks of practice. Subjects were then scanned while performing one block of the ISO condition (ISO1). On days 2–4, subjects returned to the laboratory to perform four blocks of practice on the LRN condition without scanning. On these days, subjects performed the practice sequences, but not the ISO sequences. On day 5, subjects were placed in the scanner and performed the practice sequences and three blocks of the learned sequence without scanning. They were then scanned on the final block of learning (LRN2) and the isochronous baseline (ISO2). Across the 5 d of practice subjects performed 20 blocks (240 trials) of the learned sequences and three blocks (36 trials) of the ISO sequences. After a 4 week delay with no additional practice, subjects again returned to the lab and were scanned while performing a single block of the learned sequence (REC) and the isochronous baseline (ISO3). Subjects were specifically instructed not to practice the learned sequence during the 4 week delay and were debriefed on the final day of scanning to be sure that they had complied with that instruction. No subject reported practicing during the delay.

Fig. 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2.

Illustrates the experimental design (see Materials and Methods). The top three blocks describe the 3 d of scanning. The bottom two blocks describe the days between scans.

Behavioral measures. In typical motor sequence tasks, learning is assessed by changes in error, speed, or reaction time. In an explicitly learned sequence, errors usually decrease quickly, so reaction time is the parameter most frequently used to measure learning. However, because timing was the parameter of interest in this experiment, learning could not be assessed by decreases in reaction time. Therefore, learning of the present task was assessed by examining changes in three different variables: accuracy, variance of response durations, and synchrony of responses with target stimuli. Accuracy was expected to improve quickly, whereas the other variables were expected to change more slowly over the course of learning. Accuracy for the LRN and ISO conditions was scored individually by using each subject's average short and long responses from the practice sequences for each day ± 2 SD as the upper and lower limits for correct response for short and long elements, respectively. Percentage of correct values were calculated for each trial (for additional details on the scoring method see, Penhune et al., 1998). Response variance measured the stability of the subject's response, by calculating the coefficient of variation (SD/mean) of the subject's response durations. Response asynchrony was assessed by examining differences between stimulus onset and offset and the onset and offset of the subject's key-press responses. The variability and asynchrony measures were performed on correct responses only. All behavioral measures were averaged across blocks and days of practice. Differences across days 1–5 of practice, between day 5 and recall and across blocks of practice on day 1 were assessed using repeated measures ANOVA. In addition, comparison of these same measures was made between the LRN and the ISO conditions. Differences between LRN and ISO for each of the performance variables were assessed using ANOVA for repeated measures with significant interactions analyzed using tests of simple main effects with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Scan acquisition and data analysis. PET scans were acquired using the O15 water-bolus method (60 sec scans, Siemens HR+, 3-D acquisition) resulting in a volume of 63 slices with an intrinsic resolution of 4.2 × 4.2 × 4.0 mm. T1-weighted MRI scans were acquired for all subjects (1 × 1 × 1 mm; 140–160 sagittal slices). Field of view of the PET camera allowed visualization of the entire cortex and cerebellum. MRI and PET data were coregistered (Woods et al., 1993) and automatically resampled (Collins et al., 1994) to fit the standardized stereotaxic space ofTalairach and Tournoux (1988) as defined by the MNI 305 template. PET volumes were normalized, reconstructed with a 12 mm Hanning filter, and averaged across subjects for each condition. Differences across days of learning were assessed using paired-image subtraction (Worsley et al., 1992), and by analyzing changes in normalized cerebral blood flow (nCBF) values from specific volumes of interest (VOI). For the subtraction analyses, statistically significant peaks were identified by an automatic algorithm with a threshold set att ≥ ±3.5. Activations identified as being in the same brain region that were located within 0.5 cm of each other were considered to be indistinguishable, and the location of the peak with the higher t value is reported in the table. The location of active regions in the cerebellum were identified using a 3-D atlas of the human cerebellum in stereotaxic space (Schmahmann et al., 2000). For the nCBF analyses, spherical VOIs (radius, 5 mm) were defined using the Talairach locations of specific significantly active regions identified in the subtraction analyses. Average nCBF values for individual subjects were extracted for each VOI for both the LRN and ISO conditions on day 1, day 5, and recall. These values were submitted to repeated-measures ANOVA, and significant interactions were analyzed using tests of simple main effects with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

