Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleNeurobiology of Disease

Expression of a Poly-Glutamine-Ataxin-3 Transgene in Orexin Neurons Induces Narcolepsy–Cataplexy in the Rat

Carsten T. Beuckmann, Christopher M. Sinton, S. Clay Williams, James A. Richardson, Robert E. Hammer, Takeshi Sakurai and Masashi Yanagisawa
Journal of Neuroscience 5 May 2004, 24 (18) 4469-4477; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5560-03.2004
Carsten T. Beuckmann
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christopher M. Sinton
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
S. Clay Williams
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
James A. Richardson
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Robert E. Hammer
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Takeshi Sakurai
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Masashi Yanagisawa
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The sleep disorder narcolepsy has been linked to loss of hypothalamic neurons producing the orexin (hypocretin) neuropeptides. Here, we report the generation of transgenic rats expressing a human ataxin-3 fragment with an elongated polyglutamyl stretch under control of the human prepro-orexin promoter (orexin/ataxin-3 rats). At 17 weeks of age, the transgenic rats exhibited postnatal loss of orexin-positive neurons in the lateral hypothalamus, and orexin-containing projections were essentially undetectable. The loss of orexin production resulted in the expression of a phenotype with fragmented vigilance states, a decreased latency to rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and increased REM sleep time during the dark active phase. Wakefulness time was also reduced during the dark phase, and this effect was concentrated at the photoperiod boundaries. Direct transitions from wakefulness to REM sleep, a defining characteristic of narcolepsy, occurred frequently. Brief episodes of muscle atonia and postural collapse resembling cataplexy were also noted while rats maintained the electroencephalographic characteristics of wakefulness. These findings indicate that the orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rat could provide a useful model of human narcolepsy.

  • cataplexy
  • electroencephalography
  • electromyography
  • sleep
  • REM
  • lateral hypothalamus

Introduction

Narcolepsy is a debilitating disorder, affecting 20–60 per 100,000 adults (Mignot, 1998; Overeem et al., 2001; Beuckmann and Yanagisawa, 2002). A cardinal symptom is excessive daytime sleepiness, manifested particularly as attacks of somnolence at inappropriate times. The latency for rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is also notably reduced, and the presence of pathologically short transitions from wakefulness to REM sleep is a diagnostic criterion for the disorder. Other symptoms of narcolepsy include hypnogogic and hypnopompic hallucinations and cataplexy. Cataplexy is characterized by sudden attacks of muscle weakness, frequently triggered by strong emotions (Bassetti and Aldrich, 1996) with patients remaining conscious during an attack (Scrima, 1981; Billiard, 1985; Gerhardstein et al., 1999; Taheri et al., 2002). The symptoms of narcolepsy have been considered an intrusion of REM sleep-related phenomena into wakefulness, indicating that the disorder may be one of vigilance state boundary control (Saper et al., 2001).

The concurrent discoveries that the autosomal recessive form of canine narcolepsy is caused by a mutation in the orexin receptor-2 gene (Lin et al., 1999; Hungs et al., 2001) and that orexin–/– mice exhibit a phenotype similar to human narcolepsy (Chemelli et al., 1999) provided insight into the pathophysiology of narcolepsy. In narcolepsy patients, a dysfunction or loss of orexin-containing neurons in the hypothalamus was subsequently confirmed (Peyron et al., 2000; Thannickal et al., 2000). Orexin-expressing neurons constitute a highly delimited population in the perifornical region of the lateral hypothalamus (LH) (de Lecea et al., 1998; Peyron et al., 1998; Sakurai et al., 1998; Nambu et al., 1999; Chou et al., 2001). Two neuropeptides, orexin-A and -B (hypocretin-1 and -2), are derived from the prepro-orexin gene and act at two G-protein-coupled receptors, orexin receptor-1 and -2 (de Lecea et al., 1998; Sakurai et al., 1998). In the human, mutations of the prepro-orexin or orexin receptor genes appear to be extremely rare (Peyron et al., 2000; Gencik et al., 2001). However, undetectable to very low levels of orexin-A neuropeptide in the CSF have been described in most patients with narcolepsy–cataplexy, whereas orexin-A levels of patients presenting with other disorders were comparable with those of healthy controls (Nishino et al., 2000, 2001; Ripley et al., 2001; Mignot et al., 2002). The number of orexin neurons is therefore likely to be diminished in narcoleptic patients (Peyron et al., 2000; Thannickal et al., 2000), although the cause for this apparent neuronal degeneration remains undetermined (Lin et al., 2001; Taheri et al., 2002).

In an attempt to mimic the loss of orexin neurons, we recently created transgenic mice in which orexin neurons expressed a cytotoxic gene product (a truncated form of human ataxin-3) under control of the human prepro-orexin promoter (Hara et al., 2001). This resulted in degeneration of orexin-containing neurons and a narcoleptic phenotype. We have now expressed the orexin/ataxin-3 transgene in the rat, a species more widely used for physiological and pharmacological studies, and we describe here the initial histological and phenotypic characterization of the transgene expression in this species.

Materials and Methods

Generation of transgenic rat lines. The orexin/ataxin-3 transgene expresses an N-terminally truncated human ataxin-3 protein containing a Q77 polyglutamine stretch under control of the human prepro-orexin promoter (Hara et al., 2001). The C terminus of the transgene contains a Myc oncogene epitope for histological examination. The transgene was injected into pronuclei of fertilized Wistar rat eggs to generate founder animals, which were bred to produce ten orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic lines. Two lines were chosen for additional evaluation. Although one line is described here in detail, both lines displayed an essentially comparable phenotype. Lines were maintained in the hemizygous state for these studies. The transgene was propagated in a Mendelian manner, indicating no embryonic lethality. Throughout the study, animals were housed under a constant 12 hr light/dark cycle with free access to food and water. All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Research Advisory Committee of the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center and were strictly in accordance with National Institutes of Health guidelines.

Genotyping. PCR was performed on genomic DNA from tail biopsies. Primers used were 5′-GCA GCG GCC ATT CCT TGG-3′ and 5′-CAG CGT AAT CTG GAA CAT CGT ATG GG-3′ against the orexin/ataxin-3 transgene and 5′-GCA CCG AAG ATA CCA TCT CTC CGG ATT GC-3′ and 5′-GAC TCT GGA TCC GCC CCG GGG CGC TAA AGC-3′ against the endogenous rat prepro-orexin gene as an internal control.

