Abstract
Patients and models of temporal lobe epilepsy have fewer inhibitory interneurons in the dentate gyrus than controls, but it is unclear whether granule cell inhibition is reduced. We report the loss of GABAergic inhibition of granule cells in the temporal dentate gyrus of pilocarpine-induced epileptic rats. In situhybridization for GAD65 mRNA and immunocytochemistry for parvalbumin and somatostatin confirmed the loss of inhibitory interneurons. In epileptic rats, granule cells had prolonged EPSPs, and they discharged more action potentials than controls. Although the conductances of evoked IPSPs recorded in normal ACSF were not significantly reduced and paired-pulse responses showed enhanced inhibition of granule cells from epileptic rats, more direct measures of granule cell inhibition revealed significant deficiencies. In granule cells from epileptic rats, evoked monosynaptic IPSP conductances were <40% of controls, and the frequency of GABAA receptor-mediated spontaneous and miniature IPSCs (mIPSCs) was <50% of controls. Within 3–7 d after pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus, miniature IPSC frequency had decreased, and it remained low, without functional evidence of compensatory synaptogenesis by GABAergic axons in chronically epileptic rats. Both parvalbumin- and somatostatin-immunoreactive interneuron numbers and the frequency of both fast- and slow-rising GABAA receptor-mediated mIPSCs were reduced, suggesting that loss of inhibitory synaptic input to granule cells occurred at both proximal/somatic and distal/dendritic sites. Reduced granule cell inhibition in the temporal dentate gyrus preceded the onset of spontaneous recurrent seizures by days to weeks, so it may contribute, but is insufficient, to cause epilepsy.
- dentate gyrus
- hippocampus
- GABA
- interneurons
- IPSPs
- IPSCs
- GAD
- somatostatin
- parvalbumin
- mossy fiber sprouting
- basal dendrites
- paired pulse
Introduction
Temporal lobe epilepsy is the most common type of epilepsy in adults (Engel et al., 1997). Patients with temporal lobe epilepsy display specific patterns of neuron loss, and the most consistent loss occurs in the hilus of the dentate gyrus (Margerison and Corsellis, 1966). Some of the missing cells are interneurons (de Lanerolle et al., 1989; Mathern et al., 1995a; Wittner et al., 2001). Whether granule cells, the principal neurons of the dentate gyrus, are less inhibited is an important but unresolved issue.
Despite the loss of interneurons, many previous studies that evaluated tissue from patients or models of temporal lobe epilepsy found evidence of normal or enhanced granule cell inhibition. Surviving GABAergic interneurons have been reported to sprout axon collaterals and form new inhibitory synapses with granule cells (Babb et al., 1989; Davenport et al., 1990; Mathern et al., 1995a, 1997; Andre et al., 2001;Wittner et al., 2001). Unit and field potential recordings provide evidence of robust inhibition in tissue from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (Isokawa-Akesson et al., 1989; Uruno et al., 1995; Colder et al., 1996; Wilson et al., 1998). Paired-pulse inhibition of granule cells is enhanced in models of temporal lobe epilepsy (Tuff et al., 1983; Haas et al., 1996; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997). GABAA receptor density is greater in acutely isolated granule cells from epileptic rats compared with controls (Gibbs et al., 1997). Granule cells express more GABAA receptors per synapse in kindled animals compared with controls (Otis et al., 1994; Nusser et al., 1998). Some intracellular and whole-cell patch-clamp studies of granule cells from patients and models of temporal lobe epilepsy report relatively normal inhibitory postsynaptic responses (Franck et al., 1995; Buhl et al., 1996; Isokawa, 1996; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999; Okazaki et al., 1999), but others found reduced IPSPs (Williamson et al., 1995,1999).
To address this unsettled issue we used multiple approaches to evaluate the inhibition of granule cells at different stages of epileptogenesis in the pilocarpine-treated rat model of temporal lobe epilepsy. We compared electrophysiological results with anatomical measures of interneuron loss in adjacent slices. We examined the dentate gyrus in the temporal pole of the hippocampus, which is homologous to the part of the hippocampus that is resected from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy and is the region in which neuron loss in patients and models is most severe (Babb et al., 1984; Masukawa et al., 1996; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997). We found reduced inhibition of granule cells in epileptic rats and asked the following questions. Does the loss of inhibition precede or follow the onset of spontaneous, recurrent seizures? Is there functional evidence that surviving inhibitory interneurons form additional, compensatory synapses in chronically epileptic animals? Where on the granule cells (soma, dendrites, or both) are inhibitory synapses lost?
Materials and Methods
Animal treatment. We used the pilocarpine-treated rat model of temporal lobe epilepsy because it replicates the cell type-specific pattern of neuron loss and axon reorganization found in many patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (Turski et al., 1983; Mello et al., 1993). It also is similar to a common clinical history of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (Mathern et al., 1995b), in that a brain injury precedes a seizure-free latent period before spontaneous, recurrent seizures begin. All experiments were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the Stanford University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Sprague Dawley male rats (35–77 d old) were treated with pilocarpine (380 mg/kg, i.p.) 20 min after atropine methylbromide (5 mg/kg, i.p.). Approximately 60% of the treated rats experienced status epilepticus. Diazepam (10 mg/kg, i.p.) was administered 2–3 hr after onset of status epilepticus and repeated, as needed.
