Abstract
Dopaminergic neurotransmission has been highly implicated in the reinforcing properties of many substances of abuse, including marijuana. Cannabinoids activate ventral tegmental area dopaminergic neurons, the main ascending projections of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system, and change their spiking pattern by increasing the number of impulses in a burst and elevating the frequency of bursts. Although they also increase time-averaged striatal dopamine levels for extended periods of time, little is known about the temporal structure of this change. To elucidate this, fast-scan cyclic voltammetry was used to monitor extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats with subsecond timescale resolution. Intravenous administration of the central cannabinoid (CB1) receptor agonist, R(+)-[2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-3-[(morpholinyl)methyl]pyrrolo[1,2,3-de]-1,4-benzoxazin-6-yl]-(1-naphthalenyl) methanone mesylate, dose-dependently produced catalepsy, decreased locomotion, and reduced the amplitude of electrically evoked dopamine release while markedly increasing the frequency of detected (nonstimulated) dopamine concentration transients. The CB1 receptor antagonist [N-piperidino-5-(4-chlorophenyl)-1-(2,4-dichlorophenyl)-4-methylpyrazole-3-carboxamide] reversed and prevented all agonist-induced effects but did not show effects on dopamine release when injected alone. These data demonstrate that doses of a cannabinoid agonist known to increase burst firing produce ongoing fluctuations in extracellular dopamine on a previously unrecognized temporal scale in the nucleus accumbens.
Introduction
Midbrain dopaminergic neurons normally fire at low frequencies of 5–10 Hz in freely moving rats, with periodic short bursts of high-frequency activity (∼20 Hz) that become more prevalent in response to salient stimuli (Hyland et al., 2002). Indeed, burst firing of dopamine (DA) neurons has been shown to be of functional significance in the context of sensory processing, specifically with regard to reward (Schultz, 1998). Increases in the burst rate of dopaminergic neurons produce greater peak extracellular dopamine concentrations at axon terminals (Gonon, 1988; Wightman et al., 1988; Overton and Clark, 1997). Recently, rapid surges in extracellular dopamine concentration or “transients” have been observed with fast-scan cyclic voltammetry at carbonfiber electrodes implanted in various terminal fields (Robinson et al., 2002) that would be consistent with bursting activity of dopaminergic neurons. The transients only last for a few seconds and can be time-locked to specific reward-related behaviors such as cocaine self-administration (Phillips et al., 2003) and operant responding for natural rewards (Roitman et al., 2004) during which they appear to promote reward-seeking.
Drugs of abuse share the ability to increase dopamine neurotransmission in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) (Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988; Nestler, 2002; Wise, 2002). Some abused drugs such as amphetamine and cocaine have direct actions on dopaminergic neurons, whereas others such as the main psychoactive component of marijuana, Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC) (Mechoulam and Gaoni, 1967), exert their actions indirectly. Indeed, Δ9-THC exerts its reinforcing effects primarily via an action on central cannabinoid (CB1) receptors, the endogenous ligands of which are arachidonic acid derivatives such as anandamide and 2-arachidonyl-glycerol (Freund et al., 2003). This endogenous system interacts with many neurotransmitters in the brain, including GABA, glutamate, and acetylcholine; however, cannabinoid receptors are not found on dopaminergic neurons (Hernàndez et al., 2000). Nevertheless, extracellular dopamine in the NAc is elevated by Δ9-THC or synthetic CB1 agonists as shown by microdialysis studies (Chen et al., 1991; Tanda et al., 1997; Tanda and Goldberg, 2003).