Immunohistochemistry. All histological examinations were performed on male hemizygous transgenic rats (N3 back-cross generations into Sprague Dawley) and their male wild-type littermates. Male rats (wild-type and hemizygous transgenic littermates 4, 7, 10, 13, and 17 weeks of age) were deeply anesthetized with chloral hydrate (3.5 gm/kg, i.p.) and transcardially perfused with 150 ml of ice-cold PBS (11 U/ml heparin) and 150 ml of 4% (w/v) paraformaldehyde in PBS, pH 7.4. Brains were rapidly dissected and postfixed for 12 hr in 4% (w/v) paraformaldehyde in PBS, pH 7.4, at 4°C before being cryoprotected by equilibrating them in 30% sucrose in PBS. After cyroprotection, samples were frozen and sectioned (30 μm) with an SM2000R sliding microtome (Leica, Nussloch, Germany). Sections were then stored at 4°C in 0.004% sodium azide in PBS before histochemical staining.

For orexin/Myc double-immunohistochemistry, free-floating sections were rinsed with PBS three times for 5 min, followed by 0.3% hydrogen peroxide for 30 min to quench endogenous peroxidases. Sections were then rinsed again with PBS three times for 5 min. Nonspecific binding sites were blocked with 3% (v/v) normal horse serum (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) with 0.25% Triton X-100 in PBS for 2 hr at room temperature. After blocking, sections were incubated with Myc monoclonal antibody (Ab) (9E10; 1:400 dilution; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA) against the transgene product for 12 hr at 4°C. As a secondary Ab, biotinylated horse anti-mouse Ab (Vector Laboratories) at a 1:600 dilution in 0.25% Triton X-100/PBS was applied for 2 hr. Subsequently, sections were incubated with horseradish peroxidase streptavidin at a 1:500 dilution in 0.5% Triton X-100/PBS for 1 hr, followed by application of NiSO4-enhanced DAB solution (60 μl of 10% NiSO4 per 2 ml of DAB; Dako, High Wycombe, UK) for 7–10 min at room temperature to create a dark brown-to-black precipitation. Orexin immunohistochemistry (light brown DAB staining without nickel enhancement) was performed as described previously (Chemelli et al., 1999) after Myc staining was completed. Sections were then mounted onto coated slides, dehydrated through an ascending ethanol/xylene series, and embedded using Permount (Fisher Scientific, Houston, TX).

Vigilance state determination. For behavioral evaluation, the control group consisted of male wild-type littermates of transgenic rats (N3 back-cross generations into Sprague Dawley) as well as of age- and weight-matched nonrelated male wild-type Sprague Dawley rats (Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA). At 14 weeks of age, rats (8 hemizygous orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats, 20 wild-type controls) were anesthetized (40 mg/kg ketamine, 4 mg/kg xylazine, 1 mg/kg acepromazine, i.p.) and surgically implanted with recording electrodes under sterile conditions. Miniaturized electrodes were affixed to the skull using a glass ionomer dental cement (Ketac Cem; ESPE, Norristown, PA), such that the electrodes just penetrated the skull and touched the dura. EEG signals were recorded unilaterally from fronto-occipital electrode pairs, positioned 1.5 mm rostral and 1.45 mm lateral from bregma, and 3.1 mm caudal and 1.45 mm lateral from bregma. EMG signals were concurrently recorded from two flexible wires, insulated except at the tips, and implanted bilaterally by blunt dissection into the nuchal musculature. The rats were tethered to a counterbalanced arm (Instech Laboratories, Plymouth Meeting, PA) that allowed virtually unrestricted mobility of the animals and exerted minimal weight. All rats recovered from surgery and habituated to the recording conditions for 3 weeks before recording commenced.

EEG–EMG signals were recorded under controlled conditions (12 hr light/dark cycle; 24 ± 1°C) for three consecutive days as described previously (Chemelli et al., 1999). Signals were digitized at 250 Hz, recorded to hard disk, and subsequently archived to optical media for off-line analysis. Concurrently with EEG–EMG recording, the behavior of the rats was videotaped for the first 4 hr of each dark phase using an infrared light source. For vigilance state analysis, EEG–EMG data were classified visually into 20 sec epochs by two independent observers blinded as to genotype. Standard criteria for rodent vigilance state measurements were used (Radulovacki et al., 1984), and results were summarized in terms of non-REM (NREM) sleep, REM sleep, and wakefulness. Epochs of wakefulness were not discriminated for active and quiet wakefulness. Summary vigilance state data for each rat were averaged over 3 d, before the results were grouped according to genotype, and analyzed by alternate Welch t test. The criterion for rejection of the null hypothesis was p < 0.05.

The following vigilance state parameters for the 12 hr dark and light periods were derived from the summary data for each rat before averaging by genotype. Total times spent in wakefulness, NREM, and REM sleep were derived by summing the total number of 20 sec epochs in each state. The number of episodes of wakefulness, NREM, and REM sleep was counts of the number of episodes of that state. Because we adopted a 20 sec epoch duration for vigilance state analysis, the minimum duration for an episode to be included in this analysis was 10 sec. Mean episode durations for wakefulness, NREM, and REM sleep were determined by dividing the total time spent in each state by the number of episodes of that state. Mean inter-REM interval was derived by determining the interval from the start of one REM sleep episode to the start of the next REM sleep episode and calculating the average of these intervals. A minimum of 10 sec of REM sleep was required before an episode was counted.

Mean REM sleep latency was determined by averaging the time elapsed from the beginning of a continuous NREM sleep episode to the beginning of the subsequent REM sleep episode. This calculation was also based on the 20 sec episode duration. Thus, if >10 sec of wakefulness occurred during an NREM sleep episode, the calculation was restarted and the interval was derived from the beginning of the NREM sleep episode that commenced after the intrusion of wakefulness. REM sleep latency as calculated here is thus sensitive to vigilance state fragmentation, because a brief arousal that lasted >10 sec was sufficient to reset the calculation. Similarly, >10 sec of NREM sleep before the REM sleep episode was required for the latency to be included in the average. Thus, the minimum latency in this calculation was 20 sec, and episodes of direct transition to REM sleep were not counted.