RESULTS

Behavioral data

No significant differences in overall performance were obtained for either the different tempos or the different sequences. Therefore behavioral data were collapsed across these dimensions. No significant change in simple percentage correct was observed across days 1–5 of learning (Fig. 3) (average day 1, 0.93; average day 5, 0.97; F(4,32) = 1.4;p = 0.25) probably because each sequence of short and long elements was learned explicitly to criterion before scanning. However, significant changes were observed for both response variance (average day 1, 0.17; average day 5, 0.11;F(4,32) = 20.8; p < 0.001) and response asynchrony (average day 1, 176 msec; average day 5, 108 msec; F(4,32) = 19.1;p < 0.001). These results indicate that although the order of elements in the sequence was learned very rapidly, stabilization of response variance and synchronization continued to show significant effects of learning across days of practice. Importantly, no significant differences were obtained for any of the measures when comparing day 5 of learning to recall, indicating that once learned, both the sequence of elements and the temporal parameters were well retained (percentage correct:F(1,8) = 0.53, p = 0.49; CV: F(1,8) = 2.6,p = 0.15; asynchrony:F(1,8) = 0.38, p = 0.55).

Fig. 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3.

Illustrates changes in performance for the learned and isochronous sequences across days of practice (see Results). Theleft graph shows the change in percentage correct; themiddle graph shows changes in the coefficient of variation, and the right graph shows changes in response asynchrony.

Behavioral measures for the learned sequences were also compared with those for the isochronous sequences on day 1, day 5, and recall. Results for percentage correct showed no significant change across days and no significant differences between the two conditions (day:F(2,16) = 0.25, p = 0.63; condition: F(1,8) = 0.76,p = 0.48; day × condition:F(2, 16) = 0.43, p = 0.66). For response variation, there was a significant day × condition interaction (F(2,16) = 5.3;p = 0.02), such that the learned sequences showed significant change between day 1 and day 5 (p = 0.004), but the isochronous sequences did not (p= 1.0). Furthermore, the two conditions were significantly different on day 1 (p < 0.001), where the CV was lower for the isochronous sequences than for the learned sequences. This difference is probably the result of the simplicity of the isochronous sequences and the fact that they were always performed after the four blocks of practice on the learned condition. For the asynchrony measure, analysis revealed a significant effect of day (F(2,16) = 8.0; p < 0.01), such that day 1 was significantly different than both day 5 (p = 0.06) and recall (p= 0.01). However, there was no significant effect of condition or any interaction, indicating that performance was similar across the two conditions. This is probably the result of general learning of the tapping response, irrespective of sequence complexity. Finally, behavioral data illustrating learning across blocks of practice on day 1 is shown in Figure 4 and will be considered further in the Discussion. Similar to the pattern of results across days of learning, there was no significant change in percentage correct across blocks 1–4 on day 1 (F(3,24) = 1.52; p < 0.23), but significant changes were observed for response variance (F(3,24) = 5.93; p < 0.01) and response asynchrony (F(3,24)=11.47; p < 0.01).

Fig. 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 4.

Illustrates changes in performance for the learned sequences across blocks of trials on day 1 of learning (see Discussion). The left graph shows the change in percentage correct, the middle graph shows changes in the coefficient of variation, and the right graph shows changes in response asynchrony.

Paired-image subtraction

LRN1 versus ISO1

Regions that were significantly more active during learning on day 1 were found in bilateral cerebellar cortex, extrastriate visual areas, and the hippocampal region. Active cerebellar regions included medial areas III/IV, V/VI, and IX. Activation in lateral regions were also seen bilaterally in lobules VI, VIIIA, and VIIIB (Table 1, Fig.5). Because a similar number of movements were made in both conditions, activity in motor cortical regions that may have been involved in performing the sequences was not observed. Despite similar visual input, relatively greater activation was observed medially in areas 18/19 of extrastriate visual cortex and bilaterally in the precuneus and fusiform gyri. Increased blood flow in visual regions may be related to the sensorimotor integration demands of the task, requiring precise synchronization of the motor response with the visual stimulus (Bower, 1995).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Locations of significant blood flow differences in the LRN1–ISO1 comparison

Fig. 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 5.

The top panel presentsz-statistic maps showing significant regions of activation in the cerebellum on day 1 (LRN1–ISO1). PET data are coregistered with the average MRI of the nine subjects, and slice levels are given in the standardized space of Talairach and Tournoux (t value range, 2.5–7.0). The bottom panel shows graphs of the changes in nCBF values extracted from cerebellar VOIs for the LRN and ISO conditions. The top graph shows the significant decrease between day 1 and recall collapsed across all cerebellar regions (significant differences are indicated with an asterisk). The bottom graphshows changes in nCBF for the individual cerebellar regions for the LRN condition alone.