A possible mechanism explaining this increase in NAc extracellular dopamine would be an indirect excitatory action of cannabinoids on ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopaminergic neurons, the main ascending dopaminergic projection to the NAc. Indeed, in the VTA-containing slice, CB1 agonists enhance the firing rate of dopaminergic neurons in a nondesensitizing manner (Cheer et al., 2000a) via a reduction of afferent GABAergic transmission (Szabo et al., 2002). This finding is consistent with the anatomical distribution of CB1 receptors and has added the VTA to a number of brain regions in which cannabinoids are known to exert their effects presynaptically (Schlicker and Kathmann, 2001). Cannabinoids given at doses that elevate dopamine levels in the NAc also excite dopaminergic neurons in the VTA through CB1 receptors in vivo (French et al., 1997; Gessa et al., 1998; Cheer at al., 2003). Furthermore, VTA dopamine neurons in anesthetized or paralyzed rats exhibit an elevated number of action potentials within bursts after cannabinoid application (French, 1997; Diana et al., 1998; Wu and French, 2000).
In this work we used fast-scan cyclic voltammetry in freely moving rats to measure the effects of systemic administration of a cannabinoid agonist at doses reported to increase phasic firing of VTA dopamine neurons to determine whether time-resolved dopamine release events that are functionally relevant can be observed after cannabinoid treatment. The effect of the agonist on extracellular dopamine was manifested as an increase in the frequency and amplitude of periodic dopamine transients, suggesting that changes in dopamine release dynamics follow a cannabinoid-induced enhancement of burst (phasic) activity in dopaminergic neurons.
Materials and Methods
Animals and surgery. Male Sprague Dawley rats (n = 21) implanted with a jugular vein catheter (250–350 gm; Charles River, Wilmington, MA) were housed individually in clear plastic cages with ad libitum access to food and water on an inverted 12 hr light/dark cycle. After 1 week, animals were anesthetized with xylazine hydrochloride (10 mg/kg, i.p.) and ketamine hydrochloride (100 mg/kg, i.p.) for implantation of electrodes for voltammetry as described previously (Robinson et al., 2002). Briefly, a guide cannula (BAS, West Lafayette, IN) aimed at the NAc core (1.3 mm anterior and 1.3 mm lateral relative to bregma) was permanently affixed to the skull, and a bipolar stimulating electrode (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) was lowered to the medial forebrain bundle (MFB) (4.2 mm posterior, 1.4 mm lateral, and 8.6 mm ventral from the brain surface). A Ag/AgCl reference electrode was positioned in the contralateral hemisphere. Once stimulated dopamine release was voltammetrically detected in the ipsilateral NAc core with a carbon-fiber electrode, the stimulating electrode and guide cannula were permanently affixed to the skull with the aid of machine screws and cranioplastic cement. Rats were allowed to regain their presurgical weight (at least 48 hr postoperative) before freely moving experiments were initiated. All animal protocols and care were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
Fast-scan cyclic voltammetry. Cylindrical carbon-fiber microelectrodes with an exposed tip of 50–100 μm were prepared by pulling capillary glass (A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA) around carbon fibers (T650, 6 μm diameter; Amoco Corporation, Greenville, SC). A fresh electrode was lowered each day to the core of the NAc (6–8 mm, ventral) with the aid of a microdrive that locked to the implanted guide cannula. A head-stage cable was connected to the electrode assembly, and animals were tethered to a rotating commutator situated in the top of the experimental chamber, which allowed for virtually unrestrained movement. The carbonfiber electrode was held at –0.4 V versus Ag/AgCl between scans and then driven to +1.3 V and back in a triangular manner at 400 V/sec every 100 msec. This waveform is similar to that documented previously that produces enhanced sensitivity for dopamine (Heien et al., 2003). The waveform was generated and current was monitored with an interface board (National Instruments, Austin, TX) in a Pentium IV computer running custom-designed LABVIEW (National Instruments) software. Medial forebrain stimulations consisted of 24 biphasic pulses (2 msec, 125 μA per phase) delivered at 60 Hz/min to elicit dopamine release.