Slice preparation. Animals were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital (75 mg/kg, i.p.) and decapitated. Tissue blocks including the dentate gyrus were removed rapidly and stored for 3 min in modified ice-cold artificial CSF (M-ACSF) containing (in mm): 230 sucrose, 2.5 KCl, 10 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2.5 CaCl2, and 10d-glucose. Horizontal slices were cut by a microslicer (Lancer series 3000, Vibratome, St. Louis, MO) into 425-μm-thick sections for sharp intracellular electrode recording and 350-μm-thick sections for whole-cell patch-clamp recording. Slices were incubated at 32°C for 40 min in a submersion-type holding chamber that contained 50% M-ACSF and 50% normal ACSF (ACSF), pH 7.35–7.40. ACSF contained (in mm): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 2 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2.0 CaCl2, and 10d-glucose. After that, slices were placed in ACSF at 32°C for 1 hr. ACSF was aerated continuously with a mixture of 95% O2/5% CO2. Slices were thereafter maintained at room temperature until they were used for recording.
Intracellular recording. We used sharp intracellular electrodes and current clamp in some evoked response experiments of the present study. Although whole-cell patch-clamp recording could have been used to measure IPSP conductance, we chose sharp electrode recordings because they can be compared more directly with most of the relevant published studies that also used sharp electrode recordings, they reduce cell dialysis, and in our hands, they generate more consistent and superior intracellular labeling. In a recording chamber (Fine Science Tools, Foster City, CA), slices were maintained at an interface of humidified 95% O2/5% CO2 and ACSF flowing at 1 ml/min and maintained at 30–32°C. Recording electrodes were made from quartz (P-2000, Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA) and filled with 2% biocytin and 1 m potassium acetate (100–180 MΩ). Signals were amplified (AxoClamp 2B and CyberAmp 380, Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA), observed on-line, and stored (pCLAMP, Axon Instruments).
IPSPs were recorded in normal ACSF, and monosynaptic IPSPs were recorded in the presence of glutamate receptor antagonists [50 μm 6-cyano-7-nitro-quinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX) and 100 μmd-2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate (d-APV), RBI, Natick, MA]. IPSP reversal potentials were measured by altering the membrane potential with DC current to change the amplitude and polarity of postsynaptic potentials. IPSP amplitude was plotted against the prestimulus membrane potential, and data were fit with a least-squares regression line to estimate the reversal potential. IPSP conductance was calculated by subtracting the input conductance of the cell before stimulation from its conductance during the IPSP (20 and 150 msec after the stimulus artifact). Conductance was the slope of I–Vcurves, where I was DC holding current and V was membrane potential.
Whole-cell patch-clamp recording. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were obtained from granule cells identified with Nomarski optics (40×; Nikon, Tokyo, Japan) and an infrared-sensitive video camera (Hamamatsu Photonics, Hamamatsu City, Japan). Electrical signals were amplified by a patch amplifier (Axopatch 1-D, Axon Instruments) for voltage-clamp recordings and by an intracellular amplifier (AxoClamp 2B, Axon Instruments) for current-clamp recordings. The composition of the pipette solution for voltage-clamp recordings was (in mm): 120 cesium methanesulfonate, 20 biocytin, 10 HEPES, 8 NaCl, 5 QX-314, 2 magnesium ATP, 0.3 sodium GTP, and 0.1 BAPTA. The presence of QX-314 and cesium in the pipette solution precluded recording GABAB receptor-mediated IPSCs. The pipette solution for whole-cell current-clamp recordings contained (in mm): 100 potassium gluconate, 40 HEPES, 20 biocytin, 10 EGTA, 5 MgCl2, 2 disodium ATP, and 0.3 sodium GTP. Pipette solutions had a pH of 7.3 and osmolarity of 300 mOsm. The liquid junction potential for voltage-clamp recordings was 7 mV, and all voltages were corrected accordingly. Thick-wall borosilicate patch electrodes (4–6 MΩ) were pulled on a Flaming-Brown micropipette puller (P-97, Sutter Instruments).
Recordings were obtained at 30–31°C. Seal resistance was >5 GΩ, and only data obtained from electrodes with access resistance of 8–17 MΩ and <20% change during recordings were included in this study. Series resistance was 80% compensated. Spontaneous (s), miniature (m), and evoked IPSCs were measured near the reversal potential of glutamatergic input (0 mV). For blocking GABAAand glutamate receptors, 10 μm bicuculline methiodide and 25 μm CNQX/50 μmd-APV, respectively, were bath applied. To record mIPSCs, 1 μmtetrodotoxin (TTX) was applied in combination with CNQX andd-APV. Paired patch-clamp recordings were obtained from putative basket cells and granule cells. Basket cells were recorded in current-clamp mode, and bridge balance was adjusted during recording. Membrane currents and potentials were low-pass filtered at 1–2 kHz and digitized at 4–8 kHz.
