Abstract
The koniocellular (K) pathway is one of three pathways from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) to primate visual cortex (V1). K pathway projections to the cytochrome oxidase (CO) blobs of V1 suggest involvement in chromatic processing given reports that the CO blobs in diurnal primates contain cells selective for color. K LGN layers and CO blobs, however, are also well developed in nocturnal primates such as owl monkeys, which are likely to be color blind. Thus, the K pathway plays either different roles in different species or some as yet unidentified common role(s). Because synaptic arrangements underlie functional mechanisms, the purpose of this investigation was to compare the synaptic circuitry related to the K pathway within the CO blobs of two diurnal primates (macaque monkeys and squirrel monkeys) and one nocturnal primate (owl monkey). Presynaptic K axons were labeled with wheat germ agglutinin-HRP, and presynaptic and postsynaptic profiles in CO blobs were identified with post-embedding immunocytochemistry for GABA and glutamate. In all three species, K axon terminals are glutamatergic and larger than local axon terminals, suggesting that they have a greater impact on postsynaptic CO blob targets than signals arriving via layer IV from the P or M pathways. A greater proportion of K axons, however, synapse with larger glutamatergic shafts in the diurnal monkeys than in the nocturnal owl monkey, perhaps reflecting the importance of color within the K pathway of these diurnal species. Alternatively, the loss of color vision in the owl monkey could impact K pathway circuitry earlier in the pathway. The basic similarities between K axon circuitry within the CO blobs of the three primate species examined also could indicate that this pathway plays some common role or roles across species.
A neurochemically distinct population of koniocellular (K) neurons makes up a third channel in the primate lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). This pathway has been proposed to function in various sensory as well as modulatory roles (for review, see Casagrande, 1994, 1999; Hendry and Reid, 2000). Although it is still unclear exactly what this pathway contributes to vision, a number of lines of evidence suggest the involvement of the K pathway in color vision. In particular, it has been shown that some K LGN cells in marmosets and macaque monkeys respond to the onset of a blue stimulus (blue ON cells) (Martin et al., 1997; Reid et al., 1997;White et al., 1998). Moreover, the K pathway is the only one of the three LGN pathways [koniocellular (K), magnocellular (M), and parvocellular (P)] that directly terminates within the cytochrome oxidase (CO)-rich blobs of layer III of visual cortex (V1) (Livingstone and Hubel, 1982; Lachica and Casagrande, 1992; Ding and Casagrande, 1995, 1997). Evidence exists that the CO blobs in diurnal macaque monkeys contain a high proportion of cells selective for color [however, see Thompson et al., (1979); Livingstone and Hubel (1984);Ts'o and Gilbert (1988); Lennie et al. (1990)]. This information, combined with recent proposals that midget ganglion cells (retinal input to P LGN cells) may not carry color signals (Calkins and Sterling, 1999) and that K LGN cells could carry all of the color information, has raised major questions concerning the generality of K pathway function across primates. The problem is that K LGN cells and CO blobs are as numerous and prominent in nocturnal primates, such as bush babies and owl monkeys, as they are in diurnal primates (Condo and Casagrande, 1990; Casagrande, 1994; Xu et al., 2001). Bush babies and owl monkeys have only a single mid-wavelength (M) cone and no long (L) and short (S) wavelength (i.e., blue) cones (Wikler and Rakic, 1990;Jacobs et al., 1993). In fact, it is highly likely that both bush babies and owl monkeys are color blind (G. H. Jacobs, personal communication) and use their single cones simply to extend their dynamic visual range. These facts raise questions about the functional generality of the K pathway across species. If K LGN cells provide a unique conduit for color signals in diurnal primates, what are they doing in nocturnal primates? Available data suggest that connections and neurochemistry of the K pathway are quite similar in diurnal and nocturnal primates (for review, see Casagrande, 1994). Perhaps the K pathway simply does different things in diurnal and nocturnal primates. Alternatively, the K pathway may play a more general role in all primates that remains to be identified. The purpose of this study was to investigate the resemblance and differences in the synaptic circuitry related to the K pathway within the CO blobs of two diurnal primates (the Old World macaque monkeys and the New World squirrel monkeys) and a nocturnal primate (the New World owl monkeys).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals. For this study we used two adult squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus), two adult macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and four adult owl monkeys (Aotus trivirgatus). All of the animals were cared for according to theNational Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the guidelines of the Vanderbilt University Animal Care Committee.