Behavioral testing. Spontaneous locomotor activity was monitored using a DigiScan Animal Activity Monitor (AccuScan Instruments, Columbus, OH). A set of five infrared (IR) beams 3 cm above the floor of the box were used to determine locomotor activity. Ten readings of horizontal activity were obtained every minute for 10 min. Because animals were tested multiple times in 1 d and cannabinoids are known to suppress locomotion, each rat was exposed for 10 min to the locomotor activity box before testing to avoid a habituation confound. Catalepsy was evaluated with an 8 mm diameter metal bar suspended 10 cm above the floor of the cage by two wooden blocks 20 cm apart. Both forepaws were placed on the bar, and the time the animal kept them on the bar before both limbs returned to the ground was recorded (Wu et al., 2000). The average time from three identical trials in each rat was recorded. Trials were terminated after 180 sec.
Data analysis. Cyclic voltammograms (used for identification of detected species) were background subtracted from 10 cyclic voltammograms obtained before the stimulation to remove the capacitive and charging currents. To examine all of the detected voltammetric events during a recording period, two-dimensional plots were used with time as the abscissa, triangular wave voltage as the ordinate, and the background subtracted current encoded in false color. The temporal profile of voltammetrically detected dopamine changes was obtained by plotting the current at the peak oxidation of dopamine (+600 mV vs Ag/AgCl) as a function of time. To remove contributions from ionic changes that frequently accompany stimulated dopamine release (Venton et al., 2003), currents measured at a potential at which only ionic changes occurred were adjusted in amplitude and subtracted from current at the peak potential for dopamine oxidation (Phillips and Wightman, 2003). Similar data treatment was used for dopamine calibration (1 μm) experiments in a flow injection system used after freely moving experiments.
Cyclic voltammograms were collected into 1 min data files. An MFB stimulation to evoke dopamine release occurred at 30 sec within the collection interval. A background-subtracted cyclic voltammogram of the dopamine detected at the end of the stimulation in each animal was used as a template for identification of dopamine during other times in the recording interval. Typical noise levels were equivalent to 13 nm dopamine; only signals larger than this were considered for analysis. The entire file was evaluated for dopamine transients by taking a moving average of 10 scans and subtracting it from a cyclic voltammogram recorded 1 sec later. The resultant cyclic voltammogram was compared with the template voltammogram, and a correlation coefficient was obtained. The event was termed dopaminergic only if the correlation coefficient was ≥0.75. To obtain such a correlation coefficient, the cyclic voltammogram needed to have peaks that occurred at approximately +600 and –200 mV versus Ag/AgCl (oxidation and reduction potentials characteristic of dopamine). In addition, dopamine transients were required to meet these criteria for at least two consecutive scans.
Graphs were constructed from the data using Graph Pad Prism (Graph Pad Software, San Diego, CA). The amplitudes of stimulated dopamine release in files surrounding drug injections were analyzed with respect to pre-drug values with conventional ANOVA and Scheffe post hoc tests; the criterion of significance was set at p < 0.05. Drug-induced effects on spontaneous dopamine transients were determined by averaging the number of dopamine transients in 1 min files before and after the administration of drugs. Statistical analyses used STATISTICA (StatSoft, Tulsa, OK). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM.
Drugs. R(+)-[2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-3-[(morpholinyl)methyl]pyrrolo [1,2,3-DE]-1,4-benzoxazin-6-yl]-(1-naphtmalenyl)methanone mesylate (WIN55,212–2) (WIN) (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA) and [N-piperidino-5-(4-chlorophenyl)-1-(2,4-dichlorophenyl)-4-methylpyrazole-3-carboxamide] (SR141716A) (SR) (Research Triangle Institute–National Institute on Drug Abuse, Raleigh, NC) were freshly suspended in a 1:1:18 ratio of ethanol, emulphor (Alkamuls EL-620, Rhodia, Cranbury, NJ), and heparinized saline (0.9%) for intravenous injection. Initially, rats (n = 9) received the following dosing protocol (separated by at least 15 min for all dosing regimens): vehicle (200 μl), WIN (125 and 250 μg/kg), SR (300 μg/kg), and WIN again (250 μg/kg). To test the involvement of the endogenous cannabinoid system in the regulation of fast dopamine release, rats (n = 4) were given vehicle (200 μl) followed by SR (300 μg/kg) and WIN (250 μg/kg). The last experiment evaluated the dose-dependence of WIN-induced effects; animals (n = 8) were given intravenous bolus doses of vehicle (200 μl) followed by WIN at doses of 25, 50, and 75 μg/kg, yielding a cumulative dose of 150 μg/kg. Time-matched controls received saline injections.