LRN2 versus LRN1

Comparison of LRN2 to LRN1 revealed a single area of residual activity in lobule IX of the cerebellum accompanied by relatively greater activity in the right putamen/globus pallidus (GP) and in the medial and orbital frontal cortex (Table2, Fig. 6). Medial frontal activity included gyrus rectus (area 14 of Petrides), areas 9, 10, and 8 (Chiavaras and Petrides, 2000). The lateral orbital frontal gyri (areas 47/12 of Petrides) were also active bilaterally (Chiavaras and Petrides, 2000). Because cerebellar activity present in LRN2 could be masked in the comparison with LRN1, LRN2 was also compared with ISO2 (Table3). The result of this comparison again revealed a single residual area of cerebellar activation, this time in lateral lobule VI/VIIa. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that the cerebellum is less actively involved in production of a motor sequence once it is well learned and that the BG and other cortical areas are more important during the more automatic phase of performance (Doyon and Ungerleider, 2002).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Locations of significant blood flow differences in the LRN2–LRN1 comparison

Fig. 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 6.

The left panel presentsz-statistic maps showing the significant activation in the BG observed on day 5 (LRN2–LRN1). PET data are coregistered with the average MRI of the nine subjects, and slice levels are given in the standardized space of Talairach and Tournoux (t value range, 2.5–8.5). The graph illustrates changes in nCBF values extracted from the BG VOI for the LRN and ISO conditions. These results show a significant increase in activity between day 1 and day 5 (significant differences are indicated with anasterisk).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Locations of significant blood flow differences in the LRN2–ISO2 comparison

REC versus LRN2

When REC was compared with LRN2, relatively greater CBF was observed in left M1, PMC, inferior parietal cortex (area 40) and medial area 8 (Table 4, Fig.7). No residual activity in the cerebellum or the BG was observed in either this comparison or in the REC versus ISO3 comparison (Table 5). These results are largely consistent with our working hypothesis and constitute the first demonstration that retention and production of a well learned motor sequence recruits a predominantly cortical network. Lack of cerebellar activation in these comparisons suggests that this structure is not required for the production of a well learned timed motor sequence, even after considerable delay.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Locations of significant blood flow differences in the REC–LRN2 comparison

Fig. 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 7.

The left panel presentsz-statistic maps showing significant regions of activation in M1, PMC, parietal cortex, and medial area 8 observed at recall (REC–LRN2). PET data are coregistered with the average MRI of the nine subjects, and slice levels are given in the standardized space of Talairach and Tournoux (t value range, 2.5–4.8). Theright panel graphs changes in nCBF values extracted from each VOI for the LRN and ISO conditions (significant differences are indicated with an asterisk).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 5.

Locations of significant blood flow differences in the REC–ISO3 comparison

CBF changes across days of learning

In order to directly examine blood flow changes across days of learning, nCBF values were analyzed for specific VOIs based on active regions identified in the subtraction analyses. VOIs were centered on the Talairach location of the highest t value for each region (for locations, see Tables 1, 2, and 4). Average nCBF values for each VOI were extracted from the LRN1, LRN2, and REC scans and for the ISO1, ISO2, and ISO3 scans. These values were submitted to a repeated measures ANOVA to examine changes in nCBF values across days of learning between the two conditions. Overall, analyses of the nCBF data confirmed the results of the subtraction analyses. Most importantly, they showed that nCBF changed significantly across days for the LRN, but not the ISO condition.

In the cerebellum, VOIs were created for the seven regions that were active in the LRN1–ISO1 subtraction but not the LRN2–LRN1 subtraction (Fig. 5, Table 1). Results showed a significant day × condition interaction (F(2,16) = 18.1;p < 0.001), and tests of simple main effects showed that nCBF was greater in the LRN than the ISO condition on day 1. There was also a significant main effect of condition, such that nCBF was greater overall for the LRN than the ISO condition. Separate ANOVA for the LRN condition alone showed that nCBF decreased across all cerebellar regions across days (F(2,16) = 4.08; p < 0.04). Tests of simple main effect revealed a marginally significant decrease for medial lobule III/IV between day 1 and recall (p < 0.06), and significant decreases between day 1 and day 5 (p < 0.04) and day 5 and recall (p < 0.05) for medial VI and right lateral lobule VI (day 1–day 5: p < 0.01; day 5–recall: p < 0.001). The difference between day 1 and day 5 was nearly significant for left lateral lobule VI (p = 0.10).

For the putamen/GP the VOI was centered on the peak of activation observed in the LRN2–LRN1 subtraction (Fig. 6, Table 2). Results of the ANOVA showed a significant day × condition interaction (F(2,16) = 5.9; p < 0.01), with tests of simple main effect showing a marginally significant increase in nCBF for the LRN condition between day 1 and day 5 (p = 0.06), but no significant differences across days for the ISO condition (p = 0.36).