K axon labeling. Details of the surgery are similar to those described earlier (Lachica and Casagrande, 1992; Lachica et al., 1993;Ding and Casagrande, 1997, 1998). Briefly, before surgery, atropine sulfate (0.1 mg/kg) was given to inhibit salivation. To inject tracer into the LGN K layers, monkeys were deeply anesthetized with isofluorane (3–4%) in oxygen and maintained with the same gas mixture at 1–2% during the surgery. All procedures were performed under aseptic conditions while the animals were deeply anesthetized. Heart and respiration rates were monitored continuously, and reflexes were tested periodically; animals were kept warm with a water-circulating heating pad throughout surgery. Once deeply anesthetized, the monkeys were secured in a stereotaxic apparatus, the skull was exposed, a craniotomy was performed, and a dural flap was elevated. For identification of LGN layers, responses evoked by a flashing light were recorded extracellularly through a tungsten electrode (5 MΩ; FHC Inc., Bowdoinham, ME). When the K3 layer of LGN was identified on the basis of changes in eye dominance, the electrode was removed and a glass pipette (20–30 μm inner tip diameter) filled with 1% wheat germ agglutinin conjugated to horseradish peroxidase (WGA-HRP) in saline was inserted at the same location. Recordings then were made to verify the LGN laminar position of the pipette for centering the injections within the K3 layer. Next, WGA-HRP (∼4 μl) was pressure injected slowly over 8 min. Each injection was large enough to cover all LGN layers within zones approximately one-third to one-half the volume of the LGN. Figure 1 shows an example of one of our injections made in macaque monkey LGN. When the injection was complete, the pipette was removed, the dural flap was repositioned, and the skin was sutured.
Injection site within the macaque LGN. Note that the tip of the pipette (indicated by an arrow) is placed within K3 but that all LGN layers are labeled in one portion of the LGN. K layers are indicated as K1, K2,K3, K4, K5, andK6. Scale bar, 1 mm.
After the operation, all animals were given 0.01 mg/kg of buprenorphin as analgesic and 300,000 U/kg of long-acting penicillin (Flocillin) and monitored carefully until they were fully conscious and capable of eating and drinking on their own. At that point, animals were returned to their home cages and provided with soft palatable foods and water.
Histological procedures. After a 2 d survival period, the animals were deeply anesthetized with an overdose of Nembutal. The animals were initially perfused transcardially with a brief rinse of oxygenated saline, then perfused with 2.0% paraformaldehyde/1.5% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer, pH 7.4, at 4°C, and finally with the same fixative containing 4.0% sucrose. Total duration of the perfusion was ∼15 min. The brains were removed and post-fixed in 4.0% sucrose in the fixative at 4°C for 1 hr. They were then rinsed three times in 0.1 m phosphate buffer, pH 7.4, and placed in 4.0% sucrose in the same buffer at 4°C overnight. The following day, visual cortex was blocked anterior to V2 and dissected from the remainder of the cortical hemisphere. Then, parasagittal 80 μm sections were cut on a vibratome. The thalamus was removed, frozen, and sectioned parasagittally at 52 μm on a freezing microtome.
WGA-HRP histochemistry. All cortical and LGN sections were treated with a modified tetramethyl benzidine (TMB) and stabilization procedure (Mesulam, 1978; Horn and Hoffman, 1987). Sections to be used for light microscopy were then mounted on gelatin-coated slides, air dried, defatted, dipped briefly in a clearing agent (Histo-Clear), and coverslipped. Counterstaining was unnecessary in these cases because LGN and cortical layers were clearly visible in the TMB-treated sections. All LGN injection sites were reconstructed from serial sections using a microprojector at low magnification (170×) to document the location and extent of the WGA-HRP label.