Results
Electrically evoked and spontaneous dopamine release in freely moving rats
Electrical stimulation of the MFB with parameters that rats will readily self-administer (Garris et al., 1999) evokes dopamine release in the NAc of a freely moving rat that can be monitored by fast-scan cyclic voltammetry (Garris et al., 1997) (Fig. 1A). A representative recording obtained after adjusting the position of the carbon-fiber electrode for optimal response to dopamine is shown in Figure 1A. Detection of dopamine during and after the stimulation (Fig. 1A, delivered during the red bar above the color plot) is indicated by its oxidation current encoded as a green circle centered at +600 mV, accompanied by its reverse current at –200 mV encoded by the dark blue circle (Michael et al., 1999). A cyclic voltammogram extracted from the data at the end of the stimulation (first vertical line) is identical to that for dopamine in vitro (Fig. 1B). Longer-lasting changes in current with a peak at +225 mV (dark blue) are seen after uptake of dopamine. A cyclic voltammogram extracted at this later time (indicated by the second vertical line) has the characteristics of an alkaline pH shift, characterized previously in anesthetized animals (Fig. 1B) (Venton et al., 2003a). The current at +600 mV (Fig. 1A, x) can be corrected for these ionic changes (Fig. 1A, y) to reveal the time course of dopamine release and uptake.
Another dopamine concentration transient is also apparent in this record at the time indicated by the asterisk in Figure 1A. It precedes the electrical stimulation and was not associated with any overt stimulus. The correlation coefficient obtained by comparison of its cyclic voltammogram (Fig. 1B) with that obtained during the electrical stimulation was 0.77, indicating that it is caused by dopamine. This transient had a maximal amplitude of 32 nm, considerably less than the electrically evoked response (521 nm) in this example. Spontaneous dopamine transients such as this were observed in the NAc in nine of nine rats, with an average frequency of 1.4 ± 0.4 min-1.
The cannabinoid agonist WIN55,212–2 enhances dopamine transients but depresses electrically evoked release
Immediately after systemic administration of the cannabinimimetic WIN given at a dose of 125 μg/kg, a short-lived increase in ambulation (4 sec) followed by the onset of a cataleptic state characterized by enhanced muscle tone and rigidity was observed (Sañudo-Peña et al., 2000). Voltammetric responses during the cataleptic phase are shown in Figure 2. The data are from the same animal and recording site as in Figure 1. The number of dopamine transients in the recording interval was enhanced simultaneously with the onset of changes in locomotion (five transients after WIN compared with one before drug for this particular example over the 30 sec shown). The bottom panel shows the cyclic voltammograms obtained after MFB electrical stimulation (solid line) and a cyclic voltammogram corresponding to the WIN-induced dopamine transient (correlation coefficient of 0.93) that occurred immediately before the stimulation (dashed line).
Time course of cannabinoid-induced changes in phasic dopamine neurotransmission
After injection of WIN [WIN first dose (WIN1): 125 μg/kg] and the onset of catalepsy, two events occurred: the number of confirmed dopamine transients increased and remained elevated for at least 3 min after injection, and the evoked release of dopamine was greatly reduced immediately after administration of WIN (WIN1: 125 μg/kg). Before drug administration, electrical stimulations (indicated by black lines) repeated at 1 min intervals evoked a similar maximal dopamine concentration (Fig. 3). In the time between the stimulated responses, noise and some dopamine transients (indicated by the asterisks) are seen. The maximal concentration of stimulated dopamine release was diminished immediately after drug administration, but electrically evoked release of dopamine exhibited a tendency to return to pre-drug values 2 min after injection (Fig. 3).