Data analysis. Both sIPSCs and mIPSCs were automatically detected by customized software (kindly provided by Dr. J. Huguenard, Stanford University) using the second derivative of the current traces as the trigger (Ulrich and Huguenard, 1996). Threshold values were set at three times the SD of baseline noise amplitude. For every recording we visually checked at least 10% of the file to verify that the software accurately detected events. More than 95% of the visually identified events were detected by the software, and false positives accounted for <1% of the events detected by the software. Threshold values for sIPSCs recorded in normal ACSF were 5.6 ± 0.2 pA in epileptic rats (n = 23 cells), 4.9 ± 0.2 pA in adult control rats (n = 42 cells), 4.8 ± 0.2 pA in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats (n = 42 cells), and 4.9 ± 0.2 pA in young control rats (n = 64 cells). Threshold values for mIPSC recordings were 4.0 ± 0.1 pA in epileptic rats (n = 20 cells), 3.6 ± 0.1 pA in adult control rats (n = 36 cells), 3.6 ± 0.1 pA in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats (n = 34 cells), and 3.8 ± 0.2 pA in young control rats (n = 44 cells). IPSC frequency was measured from continuous recordings that were at least 2 min long. The distributions of mIPSC rise times were fit with two Gaussians (Origin, Microcal Software Inc., Northampton, MA). To evaluate levels of spontaneous inhibitory synaptic input, the charge transfer of sIPSCs was summed over a period of time by measuring the area between the trace containing sIPSCs and the baseline. All statistical values are presented as mean ± SEM. Statistical comparisons were performed using a χ2 test, t test, or Spearman Rho test. The level of p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Anatomy. Immediately after slicing, the first and the last slice prepared were placed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1m phosphate buffer (PB), pH 7.4, at 4°C for at least 24 hr. After fixation, slices were stored in 30% ethylene glycol and 25% glycerol in 50 mm PB at −20°C or less. Slices were sectioned with a sliding microtome set at 30 μm. From each slice, one section was stained with thionin. Others were processed for somatostatin or parvalbumin immunoreactivity or GAD65 mRNA in situ hybridization using previously described protocols and reagents (Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997; Buckmaster and Jongen-Rêlo, 1999). The GAD65 cDNA (kindly provided by Drs. A. Tobin and N. Tillakaratne, University of California at Los Angeles) was ∼2.4 kb and was isolated from a λ ZapII library from adult rat hippocampus (Erlander et al., 1991). Neuronal profile counts were measured with a neurolucida system (MicroBrightField, Colchester, VT). A contour was drawn along the borders of the dentate gyrus (see Fig. 1a). The border between the hilus and the CA3 region was drawn as straight lines from the ends of the granule cell layer to the proximal end of the CA3 pyramidal cell layer. Within the contour, all of the neuron profiles of interest were counted (somatostatin- or parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons, GAD65 mRNA-positive neurons, or hilar Nissl-stained neurons). Results from the first and last slice prepared from an animal were averaged. All of the slices used for electrophysiology were between the two slices prepared for anatomy.
To visualize biocytin-labeled neurons after sharp-electrode recording, slices were fixed, cryoprotected, and sectioned (60 μm). Sections were processed using the ABC method (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) and nickel-intensified diaminobenzidine as the chromogen. All chemicals unless specified were purchased from Sigma (St. Louis, MO).
Results
Epileptic rats (n = 25) experienced pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus when they were 51 ± 3 d old. They were video-monitored for seizure activity 40 hr/week. Their first observed spontaneous seizure occurred 26 ± 3 d after status epilepticus, and they were used in an experiment 28 ± 4 d later. Controls for the epileptic rats included age-matched naive rats (n = 17) and pilocarpine-treated rats that did not experience status epilepticus and did not develop epilepsy (n = 21). In some experiments, rats (n= 15) were examined 3–7 d after status epilepticus, before the onset of spontaneous seizures. It is likely that most, if not all, of these rats would have become epileptic, because >90% of the 82 rats that we have video-monitored displayed spontaneous, recurrent seizures within 3 months after pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus. The control group for the 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats included age-matched naive rats (n = 25) and pilocarpine-treated rats that did not have status epilepticus (n = 14). We found no significant differences in the anatomical or electrophysiological results from naive controls versus pilocarpine-treated controls.
Interneuron loss after status epilepticus
Patients with temporal lobe epilepsy have fewer interneurons in the dentate gyrus than controls (de Lanerolle et al., 1989; Mathern et al., 1995a; Wittner et al., 2001). Experimental models of temporal lobe epilepsy display similar patterns of interneuron loss (Sloviter, 1991;Obenaus et al., 1993; Schwarzer et al., 1995; Houser and Esclapez, 1996; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997; Buckmaster and Jongen-Rêlo, 1999; Andre et al., 2001; Gorter et al., 2001). This was true for pilocarpine-treated rats that experienced status epilepticus. Compared with age-matched controls, the number of GAD65 mRNA-positive neuron profiles in the dentate gyrus was reduced to 66 and 71% in epileptic rats and in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats, respectively (Figs.1c,d,2a). The average number of GAD65 mRNA-positive neuron profiles per dentate gyrus in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats (215 ± 18; n = 12) was similar to that of chronically epileptic rats (200 ± 11;n = 24), suggesting that virtually all of the interneuron loss occurred by 3–7 d after status epilepticus.
There are many different types of GABAergic interneurons in the dentate gyrus. Somatostatin-immunoreactive interneurons are abundant, and they synapse preferentially with the distal dendrites of granule cells (Leranth et al., 1990; Katona et al., 1999; Buckmaster et al., 2002a). In contrast, parvalbumin-immunoreactive interneurons synapse preferentially at or near the granule cell body (Kosaka et al., 1987). In slices from control rats, somatostatin-immunoreactive profiles accounted for 27% of the GAD65 mRNA-positive profiles in the dentate gyrus, similar to previous stereological results (Buckmaster and Jongen-Rêlo, 1999). In controls, parvalbumin-positive profiles accounted for 8% of the GAD65 mRNA-positive neuron profiles in the dentate gyrus. After status epilepticus, the average number of both somatostatin- and parvalbumin-immunoreactive interneuron profiles per dentate gyrus was reduced to 46–48% of controls (Figs.1e–h, 2a).