Episodes of abnormal transition from wakefulness to REM sleep are considered a defining characteristic of narcolepsy. For this initial study in the rat, we defined an abnormal REM sleep transition as an REM sleep episode that followed a minimum of 60 sec of wakefulness with <20 sec of intervening NREM sleep. The former criterion ensured that brief arousals just before an REM sleep episode were not counted as abnormal transitions. The total count of such events, defined as the number of episodes of abnormal transition to REM sleep, was determined.

The EEG frequency distribution was analyzed by power spectral analysis [i.e., fast Fourier transform (FFT)] from 1 to 32 Hz using SleepSign 2.0 (Kissei Comtech, Tokyo, Japan). For derivation of power spectra for each vigilance state, FFT data for 150–200 representative artifact-free 4 sec epochs per rat (50 epochs for cataplexy per rat) from the 72 hr continuous EEG recordings were averaged for the transgenic rats and their wild-type littermates. Data were then normalized to a spectral density function by dividing each bin by the total average power of all epochs for that rat over the recording period to allow comparison between individual animals. Statistical analysis was by Student's t test, and the null hypothesis was rejected at p < 0.05.

Results

Generation of orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats

Expression of the orexin/ataxin-3 transgene was examined by immunohistochemical staining against the Myc tag at 4 weeks of age. We observed ectopic expression of the transgene outside the LH in 4 of 10 lines; we discarded these lines. In the line described here, the transgene product was colocalized exclusively in orexin-expressing neurons of the LH as verified by double immunostaining against Myc and orexin-A (Fig. 1A,B). In this line, ectopic expression outside the LH was absent throughout the brain (data not shown). By 4 weeks of age, the number of orexin-expressing cells was notably diminished in the perifornical region of the LH compared with wild-type littermate controls (Fig. 1C,D). The number of orexin-positive cells continued to decline at 7, 10, and 13 weeks of age (data not shown). At 17 weeks, the age of vigilance state recording, essentially no orexin-positive neurons could be found in the LH (Fig. 1E,F). However, at this age, immunostaining against Myc revealed picnotic remnants of cell nuclei that were positive for the orexin/ataxin-3 transgene product in the LH, despite the complete lack of orexin-like immunoreactivity (data not shown).

  Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A–H, Colocalization of orexin neuropeptide and the orexin/ataxin-3 transgene product (A, B) and disappearance of orexin-positive neurons (C–F) and projections (G, H) in orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic rats. The LH region of wild-type rats (A) and orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic littermates (B) was stained using antibodies against orexin-A (brown) and against the Myc tag of the transgene (black). All transgenic animals showed nuclear staining of the transgene product only in orexin neurons. Wild-type animals showed a normal distribution of orexin neurons in the LH at 4 weeks (C) and 17 weeks (E) of age. In contrast, orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic littermates showed a notable reduction of orexin-immunoreactivity by 4 weeks of age (D) and a virtually complete loss of immunoreactivity at 17 weeks of age (F). No difference in MCH neuronal population in the perifornical area could be found at 17 weeks between wild-type rats (E, inset) and their orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic littermates (F, inset). Dense orexin-containing projections in the thalamic paraventricular nucleus of wild-type animals (G) were undetectable in transgenic animals at 17 weeks of age (H). f, Fornix; 3V, third ventricle. Scale bars: A, B, 40 μm; C–H, insets, 200 μm. A–F, Bright-field microscopy; G, H, dark-field microscopy.

Orexin-containing neuronal projections were also examined. The thalamic paraventricular nucleus is an area that, inter alia, receives a dense orexinergic innervation (Fig. 1G). In transgenic rats, no orexin-positive projections could be found at 17 weeks of age in this thalamic nucleus (Fig. 1H). Other brain regions, including the tuberomammillary nucleus, central gray, perifornical nucleus, arcuate nucleus, laterodorsal and pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei, locus coeruleus, and raphe nuclei, were also examined and similarly found to be lacking orexin projections.

Neurons expressing melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) are intermingled with orexin neurons in the LH but form a distinct neuronal population (Elias et al., 1998). No difference in MCH expression was observed between orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic animals (Fig. 1F, inset) and their wild-type littermates at 17 weeks of age (Fig. 1E, inset). Thus, the disappearance of orexin expression in the LH was not the result of a generalized lesion or widespread cell degeneration in that area. In summary, the immunohistochemical evaluation of orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic rats indicated a selective disappearance of orexin-containing neurons in the LH, combined with a concomitant loss of orexinergic projections throughout the brain.

Vigilance state characteristics of orexin/ataxin-3 rats

REM sleep dysregulation

A hallmark characteristic of narcolepsy is dysregulation of REM sleep, and orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats showed marked deviations from the wild-type phenotype in this sleep stage. A representative hypnogram during the dark, active phase in wild-type rats is displayed in Figure 2A, and the corresponding hypnogram for a transgenic rat is displayed in Figure 2B. Bouts of REM sleep in the wild-type rats were always preceded by an NREM sleep episode that usually lasted for several minutes: the mean REM sleep latency was longer during the dark phase in these rats (2.9 ± 0.2 min vs 3.8 ± 0.2 min for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p = 0.002) (Table 1). In contrast, episodes of abnormal transition to REM sleep could be observed in all orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats, primarily during the dark phase, whereas this was found only once in one wild-type control rat during a total of ∼1500 hr of recording time. Orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats had 3.8 ± 0.8 episodes showing abnormal transition to REM sleep per 12 hr dark phase and 0.3 ± 0.1 of these episodes per 12 hr light phase. Episodes of abnormal transition were therefore less frequent than normal REM sleep events (42.9 ± 2.8 and 27.9 ± 2.6 REM sleep events during dark and light phase, respectively) (Table 1). The mean duration of these episodes of REM sleep after an abnormal transition was 119 ± 11 and 68 ± 15 sec for the dark and light phases, respectively. Therefore, these durations were comparable with normal REM sleep episode durations (90.5 ± 2.2 and 67.1 ± 4.8 sec during the dark and light phase, respectively) (cf. Table 1).

  Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

A, B, Representative dark-phase hypnograms of a wild-type rat (A) and an orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic littermate (B). The hypnogram of the transgenic animal shows more rapid cycling between vigilance states. It also exhibits direct transitions from wakefulness to REM sleep (indicated by arrowheads). W, Wakefulness; NR, NREM sleep; R, REM sleep.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Vigilance state parameters recorded from orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous (Tg/+) and wild-type control (+/+) rats

Figure 3 shows representative traces of EEG–EMG recording from an orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rat. Normal transitions from wakefulness to NREM sleep (Fig. 3A) were indistinguishable from those in wild-type rats. However, as noted above, abnormal transitions to REM sleep (in this example, a direct transition without intervening NREM sleep) could be observed frequently in transgenic animals (Fig. 3B). Power spectral analysis of the EEG did not reveal any differences in the frequency spectra of the vigilance states between orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic and wild-type rats (Fig. 4A,B). Furthermore, in orexin/ataxin-3 rats, the average power spectrum of REM sleep episodes after an abnormal transition was indistinguishable from the corresponding spectrum for those REM sleep episodes that were preceded by NREM sleep (Fig. 4C). Thus, an REM sleep episode after an abnormal transition was essentially identical to a normal REM sleep episode with respect to both the EEG frequency distribution and episode duration.

  Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Representative EEG–EMG recordings from an orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic rat. A, Normal transition from wakefulness to NREM sleep. The EEG signal increases in amplitude and slows in frequency, whereas neck muscle tone diminishes in the EMG signal. B, Abnormal direct transition from wakefulness to REM sleep. The EEG signal starts with the typical mixed-frequency, low-amplitude, wakefulness pattern, which directly gives way to the regular low-amplitude pattern of REM sleep, dominated by θ activity in the 6–10 Hz range, with concomitant complete neck muscle atonia. C, Cataplexy-like event. Although the EEG characteristic of wakefulness remains unchanged, a sudden and transient complete neck muscle atonia occurs, after which normal muscle activity suddenly resumes. Note that θ activity in the EEG signal is less than that recorded during REM sleep, and that no visual indications of NREM sleep are apparent during this interval.

  Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Power spectra of EEG–EMG recordings from wild-type rats and their orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic littermates. A, Representative EEG power spectra of wakefulness, NREM sleep, and REM sleep in a wild-type rat. B, Representative EEG power spectra of an orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic littermate. SOREM (i.e., sleep-onset REM) episodes designate those REM sleep episodes that follow an abnormal transition from wakefulness. Note that the mean EEG frequency distribution of SOREM episodes is essentially identical to that recorded during normally occurring REM sleep in the same animal. The signal amplitudes in A and B have been normalized in each animal to allow comparison across animals. C, D, Representative absolute EEG power spectra of REM sleep and SOREM episodes (C) and of wakefulness and cataplexy-like events in an orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic rat (D). E, Average EMG signal power recorded during the designated vigilance states from a representative orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic rat. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM.

The time spent in REM sleep during the dark phase in orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic animals was approximately twice that recorded in the wild-type controls (64.2 ± 3.0 vs 32.9 ± 2.6 min for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p < 0.0001) (Table 1). This resulted from the increase in the number of REM sleep episodes (42.9 ± 2.8 vs 23.4 ± 1.9 for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p < 0.0001). In the light phase, however, overall REM sleep time was significantly less in the transgenic rats than in the wild-type controls (30.6 ± 2.6 vs 50.4 ± 2.6 min for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p < 0.0001), resulting from a decrease in the number of REM sleep episodes (27.9 ± 2.6 vs 41.8 ± 2.4 for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p = 0.001). In contrast to these differences in REM sleep episode number, the mean duration of REM sleep episodes was not significantly different between the transgenic and wild-type rats in either the dark or light phases (Table 1). The hourly distribution of REM sleep time across the 24 hr also was strikingly different between the genotypes (Fig. 5A). In fact, during only 6 hr, primarily clustered around the photoperiod boundaries were the hourly REM sleep times not significantly different between the wild-type and transgenic rats (p > 0.05). Therefore, the genotypes not only demonstrated the opposite circadian pattern in the incidence of REM sleep, as also evidenced by the total time spent in this stage during the light and dark 12 hr periods (Table 1), but the transgenic rats also did not show the gradual increase in REM sleep time during the 12 hr light period that was a hallmark of REM sleep in the wild-type rats.

  Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

A, B, Time spent each hour (in minutes; mean ± SEM) in REM sleep (A) and wakefulness (B) for wild-type rats and their orexin/ataxin-3 hemizygous transgenic littermates. Significant differences between the genotypes (t test; p < 0.05) are marked by asterisks. The dark period is denoted by the horizontal bar.

Fragmentation of wakefulness and NREM sleep

The hypnogram of a wild-type rat (Fig. 2A) showed prolonged periods of wakefulness with occasional sleep episodes. In contrast, orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats showed marked fragmentation of vigilance states during the normally active dark phase, characterized by more rapid cycling between wakefulness and sleep (Fig. 2B). This fragmentation during the dark phase is reflected in an increased number of wakefulness episodes (91.5 ± 7.4 vs 70.0 ± 3.8 for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; mean ± SEM; p = 0.03), NREM sleep episodes (96.7 ± 6.8 vs 75.5 ± 3.9; p = 0.02), and REM sleep episodes (42.9 ± 2.8 vs 23.4 ± 1.9; p < 0.0001) (Table 1). However, although the transgenic rats had a greater number of wakefulness episodes, the reduced mean episode duration (321 ± 37 vs 490 ± 52 sec for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p = 0.014) resulted in less time spent awake (460 ± 19 vs 516 ± 10 min for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p = 0.03). We also noted a corresponding tendency toward increased NREM sleep time in the transgenic rats (194 ± 17 vs 170 ± 9 min for transgenic and wild-type rats, respectively; p = 0.23) (Table 1). A plot of the hourly distribution of wakefulness time demonstrated that the difference between the genotypes in time spent awake was concentrated primarily at the photoperiod boundaries (Fig. 5B). Figure 5B also shows that, even in terms of the hourly distribution, wakefulness time during the light period was very similar in both genotypes.