Electron microscopic post-embedding immunocytochemistry. All cortical sections were post-fixed with 1.0% osmium tetroxide in 0.1m phosphate buffer at 4°C and stained en bloc with uranyl acetate (2.0% solution in 70% ethanol, for 1 hr at 4°C), subsequently dehydrated in an ascending series of graded ethanols, and embedded in Epon resin overnight. After polymerization at 60°C for 2 d and before ultrathin sectioning, slices containing WGA-HRP-labeled K axons in cortical layer IIIB were dissected from the remainder of the tissue with a NeuroPunch (Ted Pella, Redding, CA). The neuropunches were selected from cortical regions with the most intense label. Within these cortical areas, clear axonal label was evident within layers VI, both halves of layer IVC, IVA, and IIIB blobs, and layer I, demonstrating that all P, M, and K LGN layers were involved in the injection. The neuropunches were centered on the patches of WGA-HRP label within layer IIIB. Because K axons terminate as patches that colocalize with the CO blobs in all species that have been examined, including macaque monkey (Livingstone and Hubel, 1982;Fitzpatrick et al., 1983; Lachica and Casagrande, 1992; Casagrande et al., 1997; Ding and Casagrande, 1997, 1998), we made the assumption in this study that the patches of label visualized within layer IIIB of V1 originated from K axons. A small portion of cortical layers IIIA and IIIC was included within these punches but was removed later when the block was trimmed before ultrathin sectioning. Ultrathin sections (∼70 nm) were cut with a diamond knife using an Ultracut E (AO/Reichert) and collected on uncoated 200-mesh nickel grids (EM Sciences, Washington, PA).
A modification of the technique originally described by Phend et al. (1992) was used in this study to optimize post-embedding double immunocytochemistry for glutamate and GABA. Briefly, ultrathin sections on grids were rinsed for 5 min in TBST (0.9% NaCl, 0.1% Triton X-100 in 0.05 m Tris, pH 7.6) and then incubated overnight with anti-glutamate primary antibody (rabbit polyclonal; Chemicon International, Temecula, CA) diluted 1:50 in TBST. After rinsing in TBST, pH 7.6, and TBST, pH 8.2, sections were incubated in secondary goat anti-rabbit antibody conjugated to 10 nm gold (Ted Pella) diluted 1:20 in TBST, pH 8.2, for 1 hr. After completion of the glutamate immunostaining, the binding sites were deactivated by exposure to paraformaldehyde vapors in an 80°C oven for 1 hr. Sections were then incubated with anti-GABA antibody (rabbit polyclonal; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) diluted 1:2000 in TBST, pH 7.6, for 24 hr, followed by incubation in a goat anti-rabbit IgG conjugated to 30 nm gold (Ted Pella) diluted 1:20 for 1 hr. After washing in deionized water, the sections were counterstained with uranyl acetate and lead citrate (Reynolds, 1963) and examined using a Hitachi H-800 transmission electron microscope with an acceleration voltage of 100 kV. As controls, sections were incubated in solutions omitting each step from the regular staining sequence, keeping the rest of the procedure the same as described above. Sections processed in this way were totally devoid of gold particles. Controls were also done to determine denaturation of free anti-IgG binding sites for the first secondary antibody (goat anti-rabbit IgG conjugated to 10 nm gold). The efficacy of the latter deactivation was almost 100%.
Data collection and analysis. All labeled K axon terminals (WGA-HRP positive) making synaptic contact were analyzed from all three species in at least four to five different punches (i.e., separate blocks) with 10 sections for each punch. Different punches were taken from different CO blobs. Terminal labeling and synapse identification were confirmed by examining at least two adjacent sections. All labeled terminals were photographed at 20,000×. The total CO blob area examined for all three species of monkey was the same (for squirrel monkeys, 1805.76 μm2; for owl monkeys, 1787.52 μm2; for macaque monkeys, 1696.32 μm2). Typically, glutamatergic profiles contain only small gold particles, and GABAergic profiles contain only big gold particles with a few small gold particles, indicative of glutamate as a precursor in GABAergic cells. Presynaptic and postsynaptic profiles were considered immunoreactive for glutamate if the ratio of big (30 nm) to small (10 nm) gold particles was <0.0057/1 μm2 and immunoreactive for GABA if the ratio was >1.047/1 μm2(p = 0.013; t test). The above-mentioned ratios for glutamate and GABA were estimated on photographs taken from five morphologically distinctive glutamatergic and GABAergic cells. The total area of dendrites and synapses from these cells was measured, and the total numbers of big and small particles in that area were counted to obtain the gold ratios per 1.0 μm2 of profile area for glutamatergic and GABAergic cells.