When using neurochemical markers to identify and count neurons, it is important to determine whether neurons die or just reduce marker expression. Seizure activity can reduce parvalbumin immunoreactivity in surviving interneurons (Scotti et al., 1997), and we could not measure parvalbumin-positive interneuron numbers in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats, because somata were not reliably labeled that soon after status epilepticus. In the dentate gyrus, somatostatin-immunoreactive cell bodies are found almost exclusively within the hilus. The number of somatostatin-immunoreactive interneurons was correlated with the number of Nissl-stained hilar neurons in the same slice (Fig. 2b), suggesting that these interneurons were killed and did not just lose their immunoreactivity. Surviving somatostatin-immunoreactive neurons and their axons in the outer molecular layer were labeled more intensely in epileptic rats compared with controls (Fig. 1e,f), confirming previous results from patients (Mathern et al., 1995a) and models of temporal lobe epilepsy (Wanscher et al., 1990; Schwarzer et al., 1995; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997). Similarly, the intensity of GAD65 mRNA expression in somata increased in epileptic rats (Figs.1c,d) (Feldblum et al., 1990; Houser and Esclapez, 1996).
In patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, neuron loss in the dentate gyrus is most severe in the temporal end of the hippocampus (Babb et al., 1984; Masukawa et al., 1996). Similarly, in kainate-treated epileptic rats, interneuron loss is most severe in the temporal dentate gyrus (Buckmaster and Jongen-Rêlo, 1999), and frequently the temporal dentate gyrus is the only dentate region in which the number of parvalbumin-immunoreactive interneurons is reduced (Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997). We found similar results in pilocarpine-induced epileptic rats. All of the slices examined in the present study were from the temporal part of the hippocampus, but some slices were more temporal than others. GAD65 mRNA-positive neuron loss was more severe in the ventral (most temporal) slices than in the dorsal slices. The number of GAD65 mRNA-positive neuron profiles was reduced to 58% in ventral slices and to 70% of controls in dorsal slices.
Granule cells are hyperexcitable in epileptic rats
Perforant path fibers in the outer two-thirds of the molecular layer provide the predominant excitatory synaptic input to granule cells. The outer molecular layer was stimulated with a bipolar electrode (25 μm diameter, insulated stainless steel wires) placed 100–300 μm off-center from the impaled cell. Stimulus intensity was gradually increased and set to evoke the maximal amplitude IPSP at a 150 msec latency (0.2 msec, 0.1 Hz). Stimulation of the outer molecular layer revealed hyperexcitability of granule cells in slices from epileptic rats. The maximum number of action potentials discharged per stimulus was higher in epileptic rats (1.4 ± 0.1;n = 84 cells) compared with controls (0.7 ± 0.1;n = 74 cells; p < 0.0001; unpairedt test) (Fig.3a–c). At stimulation intensities below spike threshold, prolonged depolarizations were evident in epileptic (Fig. 3b2) but not control rats. Resting membrane potential and input resistance were similar in epileptic and control groups (Table1) (Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999;Williamson et al., 1999). Granule cells from control or epileptic rats responded to current step injection with a train of action potentials and never with a burst (Fig. 3d). These findings suggest that changes in synaptic transmission, rather than changes in intrinsic firing properties, underlie the hyperexcitability of granule cells in epileptic rats. Granule cell hyperexcitability to orthodromic stimulation has been found in tissue from patients (Isokawa and Levesque, 1991; Franck et al., 1995; Williamson et al., 1995; Isokawa, 1996; Masukawa et al., 1996) and models of temporal lobe epilepsy (Sloviter, 1991; Simmons et al., 1997; Bragin et al., 1999; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999; Patrylo et al., 1999; Lynch and Sutula, 2000).
Changes in glutamate receptors (Mody and Heinemann, 1987; Mathern et al., 1998) and excitatory circuitry might enhance excitatory synaptic input to granule cells and make them hyperexcitable. We found evidence of changes in excitatory circuitry that included abnormal morphological characteristics reported previously for granule cells in epileptic tissue (Fig. 4) (Represa et al., 1993; Franck et al., 1995; Okazaki et al., 1995; Spigelman et al., 1998; Sutula et al., 1998; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999; Ribak et al., 2000). At least one axon collateral from a biocytin-labeled granule cell projected into the molecular layer in 91% of the 34 slices from epileptic rats (Fig. 4c) and in only 19% of the 27 slices from controls (p < 0.005; χ2 test). Mild mossy fiber invasion of the inner molecular layer occurs in the temporal pole of control rats (Cavazos et al., 1992; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997).
Basal dendrites were evident in 31% of the 108 labeled granule cells in epileptic rats (Fig. 4b) and in only 5% of the 58 cells in controls (p < 0.005; χ2 test). We measured the distance from the closest edge of the soma of biocytin-labeled granule cells to the hilar/granule cell layer border. Granule cells with basal dendrites were closer to the border (13 ± 3 μm; range, 0–80 μm) than granule cells without basal dendrites (29 ± 2 μm; range, 0–88 μm; p < 0.002; unpaired t test). Previous studies found fewer granule cells with basal dendrites in epileptic rats and virtually no granule cells with basal dendrites in control rats (Spigelman et al., 1998; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999; Ribak et al., 2000). Possible causes for the different results include the septotemporal level examined and how status epilepticus was initiated and terminated in different experimental models. Regardless of these differences, the presence of axon projections into the molecular layer and the presence of basal dendrites might contribute to granule cell hyperexcitability through novel recurrent excitatory circuits in epileptic rats.