Behavioral analysis

Behavior was monitored for 4 hr by infrared video recording, starting at the onset of the dark phase, while EEG–EMG signals were recorded simultaneously. Subsequent analysis of the video recordings revealed that episodes of abnormal transition to REM sleep occasionally occurred during motivated behavior, such as ambulation or drinking (cf. supplemental video 1, available at www.jneurosci.org). Each abnormal transition to REM sleep was associated with a sudden loss of muscle tone and concomitant loss of posture. Rocking movements along the body axis, as described in orexin–/– mice (Chemelli et al., 1999) and in saporin-lesioned rats (Gerashchenko et al., 2001), were not observed. Behavioral arrests associated with these abnormal REM sleep episodes in the transgenic rats were always abruptly terminated with the resumption of full mobility and purposeful behavior. As noted above, the EEG power spectrum for these episodes of REM sleep after an abnormal transition was not different from the spectrum recorded during normally occurring REM sleep (Fig. 4C).

In addition to typical behavioral arrests associated with abnormal transitions to REM sleep, another type of postural collapse was observed (cf. supplemental video 2, available at www.jneurosci.org). These latter episodes could not be behaviorally differentiated from an episode of abnormal transition by video photography alone. As in abnormal transitions to REM sleep, the rat showed a sudden and complete loss of muscle tone, and purposeful behavior ceased (Fig. 3C). However, power spectral analysis of the corresponding EEG and EMG signals revealed that orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats had this loss of muscle tone at a time when the EEG maintained a pattern very similar to that recorded during normal wakefulness (Fig. 4D). These arrests occurred both during the dark (4.7 times per 12 hr) and light (2.8 times per 12 hr) phases and were generally short in duration (dark phase, 12 ± 1 sec; light phase, 20 ± 4 sec). As displayed in Figure 4E, muscle tone, when compared with either normal wakefulness or NREM sleep, was markedly reduced and became essentially absent during REM sleep, REM sleep after an abnormal transition, and these episodes of behavioral collapse with wakefulness-like EEG. In contrast to human and canine cataplectic events, however, which are often triggered by emotional stimuli (Aldrich, 1992; Riehl et al., 1998), we could not reliably elicit behavioral arrests in the orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats using external emotive stimuli.

Discussion

Although canine and murine models of narcolepsy have been established previously (Mitler et al., 1976; Chemelli et al., 1999, 2000; Hara et al., 2001; Kisanuki et al., 2000, 2001; Willie et al., 2003), human narcolepsy is likely to result from a gradual degradation of orexin neurons and consequent loss of orexin expression. Here, we adapted to the rat the technique for producing orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic mice, in which orexin neurons were genetically ablated by selective expression of a cytotoxic poly-Q-ataxin-3 protein (Hara et al., 2001). An alternative approach was used by Gerashchenko et al. (2001), who infused orexin-B conjugated to the ribosome-inhibiting cytotoxic protein saporin into the LH of wild-type rats. Orexin-expressing neurons presumably possess orexin autoreceptors, and the infusion of saporin thus resulted in a loss of these neurons and a narcoleptic-like phenotype. However, other LH cell populations that express orexin receptors, including MCH and adenosine-deaminase-containing neurons, were also lesioned by this technique (Gerashchenko et al., 2001). Moreover, a recent electrophysiological study showed that orexin activates orexin neurons via glutamatergic interneurons (Li et al., 2002). Because orexin B-saporin might also ablate these interneurons, this approach cannot be considered a precise model of human narcolepsy, in which orexin neurons appear to be affected in a highly specific way.

The rat model described here thus combines the selective targeting of orexin-expressing neurons and a gradual loss of these cells during ontogenesis, with the advantages of a species that is widely adopted for physiological and pharmacological studies. The most important characteristics of this model are: decreased wakefulness during the normally active dark phase; fragmentation of wakefulness and NREM sleep patterns; shortened REM sleep latency; episodes of abnormal transition to REM sleep; differences in total REM sleep time; and cataplexy-like behavioral arrests, during which the animals show muscle atonia comparable with REM sleep but remain awake as judged by spectral analysis of the EEG. Additionally, vigilance state fragmentation during the dark phase indicates that transgenic rats are unable to maintain prolonged periods of wakefulness. Ongoing activity was more frequently interrupted by bouts of NREM sleep than in the wild-type controls. However, despite these marked differences in sleep during the dark phase, the time spent in wakefulness and NREM sleep over a 24 hr period remained unchanged in the transgenic rat. Overall, these results are directly comparable with vigilance state observations in narcoleptic humans, and we conclude that the phenotype of the orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rat closely resembles narcolepsy.

Cataplexy, in particular, is difficult to provoke in a clinical setting in narcoleptic patients, and in most cases, the diagnosis of cataplexy is based on self-evaluation by the patient. The mechanisms of cataplexy have been intensely studied to date in narcoleptic dogs (Riehl et al., 1998; Fujiki et al., 2002). Elicited by emotional stimuli or food presentation, cataplectic attacks in these dogs result in complete muscle atonia, frequently affecting the whole body. Concurrently, the animals are awake and remain aware of their surroundings (Nishino and Mignot, 1997). Analysis of orexin–/– mice also demonstrated the existence of episodes of postural collapse, during which the mice remained conscious as judged by behavioral observation (Willie et al., 2003). Detailed characterization of the abnormal state transitions in the orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rat are now required, but we noted similarities between these existing descriptions of cataplexy and a particular type of behavioral arrest in these rats.

The striking difference between the wild-type and orexin/ataxin-3 transgenic rats in the hourly distribution of REM sleep time throughout the 24 hr period contrasts with the similarity of the corresponding wakefulness times, especially during the light phase. The wakefulness deficit in the transgenic rats during the dark, or active, period is essentially concentrated at the photoperiod boundaries and in particular at the end of this period. This time corresponds closely to the timing, in humans, of maximal circadian alertness, which consolidates wakefulness at the end of the active part of the daily cycle when the homeostatic drive for sleepiness is highest (Dijk and Czeisler, 1994). With a polyphasic sleep pattern and no single consolidated wakefulness bout, a similar alertness signal has not been investigated in the rat. However, our data in the wild-type animals demonstrate, just before the beginning of the sleep period, a significant increase in wakefulness, which is absent after the loss of orexin in the transgenic rats. Importantly, the circadian variation in the orexin signal in rat brain peaks at the same time as this increase in wakefulness, late in the active portion of the daily cycle (Taheri et al., 2000; Fujiki et al., 2001; Yoshida et al., 2001). Our data thus support the proposal that orexin contributes to the daily variation in wakefulness at the end of the active period (Mignot, 2001). Previous corroboration for this hypothesis came from Dantz et al. (1994), who used a forced desynchrony protocol to show that narcoleptic patients have a deficit in circadian wakefulness, even though their circadian pacemaker per se and homeostatic sleep drive are normal. Also, a recent study in the squirrel monkey has demonstrated that the maximal orexin signal in this species corresponds to the timing of maximum circadian alertness at the end of the active period (Zeitzer et al., 2003).