In the present study, we used the criteria proposed originally byFreund et al. (1989) to distinguish dendritic spines from shafts. According to their criteria, all dendritic profiles that contain mitochondria and microtubules were classified as dendritic shafts regardless of their diameter, and all profiles lacking mitochondria and microtubules were classified as dendritic spines.
On the basis of the ultrastructural “size principle” proposed byPierce and Mendell (1993), in which morphological features associated with synaptic release scale directly in proportion to terminal size, the cross-sectional areas of all labeled terminals and postsynaptic dendritic shafts, in which both presynaptic and postsynaptic profiles were distinct, were measured by tracing the contour of the profiles using the BioQuant-IV image analysis system (R&M Biometric, Nashville, TN). We compared both K axon terminal sizes with all other axon terminal sizes within the local area and dendritic shafts postsynaptic to K axons with those postsynaptic to all local axons. The size of the local area was 27.36 μm2 for all measurements. Results were analyzed using a Student t test for comparisons of two groups and an ANOVA with a post hoc t test for comparison of three or more groups (p ≤ 0.05 was considered significant). No efforts were made to quantitatively compare data across the three species of monkeys because of variation in the overall size of processes and differences in the shrinkage coefficient for each species of monkey.
RESULTS
Both similarities and differences in the synaptic arrangements of K axons were found across the three primate species. In all three species, K axon terminals contain glutamate, are larger than the local population of axon terminals, and contact mainly spines; a significant minority also contact glutamatergic dendritic shafts. Approximately 15% of K axons synapse on GABAergic dendrites. The most intriguing differences between the two diurnal monkeys and the one nocturnal monkey were the following: in the macaque and squirrel monkeys, a greater percentage of K axons terminated on dendritic shafts than in the owl monkey, and the glutamatergic dendritic shafts postsynaptic to K axons were significantly larger than the glutamatergic dendritic shafts postsynaptic to the local population of axons only in the diurnal monkeys. These results are discussed in more detail below.
Similarities between species
A total of 165 WGA-HRP-labeled K axon terminal profiles were identified (46 from owl monkeys, 66 from squirrel monkeys, and 53 from macaque monkeys) (Table 1) by the presence of electron-dense reaction product. These geniculocortical K terminals, containing one to several mitochondria, were loosely packed with clear round vesicles (Fig. 2) and were all immunopositive only for glutamate, as identified by the presence of an appropriate number of small (10 nm) gold particles (Figs. 2-7) and the absence of large (30 nm) gold particles. These K axon terminals were exclusively presynaptic and usually formed small, single, asymmetric synaptic contacts with dendritic profiles, both spines (Fig. 2) and shafts (Fig. 3), but not with cell bodies or other axons.
Postsynaptic targets of K axon terminals
K axons (K), identified by black diffuse electron-dense reaction product, mainly make single asymmetric synaptic contacts (examples are indicated by arrows) with glutamatergic dendritic spines (sp). Small gold particles (10 nm) showing immunoreactivity for glutamate are seen as small black dots. A, Squirrel monkey; B, macaque monkey; C, owl monkey. Scale bar, 0.5 μm.
Squirrel monkey. A WGA-HRP-labeled K axon (K), identified by blackdiffuse electron-dense reaction product, makes an asymmetric perforated synapse (arrow) onto a glutamatergic dendritic shaft (d). Small gold particles (10 nm) showing immunoreactivity for glutamate are seen as small black dots. Scale bar, 0.5 μm.
Using the criteria described by Freund and colleagues (1989), we determined that most of the K axons in all three species of monkeys (80.4% in owl monkey, 54.6% in squirrel monkey, and 60.4% in macaque monkey) terminate on dendritic spines (Fig. 2), with the remainder terminating on dendritic shafts (Figs. 3,4, Table 1).