Changes in M1, PMC, the parietal lobe (area 40), and medial area 8 were examined for VOIs based on the peaks of activation observed in the REC–LRN2 subtraction (Fig. 7, Table 4). Results of an ANOVA including all four regions showed a significant day × condition interaction (F(2,16) = 12.4; p < 0.001) such that the LRN condition showed greater nCBF at recall than on day 5 (p < 0.0001) or day 1 (p < 0.01), but no differences were observed for the ISO condition. There was also a significant region × condition interaction, such that all regions differed for both conditions. Because of the large differences in overall nCBF between the regions, each region was submitted to a separate ANOVA. The results of these analyses showed the same overall pattern for M1 and PMC, with a significant day × condition interaction (M1:F(2,16) = 5.6; p < 0.01; PMC: F(2,16) = 3.9;p < 0.04) such that nCBF was greater at recall in comparison with day 5 (M1: p < 0.001; PMC:p < 0.01) and day 1 (M1: p < 0.07; PMC: p < 0.01). The day × condition interaction was also seen for the parietal lobe (F(2,16) = 3.5; p < 0.05), with significant differences were found between day 1 and recall (p < 0.04). A marginally significant interaction was seen for medial area 8 (F(2,16) = 3.0; p < 0.08), with a significant difference observed between day 1 and day 5 (p < 0.02). Taken together these results show that M1, PMC, and parietal cortex are more active during performance of the LRN sequences at recall than during performance of the same task as on day 5 of learning. This indicates that activity in these regions is specifically related to the delayed recall component of the task rather than any differences in task parameters.

DISCUSSION

These results demonstrate a network of cortical and subcortical structures that contribute differentially to the early and late phases of motor learning and to delayed recall. Early learning showed extensive activation of the cerebellar cortex. After 5 d of practice, cerebellar activity decreased and greater activity was observed in the BG and frontal lobe. At delayed recall, significantly greater activation was seen in M1, PMC, and the parietal lobe, with no significant activity in the cerebellum or BG. The results of the subtraction analyses were confirmed by changes in nCBF during learning compared with the isochronous baseline. Across days of learning, nCBF in the cerebellum decreased, but increased in the BG between day 1 and day 5. No significant changes were observed across days for the isochronous condition. At recall, nCBF for the learned sequences increased in M1, PMC, and parietal cortex, but not for the isochronous baseline. These findings support the working hypothesis that the cerebellum is primarily involved in the early phase of motor sequence learning, with the BG possibly contributing to a later, automatization phase. Importantly, this experiment demonstrates that relative to learning, delayed recall of a motor sequence appears to be mediated by a predominantly cortical network including M1, the PMC and parietal cortex.

Early learning

On day 1, greater activity was observed in cerebellar lobules III/IV and VI during performance of the LRN sequences than in the ISO baseline. Greater cerebellar activation during initial performance of a motor task is consistent with a large number of recent studies (for review, see Doyon, 1997; Van Mier, 2000) (Doyon and Ungerleider, 2002). In addition, the specific cerebellar regions active on day 1 are similar to those observed in a previous study of performance of timed motor sequences (Penhune et al., 1998). Finally, these regions are consistent with those identified in a meta-analysis of cerebellar activity during motor sequence learning (Desmond and Fiez, 1998).

Several current theories describe specific cerebellar mechanisms that might mediate early learning: (1) combining of individual movements and motor context into movement “synergies” (Thach, 1996); (2) motor and perceptual timing (Ivry, 1996); and (3) sensorimotor integration (Bower, 1995) and error detection (Flament et al., 1996). Evidence that these mechanisms were active comes from changes in performance across the four blocks of learning on day 1 (Fig. 4). The percentage of correctly reproduced elements increased across blocks of practice, demonstrating improved performance of the motor sequence as a whole. Response variance and synchronization also improved, indicating refinement of movement timing and integration of the motor response with the visual stimulus. Participation of sensorimotor integration mechanisms in early learning is also supported by the observed activations in visual association areas in the LRN1–ISO1 subtraction. Extrastriate visual regions, predominantly in the dorsal stream, have strong connections to the cerebellum (Schmahmann, 1997). These visual association areas were not active in a previous study in which subjects imitated timed sequences after presentation of the stimuli (Penhune et al., 1998), further suggesting that activation in these regions is related to synchronization with the visual stimulus. Finally, error correction is an important component of early learning that encompasses the ability to modify responses in all of the above domains.