Reduced monosynaptic IPSPs in epileptic rats
In addition to recurrent excitation through sprouted mossy fibers and granule cell basal dendrites, the loss of inhibitory interneurons may contribute to granule cell hyperexcitability. IPSPs were recorded with sharp intracellular electrodes and current clamp while DC current injection was used to collect a family of evoked responses at different baseline membrane potentials. The outer molecular layer was stimulated because it is a region where the axons of a vulnerable population of GABAergic interneurons concentrate and synapse with granule cell dendrites (Leranth et al., 1990; Katona et al., 1999;Buckmaster et al., 2002a). In normal ACSF, this stimulation method is likely to evoke a mixture of monosynaptic and polysynaptic responses through feedforward and feedback circuits. Synaptic conductances were calculated at latencies of 20 and 150 msec, which are near the peaks of the early GABAA receptor-mediated and late GABAB receptor-mediated IPSPs, respectively (Fig.3e,f). IPSP conductances were slightly higher than those recorded in the septal dentate gyrus in vivo with angular bundle stimulation (Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999), and they were similar to those reported for human granule cells in slice experiments (Williamson et al., 1999). In normal ACSF the average conductances at 20 and 150 msec latencies in epileptic rats were lower than those of controls (74 and 77% of controls, respectively), but the differences were not significant (unpaired t test) (Table1). The reversal potential at 20 msec was significantly more depolarized in epileptic rats (Fig. 3f), probably because the early IPSP was more contaminated by prolonged EPSPs.
Hyperexcitability of principal neurons amplifies excitatory synaptic input to interneurons, thereby enhancing feedback inhibition (Chagnac-Amitai and Connors, 1989b). Therefore, in normal ACSF there is a potential for polysynaptic amplification of IPSPs in tissue from epileptic rats, because their granule cells discharge more action potentials and may excite surviving postsynaptic interneurons more than in controls. To block polysynaptic amplification, we applied CNQX/d-APV and recorded monosynaptic IPSPs (Figs.3g,h). Monosynaptic IPSPs in control and epileptic rats had similar reversal potentials (Table 1). The reversal potential of the early IPSP in control and epileptic rats was more negative in CNQX/d-APV than in normal ACSF, probably because contaminating EPSPs had been blocked. In epileptic rats the mean conductances of monosynaptic IPSPs at 20 and 150 msec latencies were reduced to 23 and 32% of control values, respectively (Table 1).
Increased paired-pulse facilitation of IPSCs in epileptic rats
Previous studies have used paired-pulse stimulation to evaluate inhibition of the dentate gyrus in epileptic and control tissue (Tuff et al., 1983; Uruno et al., 1995; Buhl et al., 1996; Haas et al., 1996;Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997; Wilson et al., 1998). To test paired-pulse depression of GABAA receptor-mediated IPSCs we stimulated the outer molecular layer with bipolar wire electrodes (15 μm diameter) under application of CNQX and d-APV (Fig.5a). Low-intensity stimuli elicited no response, but gradual increases (to 26–65 μA, 80 μsec, 0.1 Hz) evoked IPSCs in an all-or-none manner. Stimulation intensity was set at 1.5× the threshold for minimal responses. IPSCs were completely blocked by application of bicuculline methiodide (data not shown), indicating that they were mediated by GABAA receptors. In the control group, paired-pulse depression of monosynaptic IPSCs was observed, especially at shorter interstimulus intervals. In epileptic animals, instead of paired-pulse depression, IPSCs displayed paired-pulse facilitation when stimuli were delivered at intervals ≥100 msec (Fig.5b).
Fewer IPSCs in epileptic and 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats
As described above, our evoked response analyses of granule cell inhibition yielded mixed results. Granule cells in epileptic rats had smaller conductance monosynaptic IPSPs, but their monosynaptic IPSCs were less depressed during paired-pulse stimulation. Intense electrical stimulation of the molecular layer is non-physiological, and evoked responses may reflect the activation of both synaptic and extrasynaptic receptors. Therefore, we evaluated sIPSCs, which reflect a more physiological form of GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition. In normal ACSF the frequency and charge transfer of sIPSCs in epileptic rats was reduced (51 and 72% of controls) (Fig.6a,c). These findings suggest that in epileptic rats, granule cells receive less spontaneous inhibitory synaptic input than in controls. The reduced frequency of sIPSCs could be caused by reduced spontaneous activity of presynaptic interneurons or fewer inhibitory synapses on granule cells, or both. If there were fewer inhibitory synapses on granule cells, the frequency of mIPSCs should be reduced. Therefore, we evaluated mIPSCs under application of CNQX, d-APV, and tetrodotoxin. Bicuculline methiodide completely blocked mIPSCs (data not shown). In epileptic rats the frequency of mIPSCs was reduced to 47% of controls (Fig.7a,c).
To determine whether inhibitory synaptic input to granule cells decreased before or after the onset of epilepsy, we recorded sIPSCs and mIPSCs in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats. Spontaneous IPSCs recorded in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats were less frequent and transferred less charge (30 and 42% of age-matched controls, respectively) (Fig. 6b,c). Shortly after status epilepticus, excitatory synaptic drive onto dentate interneurons is reduced (Doherty and Dingledine, 2001). That might explain, in part, why sIPSC frequency was lowest in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats compared with all other groups. Similarly, mIPSC frequency was reduced to 36% of controls (Figs. 7b,c). These results suggest that in pre-epileptic rats, before the onset of epilepsy, granule cells receive less inhibitory synaptic input.
The average amplitude of sIPSCs in epileptic rats was 143% of controls (Fig. 6c). This could be attributable to larger amplitude IPSCs per synapse or to more synchronous synaptic release. The average amplitude of mIPSCs in epileptic rats was 137% of controls (Fig. 7c), suggesting increased IPSC amplitude per synapse. In granule cells from kindled animals, mIPSC amplitude is increased, and more GABAA receptors are expressed per synapse (Otis et al., 1994; Nusser et al., 1998).
The average 10–90% rise time of sIPSCs in granule cells from epileptic rats was similar to age-matched controls, although the average 10–90% rise time in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus was smaller than that of controls (Fig. 6c). Rise times of mIPSCs were similar in all groups (Fig. 7c). These findings suggest that interneurons generating slow-rising IPSCs might be transiently and selectively inactive after status epilepticus.