When examined across animals (n = 9), the maximal dopamine evoked by electrical stimulation was significantly decreased from 478 ± 92 nm (pre-drug) to 161 ± 54 nm (WIN1: 125 μg/kg) (F(10,425) = 3.59; p < 0.005). The maximal concentration of DA evoked by electrical stimulation is attributable to the balance between uptake and release (Wightman et al., 1988). Analysis of the disappearance of dopamine after stimulation revealed that the diminished response was not caused by an increase in the rate of uptake. Indeed, the rate of dopamine clearance was unchanged as shown in Figure 4, in which the uptake portions of the average dopamine responses obtained before and after drug administration (WIN1: 125 μg/kg; n = 9 for each condition) are overlaid. Statistical analysis of the slopes of dopamine clearance before and after WIN administration revealed no significant differences (F(60,1302) = 0.32; p = 0.95; interaction time × treatment). Therefore, we conclude that the decreased maximal dopamine evoked by electrical stimulation after WIN is caused by a decrease in the amount released.
When evaluated in multiple animals, the increase in number of dopamine transients after WIN administration was found to be significant. Overall, the frequency of pre-drug dopamine transients before and after WIN (WIN1: 125 μg/kg) was enhanced from 1.4 ± 0.4 (pre-drug) to 3.87 ± 0.6 min-1 (F(10,503) = 11.99; p < 0.0001). The spontaneous frequency in the absence of drug is greater than we reported previously, but in that work a lower sensitivity approach was used and some transients may have been missed (Robinson et al., 2002). The amplitude distribution of dopamine transients before and after WIN showed that the cannabinoid agonist increased the amplitude of the dopamine transients (Fig. 5). Pre-drug spontaneous transients had a mean amplitude of 51.1 ± 9.2 nm, whereas after WIN (WIN1: 125 μg/kg) they increased to 98.7 ± 22.6 nm (p < 0.01; Student's t test), causing a rightward shift in the distribution. This effect was not observed after injection of vehicle in which the mean amplitude of transients was 47.1 ± 12.2 nm (p = 0.23; Student's t test).
The actions of WIN55,212–2 are mediated through CB1 receptors
When given to rats rendered cataleptic by WIN [WIN second dose (WIN2): 250 μg/kg], SR (300 μg/kg), a CB1 receptor antagonist, reverted the catalepsy and animals regained their righting reflex and resumed exploratory behavior within 10 sec after administration. The antagonist also reversed both the WIN-induced increase in the rate of dopamine transients and the suppression of electrically evoked dopamine release. Administration of SR abolished high-amplitude transients, and mean transient amplitude in the presence of SR was shifted to 60.5 ± 13.1 nm, which was not significantly different from pre-drug (p = 0.09; Student's t test). A representative example from a different rat is shown in Figure 6, in which the example in the third panel was collected immediately after the injection of SR.
Pooled data from multiple animals administered a series of doses of WIN and SR confirm the significance of these changes. Although administration of vehicle did not change the amplitude of stimulated release ([DA]max) (Fig. 7A) or the number of dopamine transients (Fig. 7B), administration of WIN (WIN1: 125 μg/kg) significantly suppressed [DA]max and increased the number of transients. A subsequent dose (WIN2: 250 μg/kg) did not alter either parameter further. SR reversed both effects of WIN and prevented further changes by a subsequent injection of WIN (WIN2: 250 μg/kg) (Fig. 7A,B).
A separate group of rats (n = 4) was given SR after vehicle to test whether transient dopamine release was controlled tonically by endocannabinoids. The antagonist (300 μg/kg) prevented the appearance of catalepsy and blocked the enhancing effects of WIN (WIN2: 250 μg/kg) on the rate of dopamine transients, but had no effects when injected alone as shown in Figure 7C.