Late learning

A very different pattern of brain activity was observed after 5 d of practice, when task performance had stabilized (Fig. 3). Activity decreased in the cerebellum and increased in the BG and frontal cortex. Decreasing cerebellar activity as learning progresses is consistent with a number of previous studies (Grafton et al., 1994;Seitz et al., 1994; Toni et al., 1998; Doyon et al., 1999) and suggests that once a sequence is well learned, the timing and sensorimotor integration mechanisms active during early learning may not be called into play. Greater BG activity on day 5 is consistent with neuroimaging studies showing BG involvement in performance of well learned sequences (Doyon et al., 1996; Grafton et al., 1996; Rao et al., 1997; Rauch et al., 1997). BG involvement in the later phase of learning is also supported by neurophysiological studies in animals (Graybiel, 1995) and by studies in Parkinson's disease showing impairments in late, but not early motor sequence acquisition (Doyon et al., 1997, 1998).

It has also been proposed that the BG are involved in motor and perceptual timing (Rao et al., 1997; Harrington et al., 1998). In the present experiment, timing mechanisms might be hypothesized to be maximally engaged for the LRN condition on day 1. However, no difference was seen between the LRN and ISO conditions, either in the subtraction or nCBF analyses. BG activity increased on day 5, when motor timing had become more accurate, as shown in both the LRN2–LRN1 subtraction and in the nCBF analysis. Therefore, BG appear to be most active when timing is well learned, suggesting a role in automatization for later recall. This interpretation is consistent with previous work showing greater BG activity during reproduction of simple timed sequences (Rao et al., 1997).

Finally, the BG are known to play a role in learning and memory for the motivational salience of responses (Schultz et al., 2000). Therefore, it might be possible that BG activity is the result of the rewarding properties of expert performance on day 5. However, no concomitant blood flow increase was observed in the BG during performance of the isochronous sequences, which were equally well performed.

On day 5, greater activity was also observed in ventrolateral and medial orbital frontal cortex. Ventrolateral frontal cortex has been shown to be involved in retrieval from short-term memory through connections with sensory association areas. (for review, see Petrides, 1994, 1995; Owen et al., 1996; Stern et al., 2000). Similar frontal regions were more active in ISO1 than in LRN1 (see negative peaks in Table 1), perhaps because these simple sequences could be learned quickly. Large increases in orbital frontal lobe activity were also observed on day 5. Two current studies in our laboratory show activity in this region during performance of a well learned sequence of foot movements (Lafleur et al., 1999; Jackson et al., 2001). Additionally, medial orbital frontal cortex is implicated in reward (Elliott et al., 2000) and is strongly interconnected with the BG (Cavada et al., 2000). Activity in this region may reflect intrinsic reward associated with a high level of performance.

Delayed recall

Comparison of day 5 with recall revealed a different pattern of active regions, with significantly greater blood flow seen in left M1, PMC, parietal lobe (area 40), and medial area 8 (Fig. 7). nCBF analyses for M1, PMC, and the parietal lobe showed significant increases between day 5 and recall for the LRN, but not the ISO condition. Increased activity in M1 and PMC is consistent with neuroimaging studies of overlearned skills, such as typing and writing (Seitz et al., 1994;Gordon et al., 1998). Studies in humans and monkeys have shown changes in the degree or extent of activation in M1 related to long-term practice (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994; Karni et al., 1995; Nudo et al., 1996), and Karni has hypothesized that long-term representations of motor sequences may be stored in M1 (Karni et al., 1998). However, the few studies that have examined skill learning in humans with M1 damage have shown impairments in performance, but not in learning (Cushman and Caplan, 1987; Bondi et al., 1993; Platz et al., 1994; Winstein et al., 1999). Therefore, it seems unlikely that motor sequences are represented uniquely in M1, but are distributed within several motor cortical areas. The PMC, parietal cortex, and medial area 8 may form part of this network. The parietal cortex has been seen to be active during performance of overlearned sequences and may be involved in representation of somatosensory and body-centered spatial information (Sadato et al., 1996; Seitz et al., 1997; Sakai et al., 1998).

At recall, cerebellar activity decreased significantly compared with day 1, but was unchanged from day 5. This is consistent with data showing decreased cerebellar activity with learning (Grafton et al., 1994; Seitz et al., 1994; Toni et al., 1998; Doyon et al., 1999) and suggests that cerebellar mechanisms required during early learning are less engaged at recall. Unexpectedly, no significant BG activity was seen at recall, perhaps because of the relatively small number of subjects tested. Alternatively, however, this may be related to the explicit nature (Rauch et al., 1997) and simple motoric demands of the task.