Macaque monkey. A K axon (K), identified by electron-dense WGA-HRP reaction product, makes two synaptic contacts (thin arrows) with one glutamatergic dendritic shaft (d). The dendritic shaft receives another synaptic contact (thick arrow) from an unlabeled axon terminal. Note that the target for the K terminal here might be interpreted as the shaft and spine portions of a single dendrite. Scale bar, 0.5 μm.
On the basis of single section analysis, K axons tend to be involved in simple synaptic relationships with either one dendritic spine or one dendritic shaft. All of the dendritic spines receiving synaptic input from a labeled K terminal appear to get exclusive input from that terminal. However, a number of larger dendritic shafts that receive synaptic input from K axons also receive synaptic input from identified GABAergic axons (see Fig. 6), identified glutamatergic axons, and other axonal sources that were not clearly labeled for either glutamate or GABA (Figs. 4, 7). Occasionally, K axons make two synaptic contacts with the same dendritic shaft (Fig. 4). K axons can also exhibit perforated synapses (Fig. 3) with dendritic shafts (Nieto-Sampedro et al., 1982).
As shown in Table 1, most of the postsynaptic targets for K axons in all three species of monkeys were immunoreactive for glutamate (86.3% for owl monkey, 77.6% for squirrel monkey, and 88.7% for macaque monkey); a minority were positive for GABA (5.8, 15, and 5.6%, respectively), and the remainder of the postsynaptic targets were unlabeled. Examples of postsynaptic labeling for glutamate and GABA are shown in Figures2-6 and7, respectively.
Squirrel monkey. A K axon (K) makes three synaptic contacts with spines (sp); one of them is a perforated synapse. In this case, two spines were identified as glutamatergic on the basis of the presence of small gold particles, and one was unlabeled (asterisk). This K axon likely makes another synaptic contact with the GABAergic (note large gold particles) dendritic shaft (GABA d), but the synapse is not visible in this plane of section. Larger gold particles (30 nm) showing immunoreactivity for GABA are seen as larger black dots. Scale bar, 0.5 μm.
Squirrel monkey. A glutamatergic dendrite (d) with spine (sp) receives two synaptic contacts, one from a K axon (thick arrow) and another from a GABAergic terminal (GABA term;thin arrow). Black amorphous material indicates WGA-HRP electron-dense reaction product in the K axon (K). Scale bar, 0.5 μm.
Squirrel monkey. Some K axons are involved in complex synaptic arrangements. This K axon (K) synapses (indicated by a thick arrows) on both an inhibitory GABAergic dendritic shaft (GABA d) and a spine (sp). Larger gold particles (30 nm) showing immunoreactivity for GABA are seen aslarger black dots. Note that the same GABAergic dendritic shaft receives additional input from two other glutamatergic axon terminals (thin arrows). Blackamorphous material indicates WGA-HRP electron-dense reaction product in the K axon. Scale bar, 0.5 μm.
We made a number of additional measurements in an effort to provide more detailed information about similarities and differences between the synaptic arrangements made by K LGN axons and other local axons (e.g., axons from layer IV carrying indirect signals from the LGN M and P pathways). First, we evaluated the cross-sectional area of all labeled K axon terminals and postsynaptic dendritic shafts in which both presynaptic and postsynaptic profiles were distinct. K axon terminal and target sizes were compared with the sizes of all other local glutamatergic axonal profiles and their dendritic shaft targets. Only dendritic shafts that could be clearly identified as containing glutamate or GABA as a transmitter were included in the measurements. Table 2 shows that for all three primate species, K axon terminals were significantly larger in area (on average twice the size) when compared with the remaining local population of glutamatergic axon terminals. The size distribution of K axon terminals in comparison to the local population of glutamate containing axon terminals is shown in Figure 8.
Sizes of axon terminals
Squirrel monkey. K axon terminals (black bars) are on average larger in size than local axon terminals (white bars) in the local area of CO blobs. Thearrows and the numbers above them show, respectively, the mean size of K axon terminals (1.79 ± 0.16 μm2) and the mean size of all local glutamatergic axon terminals (0.95 ± 0.10 μm2). See Results for details.