Comparison of the hourly distribution of REM sleep time between the genotypes indicates an apparently continuous effect of the loss of orexin on the expression of REM sleep. Thus, the absence of orexin in the transgenic rats results in increased REM sleep throughout the normally active phase and, consequentially, a reduced homeostatic drive for REM sleep during the light phase. In the wild-type rat, the homeostatic drive for REM sleep during the normal sleep phase is expressed as a gradual increase in the hourly times spent in REM sleep. Significantly, this distribution of REM sleep in the wild-type rats is inversely correlated with the diurnal variation of the orexin signal, which remains high throughout the dark period and reaches a minimum toward the end of the light phase in this species (Taheri et al., 2000; Fujiki et al., 2001; Yoshida et al., 2001). Together, these data suggest that the release of orexin has an inhibitory influence on the appearance of REM sleep.

Importantly, the difference in REM sleep time between the genotypes was attributable to a change in the number of episodes of REM sleep, whereas the REM sleep bout length remained unchanged. This indicates that orexin normally inhibits the onset of an episode of REM sleep but, once initiated, it does not affect the characteristics of that episode. Kiyashchenko et al. (2002) reported that orexin release is highest during active wakefulness and REM sleep. This result left open the possibility that orexin also might be involved in the generation of the REM sleep state. The current data now preclude this possibility and suggest that the link to orexin is through ongoing motor patterns, whether they are expressed as in active wakefulness or inhibited as in REM sleep. We therefore speculate that an important function of orexin is to inhibit the appearance of REM sleep and particularly when the brain state, including cortical activation, is most similar during active wakefulness. For this reason, when orexin is absent, the REM sleep switch appears biased (Saper et al., 2001; Sutcliffe and de Lecea, 2002), and direct transitions from wakefulness to REM sleep can occur.

In summary, our data show that the presence of orexin impacts vigilance state control in two ways. First, it may act as a circadian arousal signal to enhance alertness at the end of the normal wake period when the homeostatic drive for sleepiness is reaching its maximum. Second, it appears to inhibit the onset of REM sleep episodes. Orexin is excitatory to the brainstem monoaminergic cells, which are quiescent during REM sleep (i.e., the REM-off cells) (Hagan et al., 1999; Horvath et al., 1999; Brown et al., 2001), providing a mechanism by which the neuropeptide could influence REM sleep initiation. The diurnal variation in orexin levels, closely coupled to ongoing motor activity (Kiyashchenko et al., 2002), thus provides a basis for the variation in the expression of REM sleep throughout the nychthemeron. In this regard, it is interesting to note that McCarley and Massaquoi (1992), during development of a model of the REM sleep oscillator, postulated the existence of a circadian control factor that excited monoaminergic REM-off cells to prevent the occurrence of REM sleep and so influence the diurnal distribution of the state. Our data indicate that orexin could be a factor that plays such a role in REM sleep expression.

We conclude that this model of narcolepsy in the rat is likely to prove useful for research into the pathophysiology of the disorder and for the discovery and validation of pharmacological treatments based on orexin neurotransmission. With its ease of breeding propagation, in which one allele of the orexin/ataxin-3 transgene is sufficient to yield the narcoleptic phenotype combined with extensive knowledge of neurophysiology and neuropharmacology in the rat, this model will be valuable in continuing investigations into narcolepsy.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported in part by research grants from the Perot Family Foundation, W. M. Keck Foundation, and Exploratory Research for Advanced Technology of Japan Science and Technology Agency to M.Y. C.T.B. is a former Associate, R.E.H. is a Senior Associate, and M.Y. is an Investigator of the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. We thank S. Dixon, E. A. Hinnant, S. Seyedkalal, and B. Perkins for technical assistance, J. T. Willie for critical comments on this manuscript, M. Kelly for help with the statistical analysis, and Kissei Comtech (Tokyo, Japan) for supplying the software (Sleep Sign 2.0) for the power spectral analysis.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Masashi Yanagisawa, Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Department of Molecular Genetics, University of Texas, Southwestern Medical Center, 5323 Harry Hines Boulevard, Dallas, TX 75390-9050. E-mail: Masashi.Yanagisawa{at}UTSouthwestern.edu.

  • C. T. Beuckmann's present address: Eisai Company, Tokodai 5–1-3, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 300–2635, Japan.

  • DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5560-03.2004

  • Copyright © 2004 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/04/244469-09$15.00/0

  • ↵* C.T.B. and C.M.S. contributed equally to this work.