WIN55,212–2 dose-dependently modifies behavior and subsecond dopamine release
The dose-dependence of WIN on dopamine release and the onset of the well documented changes in movement that cannabinoids can induce was evaluated (n = 8) (Sañudo-Peña et al., 2000). The frequency of spontaneous dopamine transients increased in a dose-dependent manner (Fig. 8A) and reached statistical significance after the last intravenous infusion of WIN (75 μg/kg) of the dosing protocol (F(3,140) = 3.82; p < 0.05; cumulative dose of 150 μg/kg). Additionally, electrically stimulated dopamine release exhibited a trend to diminish, but this effect did not reach significance (F(3,152) = 1.71; p = 0.16) (Fig. 8B). To allow for drug clearance, the effects on locomotion were evaluated in the same rats 72 hr later. Rats were given the same dosing protocol at the same time of day. Locomotion was exquisitely sensitive to the treatment with WIN (black bars) because even the lowest dose (25 μg/kg) significantly lowered the number of IR beam breaks (134.4 ± 8.9) compared with vehicle (184.1 ± 11.6). Higher doses (50 and 75 μg/kg) further depressed locomotion (F(3,156) = 34.26; p < 0.001) (Fig. 8C). Catalepsy was also dose-dependently enhanced (Fig. 8D, black bars), and this effect reached significance compared with vehicle at the intermediate dose of 50 μg/kg and was further increased with the last dose of 75 μg/kg (F(3,44) = 9.25; p < 0.001). No significant effects on locomotion or catalepsy were observed when saline (Fig. 8C,D, white bars) was administered to drug-naive rats (n = 4) at similar intervals.
Discussion
This study addresses the role of cannabinoids on rapid dopamine neurotransmission. Although microdialysis studies have shown that cannabinoids increase dopamine over minutes (Chen et al., 1991; Tanda et al., 1997, 1999), the temporal structure of this change is not known. Here we confirm that the potent CB1 receptor agonist, WIN55,212–2, increases extracellular dopamine but reveal that this is manifested as an increase in the frequency and amplitude of rapid dopamine transients in the NAc. These effects are dose-dependent and reversed by the central cannabinoid receptor antagonist, SR141716A, indicating that they are receptor mediated.
The actions of WIN do not appear to involve altered dopamine uptake, as shown by unchanged clearance rates after stimulated release. Indeed, they are unlikely to be mediated in the NAc because cannabinoids applied directly to dopaminergic terminals in NAc brain slices do not affect evoked dopamine release (Szabo et al., 1999); however, cannabinoids do produce an increase in the overall firing rate of dopaminergic neurons in the VTA (French, 1997; French et al., 1997; Cheer et al., 2000a, 2003; Szabo et al., 2002). An augmentation in the burst-firing rate of dopaminergic neurons could increase the amplitude of transient dopamine release because, when neurons shift to a bursting pattern, the resulting action potentials cause greater extrasynaptic levels of transmitter compared with the same number of equally spaced action potentials at a lower frequency (Gonon and Buda, 1985; Gonon, 1988; Wightman et al., 1988). The failure of SR to alter the rate of dopamine transients concurs with electrophysiological findings that show no effect on the endogenous firing of VTA dopaminergic neurons (French, 1997; Cheer et al., 2003). These findings suggest that under basal physiological conditions, endogenous cannabinoid tone is not sufficient to modify neuronal activity in the VTA.
Cannabinoids are known to exert their effects presynaptically, via CB1 receptors located mainly on GABAergic and glutamatergic terminals (Schlicker and Kathmann, 2001; Wilson and Nicoll, 2002). Consistent with previous reports, WIN dose-dependently decreased spontaneous locomotion and produced marked cataleptic effects (Sañudo-Peña et al., 2000; Wiley and Martin, 2003). In this study, rats displayed vocalizations and aggressiveness toward handling when cataleptic (Sañudo-Peña et al., 2000). The dose–response experiment indicates that changes in locomotion precede the observed effects on dopamine release, which were detected when the dosing was sufficient to produce a loss of the righting reflex. Cataleptic doses of cannabinoids are also known to induce conditioned place aversion (Sañudo-Peña et al., 1997; Cheer et al., 2000b). In humans, catatonia, characterized by muscular rigidity accompanied by intense fear, is reported after withdrawal from benzodiazepines (Rosebush and Mazurek, 1995), which is associated with a reduction in GABA tone (Sañudo-Peña et al., 2000). These findings suggest that the cannabinoid-induced cataleptic state, accompanied by dysphoria, arises by diminishing GABAergic function. Presynaptic inhibition of GABA release is consistent with the current findings because dopaminergic neurons receive a strong GABAergic input in the VTA (Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999; Szabo et al., 2002). In addition, a recent report by Melis et al. (2004) shows that WIN also inhibits excitatory synaptic transmission within the VTA. This finding may explain why cannabinoid agonists can elicit increases as well as decreases (albeit in a smaller population) in the firing rate of extracellularly recorded VTA neurons (Cheer et al., 2003), indicating that the generation of dopamine transients may require reciprocal actions on VTA neurons.