Second, we compared the size of K axon terminals that made synapses with spines with those that made synapses with dendritic shafts containing either glutamate or GABA. Comparisons were made separately for each of the three primate species. No differences were found using an ANOVA (p = 0.98 for owl monkeys;p = 0.88 for squirrel monkeys; p = 0.97 for macaque monkeys).
A similar comparison was made between the sizes of GABAergic dendritic shafts receiving synapses from K axons and those receiving input from other local glutamatergic axons. We found that in all three primate species, K axons tend to make contact with larger postsynaptic GABAergic dendritic shafts than do other local axons (1.63 ± 0.09 μm2, n = 3 vs 0.82 ± 0.07 μm2, n = 69 for owl monkeys; 2.24 ± 0.63 μm2,n = 9 vs 1.65 ± 0.19 μm2, n = 12 for squirrel monkeys; and 1.44 ± 0.58 μm2,n = 3 vs 0.74 ± 0.08 μm2, n = 53 for macaque monkeys for all local GABAergic dendritic shafts, respectively). Larger samples will be required to determine whether these differences are significant.
Differences between species
Two measures appeared to distinguish the diurnal monkeys (macaque and squirrel monkeys) from the nocturnal monkey (owl monkey). In the diurnal species a much larger percentage (38–40%) of the total population of K axons terminated on glutamatergic dendritic shafts (as opposed to spines) than in the nocturnal owl monkey (25%). In addition, as shown in Table 3, when we compared the sizes of postsynaptic glutamatergic dendritic shafts contacted by K axons with the sizes of all glutamatergic dendritic shafts in the local area, we found that K axons in both diurnal primates terminate on relatively larger glutamatergic postsynaptic dendritic shafts than do K axons in the nocturnal owl monkeys.
Sizes of postsynaptic glutamatergic dendritic shafts
In addition to differences that appeared to correlate with visual lifestyle (diurnal versus nocturnal), we found one species difference that correlated with evolutionary distance. In the New World simians, a small percentage of K axons makes multiple synaptic contacts. In owl monkeys 15.2% of K axons (7 of 46) and in squirrel monkeys 9.1% of K axons (6 of 66) make contacts with several postsynaptic targets: with one spine and one shaft (Fig. 7) or with more than one spine (Fig. 5). In contrast, none of the 53 K axons in macaque monkeys made multiple contacts with postsynaptic targets. These percentages, however, are likely to be an underestimate because serial reconstructions through the entire bouton were not performed.
A final species difference that did not correlate with either life style or evolutionary similarity was the finding that ∼50% of K axons in squirrel monkeys make perforated synaptic contacts with their targets versus ∼5–6% in owl and macaque monkeys.
DISCUSSION
Common features of K LGN circuits across primates
Although CO blobs are recognized as a ubiquitous feature of primate V1, the role of the CO blobs in vision remains controversial. At one extreme are arguments that regions exhibiting dense CO staining are simply zones of thalamic input where the metabolism in cortex is slightly higher; CO blobs are the targets for K axons, just as layer IV (another CO-dense layer) is the target of M and P axons. At the other extreme is the proposal that CO blobs are artifacts of activity-dependent rules of development (Purves and LaMantia, 1990). The most popular proposal still remains that CO blobs reflect functional specialization for color in diurnal primates (Livingstone and Hubel, 1984; Ts'o and Gilbert, 1988). Support for a role in color also comes from published data on K LGN cells in marmosets (Martin et al., 1997; White et al., 1998) and preliminary data in macaque monkeys (Reid et al., 1997) suggesting that a percentage of K cells (20% in marmosets) responds to signals from S cones (blue ON); however, no LGN K cells have been identified that respond to red/green (i.e., L and M cones). Therefore, if CO blobs are part of a color channel, they must combine chromatic signals from P and K cells. If this is true, then we still need to understand what CO blobs are doing in nocturnal primates, such as owl monkeys, that do not express the S cone pigment gene (Jacobs et al., 1993) and are most likely color blind (Jacobs et al., 1996). Interestingly, in many respects (morphology and connectivity), the S cone (blue) system more closely resembles the rod system than the L and M cone channels in vision (Kolb et al., 1997). In fact, some evidence suggests that rods participate in the “blue” mechanism (Trezona, 1970) and signals from rods and S cones can interact (Naarendorp et al., 1996) and share the same pathway. Virsu and colleagues (1987) reported such sharing of signals from rods and S cones in macaque LGN, although this result remains controversial (Lee et al., 1997). If true, it could suggest a mechanism to explain why the K pathway is preserved in nocturnal primates. Perhaps the portion of the K pathway that is dominated by S cones in diurnal monkeys becomes part of a second rod-dominated pathway in nocturnal primates. This is in agreement with an older proposal that cells in the CO blobs are concerned, in general, with contrast (color or brightness) as opposed to cells in the interblobs that are more concerned with geometrical variables (Allman and Zucker, 1990; DeBruyn et al., 1993). This idea would also agree with our finding that the K pathway circuitry is quite similar across diurnal and nocturnal primates. Thus, under nocturnal conditions the K CO blob pathway may simply switch in nocturnal primates to a rod pathway.