References

  1. ↵
    Aldrich MS (1992) Narcolepsy. Neurology 42[Suppl 6]: 34–43.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  2. ↵
    Bassetti C, Aldrich MS (1996) Narcolepsy. Neurol Clin 14: 545–571.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    Beuckmann CT, Yanagisawa M (2002) Orexins: from neuropeptides to energy homeostasis and sleep/wake regulation. J Mol Med 80: 329–342.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    Billiard M (1985) Narcolepsy–clinical features and aetiology. Ann Clin Res 17: 220–226.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  5. ↵
    Brown RE, Sergeeva O, Eriksson KS, Haas HL (2001) Orexin A excites serotonergic neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus of the rat. Neuropharmacology 40: 457–459.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Chemelli RM, Willie JT, Sinton CM, Elmquist JK, Scammell T, Lee C, Richardson JA, Williams SC, Xiong Y, Kisanuki Y, Fitch TE, Nakazoto M, Hammer RE, Saper CB, Yanagisawa M (1999) Narcolepsy in orexin knockout mice: molecular genetics of sleep regulation. Cell 98: 437–451.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Chemelli RM, Sinton CM, Yanagisawa M (2000) Polysomnographic characterization of orexin-2 receptor knock-out mice. Sleep 23: A296–A297.
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    Chou TC, Lee CE, Lu J, Elmquist JK, Hara J, Willie JT, Beuckmann CT, Chemelli RM, Sakurai T, Yanagisawa M, Saper CB, Scammell TE (2001) Orexin (hypocretin) neurons contain dynorphin. J Neurosci 21: RC168.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    Dantz B, Edgar DM, Dement WC (1994) Circadian rhythms in narcolepsy: studies on a 90 minute day. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 90: 24–35.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    de Lecea L, Kilduff TS, Peyron C, Gao X, Foye PE, Danielson PE, Fukuhara C, Battenberg EL, Gautvik VT, Bartlett FS, Frankel WN, van den Pol AN, Bloom FE, Gautvik KM, Sutcliffe JG (1998) The hypocretins: hypothalamus-specific peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 322–327.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA (1994) Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans. Neurosci Lett 166: 63–68.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Elias CF, Saper CB, Maratos-Flier E, Tritos NA, Lee C, Kelly J, Tatro JB, Hoffman GE, Ollmann MM, Barsh GS, Sakurai T, Yanagisawa M, Elmquist JK (1998) Chemically defined projections linking the mediobasal hypothalamus and the lateral hypothalamic area. J Comp Neurol 402: 442–459.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    Fujiki N, Yoshida Y, Ripley B, Honda K, Mignot E, Nishino S (2001) Changes in CSF hypocretin-1 (orexin A) levels in rats across 24 hours and in response to food deprivation. NeuroReport 12: 993–997.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Fujiki N, Morris L, Mignot E, Nishino S (2002) Analysis of onset location, laterality and propagation of cataplexy in canine narcolepsy. Psychiat Clin Neurosci 56: 275–276.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. ↵
    Gencik M, Dahmen N, Wieczorek S, Kasten M, Bierbrauer J, Anghelescu I, Szegedi A, Menezes Saecker AM, Epplen JT (2001) A prepro-orexin gene polymorphism is associated with narcolepsy. Neurology 56: 115–117.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    Gerashchenko D, Kohls MD, Greco M, Waleh NS, Salin-Pascual R, Kilduff TS, Lappi DA, Shiromani PJ (2001) Hypocretin-2-saporin lesions of the lateral hypothalamus produce narcoleptic-like sleep behavior in the rat. J Neurosci 21: 7273–7283.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    Gerhardstein R, Day R, Rosenthal L (1999) Narcolepsy and other causes of excessive daytime sleepiness. Respir Care Clin N Am 5: 427–446.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. ↵
    Hagan JJ, Leslie RA, Patel S, Evans ML, Wattam TA, Holmes S, Benham CD, Taylor SG, Routledge C, Hemmati P, Munton RP, Ashmeade TE, Shah AS, Hatcher JP, Hatcher PD, Jones DN, Smith MI, Piper DC, Hunter AJ, Porter RA, Upton N (1999) Orexin A activates locus coeruleus cell firing and increases arousal in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 10911–10916.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Hara J, Beuckmann CT, Nambu T, Willie JT, Chemelli RM, Sinton CM, Sugiyama F, Yagami K, Goto K, Yanagisawa M, Sakurai T (2001) Genetic ablation of orexin neurons in mice results in narcolepsy, hypophagia, and obesity. Neuron 30: 345–354.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Horvath TL, Peyron C, Diano S, Ivanov A, Aston-Jones G, Kilduff TS, van Den Pol AN (1999) Hypocretin (orexin) activation and synaptic innervation of the locus coeruleus noradrenergic system. J Comp Neurol 415: 145–159.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Hungs M, Fan J, Lin L, Lin X, Maki RA, Mignot E (2001) Identification and functional analysis of mutations in the hypocretin (orexin) genes of narcoleptic canines. Genome Res 11: 531–539.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    Kisanuki YY, Chemelli RM, Sinton CM, Williams SC, Richardson JA, Hammer RE, Yanagisawa M (2000) The role of orexin receptor type-1 (OX1R) in the regulation of sleep. Sleep 23: A91.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    Kisanuki YY, Chemelli RM, Tokita S, Willie JT, Sinton CM, Yanagisawa M (2001) Behavioral and polysomnographic characterization of orexin-1 receptor and orexin-2 receptor double knockout mice. Sleep 24: A22.
    OpenUrl
  24. ↵
    Kiyashchenko LI, Mileykovskiy BY, Maidment N, Lam HA, Wu MF, John J, Peever J, Siegel JM (2002) Release of hypocretin (orexin) during waking and sleep states. J Neurosci 22: 5282–5286.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Li Y, Gao XB, Sakurai T, van den Pol AN (2002) Hypocretin/Orexin excites hypocretin neurons via a local glutamate neuron–a potential mechanism for orchestrating the hypothalamic arousal system. Neuron 36: 1169–1181.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Lin L, Faraco J, Li R, Kadotani H, Rogers W, Lin X, Qiu X, de Jong PJ, Nishino S, Mignot E (1999) The sleep disorder canine narcolepsy is caused by a mutation in the hypocretin (orexin) receptor 2 gene. Cell 98: 365–376.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Lin L, Hungs M, Mignot E (2001) Narcolepsy and the HLA region. J Neuroimmunol 117: 9–20.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    McCarley RW, Massaquoi SG (1992) Neurobiological structure of the revised limit cycle reciprocal interaction model of REM cycle control. J Sleep Res 1: 132–137.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  29. ↵
    Mignot E (1998) Genetic and familial aspects of narcolepsy. Neurology 50[Suppl 1]: S16–S22.
  30. ↵