Dopamine transients in the NAc have been detected previously with cyclic voltammetry at carbon-fiber electrodes. They occur spontaneously when an animal is in solitude in the absence of any overt stimuli and without any obvious behavioral response. They become more frequent in response to salient stimuli such as presentation of a conspecific (Robinson et al., 2002). They also occur in response to cues that are associated with cocaine taking (Phillips et al., 2003) or with natural rewards (Roitman et al., 2004), during which they appear to promote reward-seeking. Interestingly, the doses of WIN that elicit dopamine transients have independently been shown to alter cocaine self-administration (Fattore et al., 1999), suggesting that cannabinoids exert their effects on goal-directed behavior by modifying dopamine release on a temporal scale relevant to the coding of discreet behavioral events. The neural basis for these concentration transients has yet to be unraveled, although they have a striking similarity in time course to dopamine release evoked by rapid trains of stimulus pulses that mimic burst firing. Moreover, the dopamine transients arise with regularity similar to that of low bursting activity rates of dopaminergic neurons in freely moving rats (Hyland et al., 2002). Because transients rapidly diminish in concentration as they diffuse from their release site (Venton et al., 2003b), this rapid form of chemical communication would restrict the spatial region for interactions of dopamine with its receptors.
The increase in the frequency and amplitude of dopamine transients in this study was accompanied by a decrease in the concentration of electrically evoked dopamine, demonstrating that the amount of dopamine released per impulse was diminished. Electrically evoked dopamine release was similarly depressed in freely moving rats after doses of ethanol that cause elevated extracellular dopamine as measured by microdialysis (Budygin et al., 2001). There, we proposed that evoked dopamine release was reduced because of increased spontaneous release resulting from the documented increased neuronal activity of dopaminergic neurons caused by ethanol (Gessa et al., 1985). Because of the increased sensitivity of the voltammetric technique used in this work (Heien et al., 2003), we were able to directly measure an increase in spontaneous dopamine transients after cannabinoid administration that may underlie the depression of evoked dopamine. Indeed, the reduction in stimulated dopamine release seen after both of these drugs is consistent with suppression of the amount of dopamine released per impulse after periods of sustained activity (Montague et al., 2004). Assuming that the transients are a result of burst firing, an increase in their amplitude, despite diminished release per impulse, would require a greater number of impulses to be present in each burst. Accordingly, electrophysiological studies in VTA dopaminergic neurons have shown that CB1 receptor agonists do produce such an increase in action potentials per burst (French, 1997; Gessa et al., 1998).
Overall, this study contributes to the growing body of evidence that dopamine transients are a major form of communication in the NAc by this neurotransmitter. Because dopamine release in limbic and cortical structures is considered to play an important role in the rewarding aspects of several drugs of abuse, including marijuana (Gardner, 2002; Tanda and Goldberg, 2003), the current findings provide further insight into the manner in which marijuana exerts its reinforcing effects.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Grant DA10900 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. We thank the University of North Carolina Electronics Facility for the design and construction of the instrumentation used in this work. Thanks are also due to Dr. Donita Robinson and Garret Stuber for helpful comments regarding this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Prof. R. Mark Wightman, Department of Chemistry, CB 3290, Venable Hall, The University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3290. E-mail: mw{at}unc.edu.
DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0529-04.2004
Copyright © 2004 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/04/244393-08$15.00/0