As mentioned above, the average frequency of mIPSCs in epileptic and 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats decreased to <50% of controls. The average frequency of mIPSCs in chronically epileptic rats (4.5 ± 0.4 Hz) was similar to that in 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats (3.9 ± 0.4 Hz; p > 0.1; unpaired ttest) (Fig. 7c). These results suggest that granule cells lose more than half of their inhibitory synapses within 3 d of pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus, and this loss is maintained into the stage of chronic epilepsy.
Which inhibitory inputs to granule cells are lost?
Granule cells receive inhibitory synaptic input at proximal/somatic and distal/dendritic sites (Halasy and Somogyi, 1993). When measured at the soma, synaptic events generated in the distal dendrites of granule cells are slower rising and smaller in amplitude than those generated more proximally (Soltesz et al., 1995). To verify this we evoked IPSCs by minimal stimulation under application of CNQX and d-APV in control granule cells. Stimulating electrodes were positioned in the outer molecular layer and granule cell layer (Fig. 8a). Minimal stimulation (30 ± 1 μA in the granule cell layer; 51 ± 2 μA in the molecular layer; p < 0.001; paired t test) failed to evoke an IPSC 38 ± 6% of the time in the granule cell layer and 39 ± 6% of the time in the molecular layer (n = 13). Minimal stimulation of the granule cell layer evoked large-amplitude (91.8 ± 15.0 pA) fast-rising (1.2 ± 0.2 msec; 10–90% rise time) IPSCs with stepwise variations in amplitude. In the same granule cells, minimal stimulation of the outer molecular layer evoked smaller-amplitude (28.3 ± 4.7 pA;p < 0.001; t test) slower-rising (3.7 ± 0.3 msec; p < 0.001; paired t test) IPSCs.
To confirm that our minimal stimulation protocol evoked unitary synaptic responses, we obtained recordings from basket cells and synaptically coupled granule cells in control rats (n = 5). Putative basket cells were identified by their large pyramidal-shaped soma positioned between the granule cell layer and hilus (Kosaka et al., 1987), short-duration action potentials (data not shown), and fast spiking without adaptation in response to sustained current injection (Kraushaar and Jonas, 2000) (Fig. 8c). Action potentials in presynaptic basket cells evoked unitary IPSCs in postsynaptic granule cells (Fig. 8b) with similar amplitudes (91.1 ± 24.5 pA) and rise times (1.1 ± 0.2 msec) as IPSCs evoked by minimal stimulation in the granule cell layer. The average rise time was longer than that reported by Kraushaar and Jonas (2000), but they measured 20–80% rise time in chloride-loaded cells at a higher temperature. Our findings confirm that spontaneous and miniature IPSCs generated at proximal/somatic sites are likely to have fast rise times and large amplitudes, whereas those generated at distal/dendritic sites will tend to have slower rise times and smaller amplitudes. Similarly, CA1 pyramidal neurons have fast- and slow-rising GABAA receptor-mediated IPSCs (Wierenga and Wadman, 1999) that are generated at somatic and dendritic sites, respectively (Banks et al., 1998).
To determine whether there was a selective loss of fast- or slow-rising GABAA receptor-mediated events in epileptic rats, we constructed histograms of the 10–90% rise-times of mIPSCs in individual granule cells. Those histograms were well fit by two Gaussian distributions (Fig. 8e), suggesting that mIPSCs can be divided into two groups. The average 10–90% rise times were 0.88 msec for the fast-rising and 2.09 msec for the slow-rising group. IPSC rise time depends on GABAA receptor subunit composition in the postsynaptic membrane and site of origin on the recorded cell. Although we did not obtain direct recordings from granule cell dendrites, the similarity of rise times of mIPSCs and IPSCs evoked by minimal stimulation of the outer molecular layer suggests that slow-rising mIPSCs are generated in the dendrites. Evoked responses might be slightly slower because stimulating electrodes were placed in the outermost edge of the molecular layer, whereas mIPSCs may arise all along the dendritic arbor. Fast- and slow-rising events accounted for an average of 30 and 70% of the mIPSCs, respectively (Table 2), which is similar to the proportion of somatic versus dendritic GABAergic synapses on granule cells (25 and 75%, respectively) (Halasy and Somogyi, 1993).
As mentioned previously, the average amplitudes of granule cell mIPSCs were larger in epileptic and 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats compared with controls. After categorizing mIPSCs as fast or slow rising by fitting histograms with two Gaussian curves, it was possible to determine whether the amplitudes of fast-rising mIPSCs, slow-rising mIPSCs, or both were larger in granule cells recorded from epileptic and 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats compared with controls. For this analysis, mIPSCs were subdivided into two extreme categories: the fastest fast-rising mIPSCs and the slowest slow-rising mIPSCs. The fastest fast-rising events from each cell were mIPSCs with 10–90% rise times that were less than the median minus 1 SD in the Gaussian curve that fit the distribution of fast-rising events. The slowest slow-rising events from each cell were mIPSCs with 10–90% rise times that were more than the median plus 1 SD in the Gaussian curve that fit the distribution of slow-rising events. In all experimental groups, the average amplitudes of the fastest fast-rising mIPSCs were larger than the slowest slow-rising mIPSCs (Table 2). Compared with controls, the amplitudes of both the fastest fast-rising and the slowest slow-rising mIPSCs were larger in epileptic and 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats. The amplitudes of the fastest fast-rising mIPSCs increased more (160–182% of control values) than the amplitudes of the slowest slow-rising events (113–123% of control values). These findings suggest that GABAergic synapses that are located at or near the granule cell body display more plasticity than those located on the distal dendrites.