    Mignot E (2001) A commentary on the neurobiology of the hypocretin/orexin system. Neuropsychopharmacology 25[Suppl]: S5–S13.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    Mignot E, Lammers GJ, Ripley B, Okun M, Nevsimalova S, Overeem S, Vankova J, Black J, Harsh J, Bassetti C, Schrader H, Nishino S (2002) The role of cerebrospinal fluid hypocretin measurement in the diagnosis of narcolepsy and other hypersomnias. Arch Neurol 59: 1553–1562.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Mitler MM, Soave O, Dement WC (1976) Narcolepsy in seven dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc 168: 1036–1038.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  33. ↵
    Nambu T, Sakurai T, Mizukami K, Hosoya Y, Yanagisawa M, Goto K (1999) Distribution of orexin neurons in the adult rat brain. Brain Res 827: 243–260.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Nishino S, Mignot E (1997) Pharmacological aspects of human and canine narcolepsy. Prog Neurobiol 52: 27–78.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Nishino S, Ripley B, Overeem S, Lammers GJ, Mignot E (2000) Hypocretin (orexin) deficiency in human narcolepsy. Lancet 355: 39–40.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Nishino S, Ripley B, Overeem S, Nevsimalova S, Lammers GJ, Vankova J, Okun M, Rogers W, Brooks S, Mignot E (2001) Low cerebrospinal fluid hypocretin (orexin) and altered energy homeostasis in human narcolepsy. Ann Neurol 50: 381–388.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    Overeem S, Mignot E, van Dijk JG, Lammers GJ (2001) Narcolepsy: clinical features, new pathophysiologic insights, and future perspectives. J Clin Neurophysiol 18: 78–105.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Peyron C, Tighe DK, van den Pol AN, de Lecea L, Heller HC, Sutcliffe JG, Kilduff TS (1998) Neurons containing hypocretin (orexin) project to multiple neuronal systems. J Neurosci 18: 9996–10015.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    Peyron C, Faraco J, Rogers W, Ripley B, Overeem S, Charnay Y, Nevsimalova S, Aldrich M, Reynolds D, Albin R, Li R, Hungs M, Pedrazzoli M, Padigaru M, Kucherlapati M, Fan J, Maki R, Lammers GJ, Bouras C, Kucherlapati R, Nishino S, Mignot E (2000) A mutation in a case of early onset narcolepsy and a generalized absence of hypocretin peptides in human narcoleptic brains. Nat Med 6: 991–997.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Radulovacki M, Virus RM, Djuricic-Nedelson M, Green RD (1984) Adenosine analogs and sleep in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 228: 268–274.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Riehl J, Nishino S, Cederberg R, Dement WC, Mignot E (1998) Development of cataplexy in genetically narcoleptic Dobermans. Exp Neurol 152: 292–302.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Ripley B, Overeem S, Fujiki N, Nevsimalova S, Uchino M, Yesavage J, Di Monte D, Dohi K, Melberg A, Lammers GJ, Nishida Y, Roelandse FW, Hungs M, Mignot E, Nishino S (2001) CSF hypocretin/orexin levels in narcolepsy and other neurological conditions. Neurology 57: 2253–2258.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, Matsuzaki I, Chemelli RM, Tanaka H, Williams SC, Richardson JA, Kozlowski GP, Wilson S, Arch JR, Buckingham RE, Haynes AC, Carr SA, Annan RS, McNulty DE, Liu WS, Terrett JA, Elshourbagy NA, Bergsma DJ, Yanagisawa M (1998) Orexins and orexin receptors: a family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell 92: 573–585.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    Saper CB, Chou TC, Scammell TE (2001) The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness. Trends Neurosci 24: 726–731.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Scrima L (1981) An etiology of narcolepsy-cataplexy and a proposed cataplexy neuromechanism. Int J Neurosci 15: 69–86.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Sutcliffe JG, de Lecea L (2002) The hypocretins: setting the arousal threshold. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 339–349.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    Taheri S, Sunter D, Dakin C, Moyes S, Seal L, Gardiner J, Rossi M, Ghatei M, Bloom S (2000) Diurnal variation in orexin A immunoreactivity and prepro-orexin mRNA in the rat central nervous system. Neurosci Lett 279: 109–112.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Taheri S, Zeitzer JM, Mignot E (2002) The role of hypocretins (orexins) in sleep regulation and narcolepsy. Annu Rev Neurosci 25: 283–313.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Thannickal TC, Moore RY, Nienhuis R, Ramanathan L, Gulyani S, Aldrich M, Cornford M, Siegel JM (2000) Reduced number of hypocretin neurons in human narcolepsy. Neuron 27: 469–474.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Willie JT, Chemelli RM, Sinton CM, Tokita S, Williams SC, Kisanuki YY, Marcus JN, Lee C, Elmquist JK, Kohlmeier KA, Leonard CS, Richardson JA, Hammer RE, Yanagisawa M (2003) Distinct narcolepsy syndromes in orexin receptor-2 and orexin null mice: molecular genetic dissection of non-REM and REM sleep regulatory processes. Neuron 38: 715–730.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Yoshida Y, Fujiki N, Nakajima T, Ripley B, Matsumura H, Yoneda H, Mignot E, Nishino S (2001) Fluctuation of extracellular hypocretin-1 (orexin A) levels in the rat in relation to the light-dark cycle and sleep-wake activities. Eur J Neurosci 14: 1075–1081.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Zeitzer JM, Buckmaster CL, Parker KJ, Hauck CM, Lyons DM, Mignot E (2003) Circadian and homeostatic regulation of hypocretin in a primate model: implications for the consolidation of wakefulness. J Neurosci 23: 3555–3560.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 24 (18)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 24, Issue 18
5 May 2004
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Expression of a Poly-Glutamine-Ataxin-3 Transgene in Orexin Neurons Induces Narcolepsy–Cataplexy in the Rat
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Expression of a Poly-Glutamine-Ataxin-3 Transgene in Orexin Neurons Induces Narcolepsy–Cataplexy in the Rat
Carsten T. Beuckmann, Christopher M. Sinton, S. Clay Williams, James A. Richardson, Robert E. Hammer, Takeshi Sakurai, Masashi Yanagisawa
Journal of Neuroscience 5 May 2004, 24 (18) 4469-4477; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5560-03.2004

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Expression of a Poly-Glutamine-Ataxin-3 Transgene in Orexin Neurons Induces Narcolepsy–Cataplexy in the Rat
Carsten T. Beuckmann, Christopher M. Sinton, S. Clay Williams, James A. Richardson, Robert E. Hammer, Takeshi Sakurai, Masashi Yanagisawa
Journal of Neuroscience 5 May 2004, 24 (18) 4469-4477; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5560-03.2004
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Epilepsy-Related CDKL5 Deficiency Slows Synaptic Vesicle Endocytosis in Central Nerve Terminals
  • Involvement of GABAergic Interneuron Subtypes in 4-Aminopyridine-Induced Seizure-Like Events in Mouse Entorhinal Cortex in Vitro
  • Spared Premotor Areas Undergo Rapid Nonlinear Changes in Functional Organization Following a Focal Ischemic Infarct in Primary Motor Cortex of Squirrel Monkeys
Show more Neurobiology of Disease
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.