This experiment reveals a dynamic network of cortical and subcortical structures active during early and late motor learning and at delayed recall. Based on these results, we propose that during early learning, the cerebellum is critically involved in adjusting movement kinematics according to sensory input to produce accurate motor output. During late learning, the BG may be involved in automatization of these parameters for delayed recall. At recall, cerebellar cortical mechanisms required for early learning do not appear to be called into play. At this phase, movement parameters may be encoded in a motor representation stored in a distributed network including M1, PMC, and parietal cortex.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Natural Science Research Council of Canada (J.D., V.B.P.), the Medical Research Council of Canada (J.D.), and the Institut de réadaptation en déficience physique de Québec (V.B.P.). We acknowledge the staff of the McConnell Brain Imaging Center and the Medical Cyclotron Unit at the Montreal Neurological Institute for assistance in data collection. We thank Pierre Ahad for work in developing the experimental task, Sylvain Milot for assistance in data analysis, and Kathy DeSousa in testing of the subjects. We also thank the reviewers for their useful input in completion of the manuscript.

    Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Virginia Penhune, Department of Psychology, PY-139-3 Concordia University, 7141 Sherbrooke Street, West Montreal, Quebec, H4B 1R6 Canada. E-mail:vpenhune{at}vax2.concordia.ca.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Bondi M,
    2. Kaszniak A,
    3. Rapcsak S,
    4. Butters M
    (1993) Implicit and explicit memory following anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture. Brain Cogn 22:213–229.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bower J
    (1995) The cerebellum as a sensory acquisition controller. Hum Brain Mapp 2:255–256.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    1. Cavada C,
    2. Compañy T,
    3. Tejedor J,
    4. Cruz-Rizzolo R,
    5. Reinoso-Suárez F
    (2000) The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex: a review. Cereb Cortex 10:220–242.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Chiavaras M,
    2. Petrides M
    (2000) Orbitofrontal sulci of the human and macaque monkey brain. J Comp Neurol 422:35–54.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Collins DL,
    2. Neelin P,
    3. Peters TM,
    4. Evans AC
    (1994) Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr 18:192–205.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Cushman L,
    2. Caplan B
    (1987) Multiple memory systems: Evidence from stroke. Percept Mot Skills 64:571–577.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Desmond J,
    2. Fiez J
    (1998) Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: language, learning and memory. Trends Cogn Sci 2:355–362.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Doyon J
    (1997) Skill learning. in The cerebellum and cognition, ed Schmahmann J (Academic, San Diego), pp 273–294.
  9. ↵
    1. Doyon J,
    2. Ungerleider L
    (2002) Functional anatomy of motor skill learning. in Neuropsychology of memory, Ch 18, eds Squire L, Schacter D (Guilford, New York), pp 225–238.
  10. ↵
    1. Doyon J,
    2. Owen A,
    3. Petrides M,
    4. Sziklas V,
    5. Evans A
    (1996) Functional anatomy of visuomotor skill learning in human subjects examined with positron emission tomography. Eur J Neurosci 8:637–648.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Doyon J,
    2. Gaudreau D,
    3. Laforce R,
    4. Castonguay M,
    5. Bedard P,
    6. Bedard F,
    7. Bouchard G
    (1997) Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the learning of a visuomotor skill. Brain Cogn 34:218–245.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Doyon J,
    2. Laforce R,
    3. Bouchard G,
    4. Gaudreau D,
    5. Roy J,
    6. Poirier M,
    7. Bedard P,
    8. Bedard F,
    9. Bouchard J-P
    (1998) Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal-lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements. Neuropsychologia 36:625–641.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Doyon J,
    2. Song A,
    3. Lalonde F,
    4. Karni A,
    5. Adams M,
    6. Ungerleider L
    (1999) Plastic changes within the cerebellum associated with motor sequence learning: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 9:S506.
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    1. Elliott R,
    2. Dolan R,
    3. Frith C
    (2000) Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 10:308–317.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Flament D,
    2. Ellermann J,
    3. Kim S-G,
    4. Ugurbil K,
    5. Ebner T
    (1996) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of cerebellar activation during the learning of a visuomotor dissociation task. Hum Brain Mapp 4:210–226.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Gordon A,
    2. Lee J-H,
    3. Flament D,
    4. Ugurbil K,
    5. Ebner T-J
    (1998) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor, sensory and posterior parietal cortical areas during performance of sequential typing movements. Exp Brain Res 121:153–166.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Grafton S,
    2. Woods R,
    3. Tyszka M
    (1994) Functional imaging of procedural motor learning: relating cerebral blood flow with individual subject performance. Hum Brain Map 1:221–234.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. ↵
    1. Grafton ST,
    2. Fagg AH,
    3. Woods RP,
    4. Arbib MA
    (1996) Functional anatomy of pointing and grasping in humans. Cereb Cortex 6:226–237.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Graybiel AM
    (1995) Building action repertoires: memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 5:733–741.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Harrington D,
    2. Haaland K,
    3. Hermanowicz N
    (1998) Temporal processing in the basal ganglia. Neuropsychology 12:3–12.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Hikosaka O,
    2. Nakahara H,
    3. Rand M,
    4. Sakai K,
    5. Lu X,
    6. Nakamura K,
    7. Miyachi S,
    8. Doya K
    (1999) Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends Neurosci 22:464–471.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Ivry R
    (1996) The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:851–857.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Jackson P,
    2. Lafleur M,
    3. Malouin F,
    4. Richards C,
    5. Doyon J
    (2001) Dynamic functional changes associated with the learning of sequential foot movements. NeuroImage 13:1197.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Jenkins I,
    2. Brooks D,
    3. Nixon P,
    4. Frackowiak R,
    5. Passingham R
    (1994) Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography. J Neurosci 14:3775–3790.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Karni A,
    2. Meyer G,
    3. Jezzard P,
    4. Adams M,
    5. Turner R,