There were no significant differences in the average rise times or proportion of fast- versus slow-rising events between epileptic, 3–7 d post-status epilepticus, and control rats (Table 2). Histograms of the average mIPSC frequency versus rise time reveal fewer fast- and slow-rising events in epileptic and 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats compared with their controls (Fig.9a). Similarly, histograms of the average mIPSC frequency versus amplitude reveal fewer large- and small-amplitude events in epileptic and 3–7 d post-status epilepticus rats compared with their respective controls (Fig. 9b). Therefore, our data suggest that in epileptic rats there is a loss of both proximal/somatic and distal/dendritic GABAergic synapses to granule cells. The mIPSC rise time and amplitude data are consistent with the anatomical results that show a loss of both proximally synapsing parvalbumin-immunoreactive interneurons and distally synapsing somatostatin-immunoreactive interneurons (Figs.1e–h, 2a). This is somewhat different from the situation in the CA1 region where spontaneous inhibition is reduced at the dendritic level but increased at the soma of pyramidal cells (Cossart et al., 2001).
Discussion
The principal finding of this study is the reduction of granule cell inhibition in a model of temporal lobe epilepsy. The use of multiple techniques to evaluate granule cell inhibition and the comparison of electrophysiological and anatomical data at different stages of epileptogenesis provided new clues about the role of granule cell inhibition in temporal lobe epilepsy.
Reduced granule cell inhibition in epileptic rats
Our most direct measures of granule cell inhibition (mIPSC and sIPSC frequencies and evoked monosynaptic IPSP conductance) detected reductions to <50% of controls. This is a substantial loss, considering that relatively small reductions of GABAA receptor function to 80–90% of control values permit the spread of epileptiform activity in neocortex (Chagnac-Amitai and Connors, 1989a). It is likely that the loss of granule cell inhibition is attributable in large part to the loss of inhibitory interneurons. However, reduced mIPSC frequency also could be caused by lower probability of transmitter release (Hirsch et al., 1999). Our findings of reduced inhibitory synaptic input to granule cells in epileptic rats are consistent with some previous studies of tissue from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Williamson et al. (1995, 1999) found reduced evoked IPSPs in granule cells of epileptic patients. These findings support the hypothesis that interneuron loss in the dentate gyrus reduces granule cell inhibition and contributes to temporal lobe epilepsy (de Lanerolle et al., 1989).
If the reduction in granule cell inhibition is so substantial, why have many previous studies reported strong inhibition in the dentate gyrus of epileptic patients and models (Tuff et al., 1983; Isokawa-Akesson et al., 1989; Franck et al., 1995; Uruno et al., 1995; Colder et al., 1996; Haas et al., 1996; Isokawa, 1996; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997,1999; Wilson et al., 1998)? Possible explanations include the septotemporal level examined, the methods used to evaluate granule cell inhibition, and compensatory mechanisms. One's ability to detect reductions in granule cell inhibition might be affected by the septotemporal level of the dentate gyrus. Previous in vivo studies in rats examined the hippocampus at its more accessible septal end. In the present study we used slices from the temporal dentate gyrus, because tissue resected to treat patients with temporal lobe epilepsy includes the temporal (or anterior) dentate gyrus and because it is the region where hilar neuron loss is most severe (Babb et al., 1984) and where the number of GAD-positive (Buckmaster and Jongen-Rêlo, 1999) and somatostatin- and parvalbumin-immunoreactive interneurons are most reduced (Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997). Therefore, studies of the septal dentate gyrus might miss the more severe interneuron loss and reduced granule cell inhibition in the temporal dentate gyrus.
Potential compensatory mechanisms
Several mechanisms appear to compensate, albeit incompletely, for reduced inhibitory synaptic input to granule cells. Without these compensatory mechanisms, granule cell hyperexcitability might be worse. First, in epileptic rats granule cells discharge more action potentials when stimulated. Therefore, surviving interneurons are likely to receive more excitatory synaptic drive, and that could amplify feedback inhibition of granule cells. Studies that evaluate evoked IPSPs in normal ACSF might overestimate the inhibitory synaptic input to granule cells in epileptic tissue. Second, mIPSC amplitude increases, suggesting that more GABAA receptors were expressed per synapse (Otis et al., 1994; Nusser et al., 1998). However, in the present study the increased amplitude of mIPSCs in epileptic rats was offset by the large decrease in mIPSC and sIPSC frequencies, such that the average sIPSC charge transfer was lower than that of controls. Third, paired-pulse depression of IPSCs decreases in granule cells in epileptic rats (Buhl et al., 1996; Haas et al., 1996), consistent with increased paired-pulse inhibition of granule cell-generated field potentials (Tuff et al., 1983; Uruno et al., 1995;Haas et al., 1996; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997; Wilson et al., 1998). Results from paired-pulse analyses, therefore, suggest that granule cell inhibition would be more persistent during repetitive stimulation in epileptic tissue. Our findings confirm that granule cells from epileptic rats have reduced paired-pulse depression of monosynaptic IPSCs, which could contribute to increased paired-pulse inhibition of granule cell discharge. However, that is just one of many factors affecting granule cell inhibition. We found that the less direct measures of granule cell inhibition (conductances of IPSPs evoked in normal ACSF and paired-pulse depression) failed to reveal the loss detected by analysis of sIPSCs, mIPSCs, and evoked monosynaptic IPSPs.