In fact, the similarities in the K pathway far outweigh the differences, suggesting that there has not been a major change in the K pathway circuitry within the CO blobs that correlates with the shift from color vision to lack of color vision and that the common structural elements of the K pathway across primates reflect some function that they share or that all LGN input pathways share. The similarities that we see between K axon populations include the demonstration that K axons are (1) glutamatergic, (2) make asymmetric (Gray's type I) synapses mainly with spines, and (3) engage in simple synaptic arrangements with glutamatergic postsynaptic targets typically involving single synaptic contacts. The tendency for K geniculocortical axons to contact predominantly spines of excitatory cells extends also to both cat X and Y geniculocortical boutons in area 17 and cat Y axons in area 18, and M and P axons in layer IV of primates (Garey and Powell, 1971; Tigges and Tigges, 1979; Winfield et al., 1982; Winfield and Powell, 1983; Freund et al., 1989; Ding and Casagrande, 1998), as well as to rat geniculocortical axons in area 17 (Garey and Powell, 1971; LeVay and Gilbert, 1976; Winfield and Powell, 1976, 1983; Peters and Feldman, 1977; Freund et al., 1985, Kharazia and Weinberg, 1994), suggesting that these are general features of all geniculocortical axons in mammals.
In addition to spines and dendritic shafts containing glutamate, a minority (5–15%) of K axons in all three species makes synapses with the dendritic shafts of inhibitory (GABAergic) interneurons. This percentage of contacts with inhibitory interneurons is similar to that of the local population of axons, suggesting that K axons do not relate in unique ways to the GABAergic circuitry, with the exception that K axons tend to contact larger GABAergic dendrites than the local population of axons (see also below). Approximately 15–20% of all cortical neurons are GABAergic (Fitzpatrick et al., 1987; Hendry et al., 1987), and the proportion of GABAergic dendrites reported in the CO blob neuropil of macaque monkeys is similar (Beaulieu et al., 1992;Nie and Wong-Riley 1995).
Comparison among K, M, and P LGN synaptic circuits
The CO blobs in layer IIIB receive numerous intrinsic and a greater variety of extrinsic connections than have been reported in layer IV. In addition to axons from cells in both IVα and IVβ layers (the M and P recipient sublayers), CO blobs receive input from cells in all cortical layers except layer I, as well as input from local cells (Lund and Yoshioka, 1991; Peters and Sethares, 1991;Casagrande et al., 1992; Lachica et al., 1992, 1993; Boyd et al., 2000). Extrinsic connections of the CO blobs include not only the LGN K pathway, but also input from a number of extrastriate visual areas (e.g., feedback from V2), as well as input from the pulvinar (Kaas and Huerta, 1988; Desimone et al., 1990). Within the CO blobs there are no spiny stellate cells (Lund, 1984). Therefore, in the CO blobs, all dendritic spines arise from either local pyramidal cells or pyramidal cells from other layers, especially layer V (Peters and Sethares, 1991). These relationships indicate that K axons could have direct access to output neurons, unlike the axons of M and P cells, which terminate exclusively on spiny stellate cells in layer IV and avoid pyramidal dendrites (Lund, 1984). Our quantitative comparison reveals that K axon terminals are larger in area than local glutamatergic axon terminals. This suggests that K axons have a greater impact on V1 output neurons, unless the terminals arriving from layer IV (reflecting the indirect inputs from the P and M pathways) are skewed in size away from the local mean. Although the proportion of GABAergic target elements contacted by the K pathway (5–15%) is similar to that described in the literature for M and P cells (5–10%) (Freund et al., 1989), it is intriguing that K axons in all three monkey species contact significantly larger postsynaptic GABAergic dendritic shafts than do local axons, suggesting that K axons terminate closer to the cell bodies of these inhibitory cells than axons carrying M and P signals from layer IV.