    6. Ungerleider L
    (1995) Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature 377:155–158.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Karni A,
    2. Meyer G,
    3. Rey-Hipolito C,
    4. Jezzard P,
    5. Adams M,
    6. Tuner R,
    7. Ungerleider L
    (1998) The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:861–868.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Lafleur M,
    2. Jackson P,
    3. Malouin F,
    4. Richards C,
    5. Evans A,
    6. Doyon J
    (1999) Functional neuroanatomy of executed and imagined sequential movements of the foot in humans examined with PET. Soc Neurosci Abstr 25:1899.
    OpenUrl
  28. ↵
    1. Nudo R,
    2. Milliken G,
    3. Jenkins W,
    4. Merzenich M
    (1996) Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci 16:785–807.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Owen AM,
    2. Evans AC,
    3. Petrides M
    (1996) Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. Cereb Cortex 6:31–38.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Pascual-Leone A,
    2. Grafman J,
    3. Hallett M
    (1994) Modulation of motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. Science 263:1287.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Pascual-Leone A,
    2. Dang N,
    3. Cohen L,
    4. Brasil-Neto J,
    5. Cammarota A,
    6. Hallett M
    (1995) Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation during the acquisition of new fine motor skills. J Neurosci 74:1037–1045.
    OpenUrl
  32. ↵
    1. Penhune V,
    2. Zatorre R,
    3. Evans A
    (1998) Cerebellar contributions to motor timing: a PET study of auditory and visual rhythm reproduction. J Cogn Neurosci 10:752–765.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Petrides M
    (1994) Frontal lobes and working memory: evidence from investigations of the effects of cortical excisions in nonhuman primates. in Handbook of neuropsychology, eds Boller F, Grafman J (Elsevier), pp 59–82.
  34. ↵
    1. Petrides M
    (1995) Functional organization of the human frontal cortex for mnemonic processing: evidence from neuroimaging studies. Ann NY Acad Sci 769:85–96.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Platz T,
    2. Denzler P,
    3. Kaden B,
    4. Mauritz K
    (1994) Motor learning after recovery from hemiparesis. Neuropsychologia 32:1209–1223.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Rao SM,
    2. Harrington DL,
    3. Haaland KY,
    4. Bobholz JA,
    5. Cox RW,
    6. Binder JR
    (1997) Distributed neural systems underlying the timing of movements. J Neurosci 17:5528–5535.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Rauch S,
    2. Whalen P,
    3. Savage C,
    4. Curran T,
    5. Kendrick A,
    6. Brown H,
    7. Bush G,
    8. Breiter H,
    9. Rosen B
    (1997) Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp 5:124–132.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Sadato N,
    2. Ibanex V,
    3. Deiber M-P,
    4. Campbell G,
    5. Leonard M,
    6. Hallett M
    (1996) Frequency-dependent changes of regional cerebral blood flow during finger movements. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 16:23–33.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Sakai K,
    2. Hikosaka O,
    3. Miyauchi S,
    4. Takino R,
    5. Sasaki Y,
    6. Pütz B
    (1998) Transition of brain activation from frontal to parietal areas in visuomotor sequence learning. J Neurosci 18:1827–1840.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Schmahmann J
    (1997) The cerebrocerebellar system. in The Cerebellum and Cognition, ed Schmahmann J (Academic, San Diego), pp 31–55.
  41. ↵
    1. Schmahmann J,
    2. Doyon J,
    3. Toga A,
    4. Petrides M,
    5. Evans A
    (2000) MRI atlas of the human cerebellum. (Academic, San Diego).
  42. ↵
    1. Schultz W,
    2. Tremblay L,
    3. Hollerman J
    (2000) Reward processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Cereb Cortex 10:272–283.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Seitz R,
    2. Canavan A,
    3. Yaguez L,
    4. Herzog H,
    5. Tellmann L,
    6. Knorr U,
    7. Huang Y,
    8. Homberg V
    (1994) Successive roles of the cerebellum and premotor cortices in trajectorial learning. NeuroReport 5:2541–2544.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Seitz R,
    2. Canavan A,
    3. Yaguez L,
    4. Herzog H,
    5. Tellmann L,
    6. Knorr U,
    7. Huang Y,
    8. Homberg V
    (1997) Representations of graphomotor trajectories in the human parietal cortex: evidence for controlled processing and automatic performance. Eur J Neurosci 9:378–389.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Stern C,
    2. Owen A,
    3. Tracey I,
    4. Look R,
    5. Rosen B,
    6. Petrides M
    (2000) Activity in ventrolateral and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during nonspatial visual working memory processing: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage 11:392–399.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Talairach J,
    2. Tournoux P
    (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. (Thieme, New York).
  47. ↵
    1. Thach W
    (1996) On the specific role of the cerebellum in motor learning and cognition: clues from PET activation and lesion studies in man. Behav Brain Sci 19:411–431.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  48. ↵
    1. Toni I,
    2. Krams M,
    3. Turner R,
    4. Passingham R
    (1998) The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: a whole-brain fMRI study. NeuroImage 8:50–61.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Van Mier H
    (2000) Human learning. in Human brain mapping: the Systems, ed Mazziota J (Academic), pp 605–662.
  50. ↵
    1. Van Mier H,
    2. Ojemann J,
    3. Miezin F,
    4. Akbudak E,
    5. Conturo T,
    6. Raichle M,
    7. Peterson S
    (1997) Practice-related changes in motor learning measured by fMRI. Soc Neurosci Abstr 23:1051.
    OpenUrl
  51. ↵
    1. Winstein C,
    2. Merians A,
    3. Sullivan K
    (1999) Motor learning after unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia 37:975–987.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Woods R,
    2. Mazziotta J,
    3. Cherry S
    (1993) MRI-PET registration with an automated algorithm. J Comput Assist Tomogr 17:536–546.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Worsley K,
    2. Evans A,
    3. Marrett S,
    4. Neelin P
    (1992) A three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 12:900–918.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 22 (4)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 22, Issue 4
15 Feb 2002
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dynamic Cortical and Subcortical Networks in Learning and Delayed Recall of Timed Motor Sequences
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dynamic Cortical and Subcortical Networks in Learning and Delayed Recall of Timed Motor Sequences
Virginia B. Penhune, Julien Doyon
Journal of Neuroscience 15 February 2002, 22 (4) 1397-1406; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-01397.2002