Our data do not support another potential compensatory mechanism. Previous studies described increased staining of inhibitory interneuron fibers in the dentate gyrus molecular layer of patients (Babb et al., 1989; Mathern et al., 1995a) and models of temporal lobe epilepsy (Davenport et al., 1990; Wanscher et al., 1990; Schwarzer et al., 1995;Buckmaster and Dudek, 1997; Mathern et al., 1997; Esclapez and Houser, 1999; Andre et al., 2001). These findings suggested that surviving interneurons might sprout axon collaterals to restore some of the lost inhibitory synaptic input to granule cells. On the other hand, GABAergic axon sprouting might be epileptogenic by producing hypersynchrony (Babb et al., 1989). We, too, found increased staining of somatostatin-immunoreactive fibers in the molecular layer of epileptic rats compared with controls. However, functional evidence for additional, compensatory, GABAergic synapses was lacking. The frequency of mIPSCs in granule cells did not increase significantly from 3 to 7 d after status epilepticus into the stage of chronic epilepsy. It seems unlikely that new GABAergic synapses at distal dendritic sites went undetected, because we detected IPSCs evoked with minimal stimulation of the outer molecular layer. We cannot exclude the possibility that functional evidence for additional GABAergic synapses would have eventually developed after longer periods of chronic epilepsy, but at the stage examined, glutamatergic axon sprouting and synaptogenesis by granule cells are well established (Mello et al., 1993; Mathern et al., 1998; Okazaki et al., 1999; Wuarin and Dudek, 2001), so one might expect that GABAergic synaptogenesis would have occurred. Perhaps surviving interneurons only appear to sprout axon collaterals, because seizure activity increases antigen expression (Feldblum et al., 1990; Wanscher et al., 1990; Shinoda et al., 1991;Schwarzer et al., 1995; Houser and Esclapez, 1996; Esclapez and Houser, 1999), making preexisting axon collaterals more visible by immunocytochemical methods. Stereological analysis of inhibitory synapse numbers at different stages of epileptogenesis would help resolve this issue.
Previous studies of granule cell mIPSCs
Some studies have used direct measures of granule cell inhibition in the temporal dentate gyrus and have found results different from ours. Previous studies found a similar frequency of mIPSCs in granule cells of control (Molnár and Nadler, 2000) and pilocarpine-induced epileptic rats (Molnár and Nadler, 2001). In contrast, we found an average decrease to <50% of controls. And, previous studies of granule cells found many fast-rising GABAA-mediated mIPSCs but few slow-rising mIPSCs (Soltesz et al., 1995). In contrast, we found that an average of 70% of the GABAA-mediated mIPSCs fit within the slow-rising category. The causes of these differences are unclear, but possibilities include differences in rat strains and experimental models (for example, whether and how status epilepticus was stopped and the consequent effects on excitotoxic damage) and differences in slice preparation, recording, and data analysis techniques. Filtering associated with low-quality recording conditions could produce an artificially high proportion of slow-rising events. This is an unlikely explanation for our data because (1) the series resistances of our recordings were similar to those of previous mIPSC recordings from granule cells (Soltesz et al., 1995), (2) series resistance was 80% compensated in our recordings, and (3) all of our recordings included both fast- and slow-rising events from the same cell (Fig. 8). If recording conditions had been poor, then all events should have been filtered.
Functional implications of reduced inhibition
Our findings suggest that granule cells receive less inhibitory synaptic input in epileptic rats than in controls. Frequencies of sIPSCs and mIPSCs also were reduced in pre-epileptic rats days to weeks before the onset of recurrent, spontaneous seizures. Therefore, reduced granule cell inhibition alone is insufficient to cause epilepsy, because it was present when rats were not experiencing spontaneous seizures. This finding suggests that epileptogenesis requires other changes. Those other changes might include alterations in the excitatory synaptic input to granule cells. Granule cells sprout axon collaterals into the molecular layer (Represa et al., 1993; Franck et al., 1995; Okazaki et al., 1995; Sutula et al., 1998; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999; Buckmaster et al., 2002b) and extend novel basal dendrites into the hilus (Spigelman et al., 1998; Buckmaster and Dudek, 1999;Ribak et al., 2000). Both developments might take weeks to establish, and both could contribute to the abnormal recurrent excitation of granule cells found in epileptic rats (Molnár and Nadler, 1999;Lynch and Sutula, 2000; Wuarin and Dudek, 2001). Consistent with enhanced recurrent excitation, orthodromically evoked responses of granule cells in epileptic rats displayed prolonged depolarizations and more action potentials compared with controls. Recurrent excitation can be controlled when inhibition is sufficiently strong (Miles and Wong, 1987; Cronin et al., 1992). The findings of the present study suggest that in epileptic rats the inhibitory synaptic input to granule cells is reduced to a level that sometimes fails to control recurrent excitation and seizure activity. Reduced inhibition and inadequate control of recurrent excitation might be common mechanisms that contribute to epileptogenesis in other regions of the hippocampal formation (Morin et al., 1998; Esclapez et al., 1999) and in neocortex (Li and Prince, 2002).
Footnotes
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS39110 and NS40276). M.K. is the recipient of a fellowship from the Uehara Memorial Foundation. P.S.B. is the recipient of a Burroughs Wellcome Fund Career Award. We thank Dr. J. Huguenard for software and advice on IPSC data analysis, Dr. D. Prince for suggestions on this manuscript, Drs. A. Tobin and N. Tillakaratne for GAD cDNAs, and Dr. X. Wen for assistance with in situ hybridization.
Correspondence should be addressed to Paul Buckmaster, Department of Comparative Medicine, Stanford University School of Medicine, 300 Pasteur Drive, R321 Edwards Building, Stanford, CA 94305-5342. E-mail:psb{at}stanford.edu.
M. Kobayashi's present address: Department of Oral Physiology, Osaka University Graduate School of Dentistry, Yamadaoka 1-8, Suita, Osaka 565-0871, Japan.