Taken together, these comparisons among the synaptic relationships of K, M, and P axons suggest that all three parallel LGN streams use similar strategies at the first stage of information transfer to cortical circuits, but that the K pathway has a stronger than suspected impact on messages outgoing from V1 given the location and size of its axons. Additionally, the K pathway appears to be in a position to more efficiently activate inhibitory neurons. This could allow signals arriving from K axons to be more rapidly shut down in comparison to signals arriving via M and P axons, at least according to models that propose feedforward inhibition of incoming LGN afferent signals (Somogyi, 1989; Berman et al., 1991, 1992; Douglas and Martin, 1991).
Differences in K LGN circuits across primates
Despite the many across-species similarities in K pathway organization, there are intriguing differences. The most interesting of these differences are those that could correlate with the presence or absence of color vision. Comparison of the diurnal monkeys (macaque and squirrel monkeys) that have color vision with a nocturnal owl monkey that lacks color vision showed that a greater proportion of K axons in the diurnal monkeys synapse with dendritic shafts (as opposed to spines) as well as with larger glutamatergic dendritic shafts. This shift could reflect a loss of one population of K axons in owl monkeys, a population conveying information from the missing S cone pathway (Jacobs et al., 1996). This change also fits with data in marmosets showing that only a small proportion of K axons (20%) actually carry color signals (i.e., S cone signals); the remainder do something else (Martin et al., 1997; White et al., 1998). Such a scenario would also fit the proposed evolutionary history of the owl monkey, suggesting that the ancestors of owl monkeys had S cones (and likely color vision) and that they only recently lost S cones under some pressure to become nocturnal (Martin, 1990). Another conclusion that could be drawn from these data is that the K pathway input to CO blob glutamatergic dendrites in diurnal primates occurs at sites that can exert relatively greater dominance compared with other afferents, and that this distinctive feature of the K pathway has been minimized in the nocturnal owl monkey, perhaps as a consequence to the loss of color vision. In that case the prediction would be that other diurnal primates with S cones (e.g., marmosets) should show the pattern that we have found in the macaque and squirrel monkeys, whereas other nocturnal primates that lack S cones (e.g., bush babies) should show the pattern seen in the owl monkey. Nevertheless, these diurnal/nocturnal differences account for only a small percentage of the K axons in these three species (∼20%). These findings support the conclusion that most of the signals that the K pathway conveys to the CO blobs are concerned with something other than color.
Footnotes
This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant EY01778 (V.A.C.) and core Grants EY08126 and HD15052. We are especially grateful to Mary Feurtado for her excellent assistance with animal care and anesthesia and to Dr. Gyula Sáry for expert assistance in surgery. We also thank Dr. Jeffrey Schall, Dr. Jon Kaas, Dr. Amy Wiencken-Barger, Jennifer Ichida, Xiangmin Xu, Zhuang Song, and David Royal for helpful comments on this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Vivien A. Casagrande, Department of Cell Biology, Vanderbilt University Medical School, Medical Center North C-2310, Nashville, TN 372327-2175. E-mail:vivien.casagrande{at}mcmail.vanderbilt.edu.
Y. Ding's present address: Department of Neurological Surgery, Wayne State University Medical School, University Health Center, 4201 St. Antonine 6E, Detroit, MI 48201.