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dynamic Cortical and Subcortical Networks in Learning and Delayed Recall of Timed Motor Sequences
Virginia B. Penhune, Julien Doyon
Journal of Neuroscience 15 February 2002, 22 (4) 1397-1406; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-01397.2002
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • MATERIALS AND METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • motor-skill learning
  • motor cortex
  • basal ganglia
  • PMC
  • cerebellum
  • frontal lobe
  • memory
  • human
  • procedural learning

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

ARTICLE

  • Emotion Processing in Chimeric Faces: Hemispheric Asymmetries in Expression and Recognition of Emotions
  • Single-Cell Microarray Analysis in Hippocampus CA1: Demonstration and Validation of Cellular Heterogeneity
  • Gating of Hippocampal-Evoked Activity in Prefrontal Cortical Neurons by Inputs from the Mediodorsal Thalamus and Ventral Tegmental Area
Show more ARTICLE

Behavioral/Systems

  • Emotion Processing in Chimeric Faces: Hemispheric Asymmetries in Expression and Recognition of Emotions
  • Single-Cell Microarray Analysis in Hippocampus CA1: Demonstration and Validation of Cellular Heterogeneity
  • Gating of Hippocampal-Evoked Activity in Prefrontal Cortical Neurons by Inputs from the Mediodorsal Thalamus and Ventral Tegmental Area
Show more Behavioral/Systems
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.